[go: up one dir, main page]

Academia.eduAcademia.edu

Child-caregiver interaction in two remote Indigenous Australian communities

2015, Frontiers in Psychology

This paper reports on a study in two remote multilingual Indigenous Australian communities: Yakanarra in the Kimberley region of Western Australia and Tennant Creek in the Barkly region of the Northern Territory. In both communities, processes of language shift are underway from a traditional language (Walmajarri and Warumungu, respectively) to a local creole variety (Fitzroy Valley Kriol and Wumpurrarni English, respectively). The study focuses on language input from primary caregivers to a group of preschool children, and on the children's productive language. The study further highlights child-caregiver interactions as a site of importance in understanding the broader processes of language shift. We use longitudinal data from two time-points, approximately 2 years apart, to explore changes in adult input over time and developmental patterns in the children's speech. At both time points, the local creole varieties are the preferred codes of communication for the dyads in this study, although there is some use of the traditional language in both communities. Results show that for measures of turn length (MLT), there are notable differences between the two communities for both the focus children and their caregivers. In Tennant Creek, children and caregivers use longer turns at Time 2, while in Yakanarra the picture is more variable. The two communities also show differing trends in terms of conversational load (MLT ratio). For measures of morphosyntactic complexity (MLU), children and caregivers in Tennant Creek use more complex utterances at Time 2, while caregivers in Yakanarra show less complexity in their language at that time point. The study's findings contribute to providing a more detailed picture of the multilingual practices at Yakanarra and Tennant Creek, with implications for understanding broader processes of language shift. They also elucidate how children's language and linguistic input varies diachronically across time. As such, we contribute to understandings of normative language development for non-Western, non middle-class children in multilingual contexts.

ORIGINAL RESEARCH published: 29 April 2015 doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00514 Child-caregiver interaction in two remote Indigenous Australian communities Jill Vaughan 1 , Gillian Wigglesworth 1*, Deborah Loakes 1 , Samantha Disbray 2 and Karin Moses 3 1 School of Languages and Linguistics, University of Melbourne, Parkville, VIC, Australia, 2 Northern Institute, Charles Darwin University, Alice Springs, NT, Australia, 3 Student Learning, La Trobe University, Bendigo, VIC, Australia Edited by: Carmel O’Shannessy, University of Michigan, USA Reviewed by: Jennifer Smith, University of Glasgow, UK Ian Grenville Malcolm, Edith Cowan University, Australia (retired) *Correspondence: Gillian Wigglesworth, School of Languages and Linguistics, University of Melbourne, Darling Street, Parkville, VIC 3010, Australia gillianw@unimelb.edu.au Specialty section: This article was submitted to Language Sciences, a section of the journal Frontiers in Psychology Received: 07 August 2014 Accepted: 11 April 2015 Published: 29 April 2015 Citation: Vaughan J, Wigglesworth G, Loakes D, Disbray S and Moses K (2015) Child-caregiver interaction in two remote Indigenous Australian communities. Front. Psychol. 6:514. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00514 Frontiers in Psychology | www.frontiersin.org This paper reports on a study in two remote multilingual Indigenous Australian communities: Yakanarra in the Kimberley region of Western Australia and Tennant Creek in the Barkly region of the Northern Territory. In both communities, processes of language shift are underway from a traditional language (Walmajarri and Warumungu, respectively) to a local creole variety (Fitzroy Valley Kriol and Wumpurrarni English, respectively). The study focuses on language input from primary caregivers to a group of preschool children, and on the children’s productive language. The study further highlights child-caregiver interactions as a site of importance in understanding the broader processes of language shift. We use longitudinal data from two time-points, approximately 2 years apart, to explore changes in adult input over time and developmental patterns in the children’s speech. At both time points, the local creole varieties are the preferred codes of communication for the dyads in this study, although there is some use of the traditional language in both communities. Results show that for measures of turn length (MLT), there are notable differences between the two communities for both the focus children and their caregivers. In Tennant Creek, children and caregivers use longer turns at Time 2, while in Yakanarra the picture is more variable. The two communities also show differing trends in terms of conversational load (MLT ratio). For measures of morphosyntactic complexity (MLU), children and caregivers in Tennant Creek use more complex utterances at Time 2, while caregivers in Yakanarra show less complexity in their language at that time point. The study’s findings contribute to providing a more detailed picture of the multilingual practices at Yakanarra and Tennant Creek, with implications for understanding broader processes of language shift. They also elucidate how children’s language and linguistic input varies diachronically across time. As such, we contribute to understandings of normative language development for non-Western, non middle-class children in multilingual contexts. Keywords: child language acquisition, language input, language shift, Walmajarri, Fitzroy Valley Kriol, Warumungu, Wumpurrarni English Introduction Indigenous children in Australia grow up in a range of diverse language settings. In the cities and metropolitan areas, particularly along the populated coastlines of Australia, which were settled early 1 April 2015 | Volume 6 | Article 514 Vaughan et al. Child-caregiver interaction in Indigenous Australia Aboriginal Child Language Acquisition (ACLA1) study3 , carried out from 2004 to 2007, which focused on child-directed speech, as well as language use by children, in three Indigenous Australian communities (for more information on ACLA1 see Wigglesworth and Simpson, 2008)—Yakanarra, Tennant Creek and Kalkaringi4 . This is particularly interesting because of the relative dearth of information on how non-Western children acquire language, and also because these particular children live in multilingual societies which are undergoing rapid language shift. We address prior research in this area further below. by Europeans, traditional languages have long ceased to be spoken as the primary means of day-to-day communication. Instead, local varieties of Aboriginal English are spoken as in-community and in-family codes. “Aboriginal English” is an umbrella term, covering a range of varieties in different sociolinguistic and geographic settings (see Malcolm and Kaldor, 1991). Aboriginal English is largely mutually intelligible with standard Australian English, and in its “lightest” styles differs only in minor ways from the standard, usually in pronunciation, lexis, grammatical patterns, speech pragmatics and conceptualization (see e.g., Kaldor and Malcolm, 1982; Malcolm and Kaldor, 1991; Eades, 1996; Malcolm and Sharifian, 2002, 2007; McGregor, 2004; Sharifian, 2005; Butcher, 2008). In more remote locations, particularly in central and northern Australia, the picture is more complex. In some places, traditional languages continue to be acquired as first languages by the current generation of children, and in other places, language shift to contact varieties, including mixed languages and local creole varieties, has taken place, or is in process (see e.g., McConvell and Thieberger, 2001). Creole varieties on mainland Australia are referred to as Kriol, the name being based on the word creole1 . There are a number of locally named varieties of Kriol, for example in Yakanarra (one of our field sites), the variety is called Fitzroy Valley Kriol (or Kimberley Kriol) (see e.g., Hudson, 1985), henceforth FVKriol. Another example is Roper River Kriol spoken in the Ngukurr region. In Tennant Creek (our second field site) the creole is referred to as Wumpurrarni English, henceforth WE, and varies from acrolectal (more like English) to basilectal (a heavy creole) (see e.g., Disbray and Simpson, 2004; Disbray, 2009)2 . Recent research into such language settings in Australia is providing descriptions of children’s language acquisition in diverse and dynamic contexts (see, for example, the variety of papers in Simpson and Wigglesworth, 2008). Other research focuses on the rapidly changing language environments and the emergence of new contact varieties (e.g., Bavin and Shopen, 1985 for Yuendumu Warlpiri; McConvell and Meakins, 2005; Meakins, 2011 for Gurindji Kriol; O’Shannessy, 2005 for Light Warlpiri and McConvell, 2008 for general discussion of mixed languages in Indigenous Australia). This study is an investigation of language use amongst Indigenous caregivers and their young children in two remote communities in Australia. There are two themes to the paper. The first regards child language development, and the second compares how this manifests in two communities of Indigenous people that are in many ways comparable (remote, multiple codes spoken, community language endangerment, and shift) but are markedly different in terms of the sociolinguistic setting and the mixture of languages that children are exposed to. Our own work has looked in some detail at the language situation in Indigenous Australia. The work is associated with The Studies of Child-Directed Speech We have looked, for example, at the way Yakanarra children deal with questions in an informal setting (Moses and Yallop, 2008), at the language codes children receive from different aged interlocutors (Loakes et al., 2013), and children’s knowledge of their traditional language (Loakes et al., 2012). From this, we know that Indigenous children in Yakanarra frequently experience questions and respond to questioning, debunking earlier views that Indigenous Australians are not familiar with question-answer routines (Moses and Yallop, 2008). We know that children speak and hear FVKriol predominantly, but are also exposed to the traditional language Walmajarri, especially from older community members. As far as Walmajarri is concerned, we also know that children have relatively good receptive knowledge when it comes to nouns (Loakes et al., 2012), and we also know that their traditional language knowledge goes beyond this, because they may respond to Walmajarri utterances from an elder entirely (and appropriately) in FVKriol (examples shown in Loakes et al., 2013). In Tennant Creek we have investigated interactions between caregivers and children during joint picture-book viewing (Disbray, 2008). In these interactions we saw that caregivers of the children aged between 18 months and 5 years of age were sensitive to the child’s level of attention at different ages, and that through questioning and prompting, and through repetition and elaboration, they collaboratively “built” a rich narrative. This study also established common patterns of use of Warumungu features in WE, further explored as a factor of the age of the adult speaker in Morrison and Disbray (2008). These studies have shown that children hear full Warumungu only from elderly speakers, and that in the WE speech of younger adults, some common Warumungu nouns and fewer Warumungu verbs are used, a bilingual speech pattern which children also later develop (Disbray, 2009). Additionally, semantic case-markers, derived from Warumungu, have been incorporated into WE, and are used variably (Disbray and Simpson, 2004; Morrison and Disbray, 2008). We know that, in general, children have acquired most of the skills needed for adult-like language proficiency by the time they attend primary school (Hoff, 2001), and spend their school years mastering more complex grammatical and phonological features. However, studies of the development of grammaticality in Indigenous Australian children have only recently gained 1 In the Torres Strait Islands off Northern Queensland, the creole variety spoken is referred to as “Torres Strait Creole.” The spelling Kriol stems from the orthography developed for Roper River Kriol (Sandefur, 1979). 2 See Harris (1993) for a discussion of the development of Kriol in Australia, and more information about the varieties spoken. Frontiers in Psychology | www.frontiersin.org 3 4 2 http://languages-linguistics.unimelb.edu.au/past-projects/acla1 See Moses (2009), Disbray (2009), and Meakins (2011) respectively. April 2015 | Volume 6 | Article 514 Vaughan et al. Child-caregiver interaction in Indigenous Australia the production of multiclause utterances by 4-year olds in relation to parental input at home, and teacher input at pre-school, and found that the style of parental input was the major predictor of syntactic complexity in children’s speech. In addition, various studies have shown the complexity, quantity, and lexical diversity of caregiver speech to have strong correlations with children’s vocabulary development (Huttenlocher et al., 1991; Hart and Risley, 1995; Bornstein et al., 1998; Naigles and Hoff-Ginsberg, 1998; Hoff and Naigles, 2002; Hoff, 2003; Pan et al., 2005; Rowe, 2008). Recent studies of the acquisition of sociolinguistic variation (e.g., Foulkes et al., 1999, 2005; Díaz-Campos, 2005; Smith et al., 2007, 2013; O’Shannessy, 2011) emphasize that childdirected speech plays a central role in socializing children into the appropriate use and the social-indexical values of variables (Foulkes et al., 2005, p. 177). For example, in their study of 29 child/caregiver pairs in a small Scottish fishing village, Smith et al. showed that patterns of variation are acquired at a very early age in tandem with language acquisition more generally, but that the nature of this process may vary according to the variable in question (2013, p. 321). Foulkes et al. (2005), in their study of childdirected speech among 39 mothers in North East England, found that phonetic variants used in this register differed markedly from inter-adult speech, and further noted effects with regard to the gender of the children in the dyads: speech to girls contained more standard variants than speech to boys, which contained higher rates of vernacular variants. This effect was more apparent for younger children in the sample. momentum. Malcolm (1996) reports on development of the verb phrase in Aboriginal English spoken in a remote Indigenous region, and concludes that there is evidence that language development may be ongoing for bilingual/bidialectal Indigenous children even between 5 and 10 years of age. Disbray (2009) investigated the development of discourse competence among 5–14 year old WE-speaking children, demonstrating that the children pass through similar developmental stages to children speaking other languages to create a cohesive text. A great deal of further research is needed to understand language development amongst Indigenous Australian children. Previous studies of child-directed speech have noted that parental speech to children can involve utterances that are shorter, morphosyntactically simpler, and more redundant than speech directed at adults (e.g., Snow, 1972, 1977; Sachs et al., 1976; Ferguson, 1977). These findings contributed to the idea that simplified language input may help children’s language development (e.g., Snow, 1972), often referred to as the “motherese hypothesis.” In the 2–5 year old age range, as in the present study, language development is characterized by a move from simple two- and three-word combinations to the production of more grammatically sophisticated utterances, as children become skilled at using most of the requisite grammatical morphemes and begin to construct complex multiclause sentences (Hoff, 2001; Berko Gleason, 2005). Snow (1972) observed that maternal speech to 2-year olds is simpler and more redundant than speech directed at 10year-olds, and other studies focusing on younger children in the fundamental stages of language acquisition have demonstrated that caregivers make fine adjustments to the complexity of their speech according to the age of the child (e.g., Vosoughi, 2010). Kaye (1980), Philips (1973) and Rondal (1980) all observed markedly shorter, simpler, and more repetitive utterances to infants than to toddler-aged children. A more recent study by Huttenlocher et al. (2007) with children aged 14–30 months reinforces these early findings; their analysis of the speech used by English-speaking parents with their children at 4-month intervals revealed that caregivers made substantial adjustments to the syntactic complexity and diversity of their speech over the 16-month time period. In the sample of 50 parents, Huttenlocher et al. also found individual differences in input patterns related to caregiver education level, and noted that caregivers maintained their idiosyncratic input patterns at the same time as making incremental adjustments to their speech depending on the age of their child interlocutor. The researchers suggest that this consistent adjustment indicates caregiver sensitivity to the language levels of young children, while providing the children with a progressively broader set of language models. Other studies incorporating child production data have demonstrated that longer and more complex utterances from caregivers correlate with more advanced morphosyntactic development in children. Hoff-Ginsberg (1998), in a study of maternal speech to children based on birth order, found that first-born children showed “more precocious lexical and grammatical development than later borns” (Hoff-Ginsberg, 1998, p. 626), and that this correlated with longer and more complex utterances from their mothers. Huttenlocher et al. (2002) looked at Frontiers in Psychology | www.frontiersin.org Studies of Cross-Cultural Language Input Cross-culturally, children have different early language experiences, and the existing research on non-Western cultural groups indicates that there is a great deal of variation in the type and amount of direct language input children receive in the preschool years (Hoff, 2006). Various studies have observed that in some non-Western cultures, young children are not seen as communicative partners, and receive very little child-directed speech as infants, in contrast to the patterns observed for North American and European caregivers. This been noted for Gusii mothers in Kenya (Richman et al., 1992; LeVine, 2004), Gapuners in Papua New Guinea (Kulick, 1992), the Kaluli in Papua New Guinea (Ochs and Schieffelin, 1994), Samoans in Western Samoa (Ochs and Schieffelin, 1994), the Warlpiri of central Australia (Bavin, 1992), K’iche’ Maya speakers in Guatemala (Pye, 1992), Tzeltal speakers in Mexico (Brown, 2001), Javanese speakers in East Java (Smith-Hefner, 1998), and African-Americans in South Carolina (Heath, 1983). In many of these studies (e.g., Ochs and Schieffelin, 1994), the researchers suggest that a large amount of early language learning must be based on overheard, rather than childdirected, language, and that minimal direct speech to children may correlate with relatively late language development (Brown, 2001). However, there is very little research describing language input in these non-Western societies on occasions when input is provided by caregivers, and even less looking at child language development in relation to input, beyond the body of research on the acquisition of language-specific grammatical and phonological 3 April 2015 | Volume 6 | Article 514 Vaughan et al. Child-caregiver interaction in Indigenous Australia found that less traditional language is found in children’s interactions with younger interlocutors. These young interlocutors also use markedly more talk than older interactants. While there is some research into normative language development for non middle-class, non-Western children, this area remains poorly understood, particularly in situations where the children grow up in multilingual communities. Research suggests that bi- or multilingual children progress through linguistic developmental stages at similar rates to monolingual children (Pearson et al., 1993; Paradis and Genesee, 1996; De Houwer, 2005), and in particular that when all the child’s linguistic resources are considered (i.e., their knowledge of both/all languages), multilingual children have similar vocabulary sizes and grammatical abilities to monolingual children (Pearson and Fernandez, 1994; Nicoladis and Genesee, 1997; Patterson, 1998; Patterson and Pearson, 2004). However, in assessing children’s abilities in each separate language, some research suggests that at comparable ages, bi- or multilingual children may have less developed vocabularies and grammatical abilities in each individual language than monolingual children in their sole language (Bialystok and Feng, 2011; Hoff et al., 2012). Other research indicates that language development progresses language-specifically (Marchman et al., 2004; Conboy and Thal, 2006). Language development for multilingual children is also clearly tied in with the issue of language dominance, which in turn is closely related to the type and quantity of language input children receive from carers (Lanza, 1997; De Houwer, 1999, 2007; Hoff et al., 2012). As for other areas of acquisition research, knowledge about language socialization in multilingual families tends to be based on research in Western cultures. Some research has investigated language socialization in language contact settings (Garrett and Baquedano-López, 2002; Luykx, 2005; Makihara, 2005; Gafaranga, 2010; O’Shannessy, 2011) including creole contexts (Garrett, 2005; Paugh, 2005), and it is clear that these non-Western contexts encompass an extremely diverse range of language learning environments, with many unique and contextspecific factors that need to be considered in investigations of child language development. We cannot assume that current understandings of child language input and language development hold true for children in linguistically diverse non-Western settings, and there is still a large amount of research to be done on the nature of the language received and produced by children in these settings, as well as the language development patterns that are normal for them. As Saxton (2009, 2010) points out, many non-Western language socialization studies are anthropological in nature, rather than designed specifically to investigate child language input and acquisition, particularly those cited as evidence of minimal or non-modified child directed speech. There is evidently a need for targeted exploration of caregiver input behavior and child language production cross-culturally and cross-linguistically, and the present study contributes to this by providing input and production data for two Indigenous Australian communities: Yakanarra in the Kimberley area of Western Australia, and Tennant Creek in the Barkly region of the Northern Territory. The study focuses on language input by primary caregivers to a group of preschool children, and also focuses features. In Indigenous Australia, a number of researchers have observed the use of “baby talk” registers (Laughren, 1984; Lee, 1987; Bavin, 1992, 1993; Kral and Ellis, 2008; Jones and Meakins, 2013; Turpin et al., 2014). This register is typically characterized by such features as phonological simplification, semantic simplification (for some groups of vocabulary items), repetition, slower speech rate and falling prosodic contour. Outside the Australian context, Harkness’ (1977) crosssectional study found that Kipsigis caregivers in Kenya modified the length and complexity of their utterances as a function of their child interlocutor’s MLU, adjusting their speech to the developmental stage of the child, leading Harkness to observe that “mothers and children in cultures far removed from our own modify their speech to children learning to talk in the same way that Americans do” (1977: 315). In contrast, there are claims that when caregivers in some other cultures talk to children, they do not use “baby talk”; for example, Ochs and Schieffelin (1994) observe that Kaluli and Samoan caregivers, though they may use pitch manipulation, do not engage in the morphosyntactic simplification synonymous with child-directed speech. Despite this, “Kaluli and Samoan children become fluent speakers within the range of normal developmental variation” (Ochs and Schieffelin, 1994, p. 494). Crago et al. (1997) have similarly found that of the minimal language input received by children in Inuktitutspeaking communities in Quebec, very little is morphosyntactically modified, but the children still achieve the major language development milestones at ages comparable to those for Western children. In reviewing the literature on child-directed speech, Lieven (1994) argues that the child-centered style of speaking may be one way of enabling children to learn and use their mother tongue, “but it is clearly not essential” (1994: 72), and concludes that children worldwide tend to learn language at around the same time, despite the diverse ways of speaking to (and around) children. A further important aspect of the linguistic socialization of Indigenous children in Australia pertains to the high level of input children receive from interactants other than adults. In many Indigenous communities, children spend a great deal of their time playing and interacting with other children, with older children often taking on caregiving roles. Hamilton’s (1981) observations in Arnhem Land describe how from the age of two, children are absorbed into a peer group of related children for support and learning experiences. These groups may develop into more structured “kid mobs” common for children from 5 to 9 years of age, resulting in less frequent interaction with adults. After-school and weekend activities may take place in kid mob groupings, which are largely determined by common linguistic and kinship ties. The linguistic consequences may be significant, with the “kid mob” a potentially dominant force in community language shift whereby younger speakers may even socialize adults in language choice (see Luykx, 2005 and Gafaranga, 2010). O’Shannessy (2012, 2013) has shown that children can play a significant role in language change, partly due to their spending time in peer groups, and partly due to the kind of input they receive. Loakes et al. (2013) addressed this aspect of community language shift in Yakanarra by investigating the input received from different-aged interlocutors with regard to code choice and Frontiers in Psychology | www.frontiersin.org 4 April 2015 | Volume 6 | Article 514 Vaughan et al. Child-caregiver interaction in Indigenous Australia Aims on the children’s productive language. We use longitudinal data from two time-points, approximately 2 years apart, to describe changes in adult input over time and developmental patterns in the children’s speech. As discussed earlier, our previous work in both Yakanarra and Tennant Creek has shown that language shift is occurring rapidly, and children hear a variety of different input codes, with FVKriol in Yakanarra and Wumpurrani English in Tennant Creek being the main codes used. The input they hear varies along a continuum from more acrolectal to more basilectal determined by various factors including the person, the setting and the interlocutor. This study therefore examines both features of input to the children, and features of the production of the children at two points in time 2 years apart, when the children were approximately two and four. The specific research questions we address are: Profiles of the Communities Both communities in this study are located in remote desert regions of Australia. Yakanarra is in the far north of the state of Western Australia, and Tennant Creek is centrally located in the Northern Territory. Yakanarra Yakanarra consists of about 30 houses 110 km south-east of Fitzroy Crossing in the Kimberley region. Yakanarra is typical of a remote rural Indigenous community in Australia, with the majority of residents being Indigenous, and the few nonIndigenous residents working in the school, community center, or shop. Yakanarra was established in 1989 by Walmajarri people, the oldest of whom had left their traditional country and hunter-gatherer way of life in the 1950s. The traditional language of Yakanarra is Walmajarri, but by 2006, when Moses was carrying out fieldwork, FVKriol had displaced Walmajarri as the language of everyday talk, although Walmajarri was still spoken to some degree by the older members of the community (see especially Loakes et al., 2013). Some Standard Australian English is spoken in limited circumstances, typically in formal contexts, and with non-Indigenous people (see e.g., Wigglesworth and Simpson, 2008, p. 20). (1) What are the characteristics of the children’s language use at ages two and four in terms of MLT and MLU? (2) What are the characteristics of the caregivers’ language use at the two time points in terms of MLT and MLU? (3) In these two communities, what kinds of multilingual practices can be observed in the children’s and the caregivers’ language use? While analysis of Time 1 compared to Time 2 is sometimes considered a gross measure of development (c.f. Snow, 1995), we argue that it is a crucial first step to understanding longitudinal changes in child language in these communities. Given the lack of understanding about normative language development in Indigenous Australian communities generally, and particularly in areas undergoing rapid language shift, we hope that results of the current study will be used as a reference sample for other researchers working in this area. In general, ongoing work in Indigenous Australian communities is largely a response to the fact that language acquisition in monolingual (especially English-speaking) communities is wellunderstood, yet language acquisition in multilingual Indigenous societies such as those in Australia is understudied (see for example the discussion in Wigglesworth and Simpson, 2008, p. 14). Additionally, it is a response to the need for basic, languagespecific work on developmental patterns before a full theory of child language acquisition can be attained (see e.g., Slobin, 1997). Tennant Creek Tennant Creek is a remote, urban township in the Barkly region of Central Australia. It is a small town with a population of approximately 3000, half of whom are Indigenous. The township is located on Warumungu country, the traditional language of the area. The local Kriol variety, Wumpurrarni English (see Disbray, 2009), is the main language of everyday communication for most Warumungu people. This local variety shares features with other Kriol varieties spoken in Indigenous Australia, but has a number of local features, including the use of Warumungusource features, such as insertional code-switches and semantic case-marking (Morrison and Disbray, 2008). Like many contact settings, there is substantial variation in the way people in Tennant Creek use the contact variety Wumpurrarni English, with the speech varieties best understood as occupying a continuum from “lighter” or more acrolectal to “heavier” or more basilectal styles. Similarly to the situation in Yakanarra, Warumungu has undergone significant language shift, and is spoken as a full code only by a small number of elders, with younger adult speakers tending to be partial speakers (Morrison and Disbray, 2008). Method Participants For both communities there are four child participants in the study, as well as their main caregiver, who were their mothers in all cases but one—BM is Belinda’s great-grandmother5 . This gives four interactional pairs and eight participants in each community, with sixteen participants in total. Information about the participants is shown in Table 1 below. This includes pseudonyms which refer to the children, their sex, the code for their caregiver, the children’s ages at Time 1 and 2, and the age difference across the time-points. The caregiver code corresponds to the first initial of the child and the initial “M” for mother, so for example the mother of the first child in the table, Natalie, is referred to as NM. Comparing Yakanarra and Tennant Creek Yakanarra and Tennant Creek are both located in desert regions of Australia. Yakanarra is a remote and relatively closed community with little outside influence, while Tennant Creek is a larger town on a major highway, with a greater variety of people from varied language backgrounds. Frontiers in Psychology | www.frontiersin.org 5 5 It is not unusual to have a great grandmother in an Indigenous community. April 2015 | Volume 6 | Article 514 Vaughan et al. Child-caregiver interaction in Indigenous Australia TABLE 1 | Participant details. Community Child Sex Caregiver Child age time 1 Tennant creek Natalie F NM 2;7 4;7 2;0 Tennant creek Melanie F MM 2;7 4;3 1;8 Tennant creek Sarah F SM 1;8 3;7 1;11 Tennant creek Belinda F BM 2;2 4;2 2;0 Yakanarra Katherine F KM 3;6 5;6 2;0 Yakanarra Olivia F OM 2;7 4;7 2;0 Yakanarra Andrew M AM 2;2 4;7 2;5 Yakanarra Emily F EM 2;6 4;6 2;0 Procedure The corpus for this study comprises sixteen transcripts transcribed into CLAN6 . Transcripts were based on video and audiorecorded interactions between the eight focus children and their primary caregivers, at the two different time periods. All transcripts are of equal length, with 100 lines being chosen for commensurability. Participants were primarily engaged in two 7 The first phase of this project is described in greater detail in Wigglesworth and Simpson(2008, see esp. pp. 19–27). The second phase of the project, currently underway, is investigating issues faced by Indigenous children as they enter the formal school system where they encounter Standard Australian English, of which they often have little knowledge, as well as significant cultural differences. http://childes.psy.cmu.edu/ Frontiers in Psychology | www.frontiersin.org Time 2-time 1 types of tasks: prompted play (for example using toy cash registers, a doctor’s kit or building with wooden blocks) and picture book-prompted story telling (Egan, 1986; O’Shannessy, 2004). The data are a subset of materials collected in the first phase of a longitudinal study, The Australian Child Language Acquisition project (ACLA)7 , now in its second phase. The first phase of the project focused on caregiver input to young Indigenous children, who were between the ages of 18 months and two and a half years at the beginning of the project. Language data were collected from children and caregivers every 6-months over a 4year period. While data were collected in three remote Australian communities, this paper focuses only on Yakanarra and Tennant Creek. In the transcripts, each morpheme was coded as either the local contact variety, or as the traditional language. It was not always straightforward to determine which words have been incorporated as loan words (and so should be coded as Kriol), and which are simply the product of code-switching (and so should be coded as traditional language). This is in part because no definitive description of the Kriol lexicon exists in either location. However, we were able to consistently apply our analysis across the data, with work on the Kriol lexicon in Yakanarra by Hudson (1985), and with Sandefur (1979) and Lee (2004) guiding our transcription, glossing and, in some areas, description of both the Tennant Creek and Yakanarra data. Words were coded as traditional language when the phonological and semantic structure matched that of the traditional language. All other cases were coded as the contact variety. In the extracts given throughout this paper, tokens identified as Walmajarri or Warumungu are underlined. Standard English, while certainly used in some instances in these communities, was not found in the child-caregiver interactions that we recorded. For this study, we have collapsed acrolectal and basilectal Kriol forms into one category, despite the fact that speakers vary considerably along a continuum with respect to this. Separating out these varieties is, in practice, next to impossible as many elements are shared across the continuum and variation occurs in relation All participants were female with the exception of Andrew in Yakanarra. At Time 1, they ranged in age from 1;8 to 3;6, and at time 2 from 3;7 to 5;6. The age range between the two time periods varied from 1;8 to 2;5, (average 2;0) with five children having exactly 2 years between the two times. The average age of the children in Tennant Creek at time 1 was 2;3 and 4;2 at Time 2; in Yakanarra it was 2;8 at time 1 and 4;10 at time 2. The caregivers range from being in their early twenties to early thirties, while BM is in her early seventies. The fact that age is not balanced across the sessions is a limitation, but a reflection of the reality of data collection in Indigenous communities. In Indigenous Australia, people tend to be highly mobile. Communities are small and members are often traveling between different communities, visiting family and taking part in local events across a region. Following participants to record them in other locations is not feasible, given the remoteness of the communities and the distance that would need to be covered. As such, we have had to make compromises as far as exact comparability of child ages and difference between sessions is concerned. Furthermore, in Indigenous Australia, caregiving is shared by a range of older kin, including older children (i.e., the “kid mob” Hamilton, 1981; Andrews, 2008), grandparents and great grandparents and children therefore receive language input from a diverse group of speakers. To achieve maximal comparability, we have limited our study to four dyads in each location, and to child-caregiver interactions. This poses limitations to the breadth of representation of the range of participants and speech styles in such inherently dynamic speech settings. Despite this, we expect that results will be an important contribution to knowledge in the field of child language generally, with data from rarely studied locations which are undergoing rapid language shift, and where little is known about how children acquire language. 6 Child age time 2 6 April 2015 | Volume 6 | Article 514 Vaughan et al. Child-caregiver interaction in Indigenous Australia although there are small increases in its use between T1 and T2. This is commensurate with our findings in Yakanarra (i.e., Loakes et al., 2013), where we saw that the older people in the community were more likely to use the traditional language in their interactions with children than younger adults were, as a result of which children used more traditional language in response. Similarly children sometimes responded entirely in Kriol when spoken to by older people entirely in the traditional language (Loakes et al., 2013)—a case of “receptive multilingualism” (see e.g., ten Thije and Zeevaert, 2007; ten Thije et al., 2012), or “two-way” conversation (Elwell, 1982). Meakins (2008) made similar observations in Kalkaringi for Gurindji and Gurindji Kriol. A more detailed examination of the nature of the interaction between the codes may provide some insights about the ongoing pragmatic and interactional role of traditional languages. In Extract 1, where Melanie is looking at a picture book with her mother, the turn is predominantly in Wumpurrarni English, but with Warumungu (underlined) used occasionally. The Warumungu-usage which priviledges nouns (e.g., julaka “bird”) and case-markers (such as the possessive –kayi) is typical. to interlocutor and topic as well as age and idiolect. Since distinguishing Kriol varieties is not a central concern of this study, we have not attempted such a categorization. While the resultant procedure is broad, and potentially limiting in terms of overrepresenting the traditional language, this exact coding procedure worked well for the Yakanarra data analyzed by Loakes et al. (2013). In that study, we were able to demonstrate distinct differences across age-groups in terms of language use with the contact variety, FVKriol, used as the main language by all speakers when interacting with young children. However, the traditional language Walmajarri, while rarely used by younger people, was used about a third of the time by speakers over the age of 50. The following quantitative measures were used: • Mean length of turn (MLT) • MLT ratio • Mean length of utterance (MLU) This battery of measures illustrates the overall utterance length (MLT), conversational load (MLT ratio) and sentence complexity (MLU). Mean length of turn is the ratio of words to turn, where a turn is “a sequence of utterances spoken by a single speaker” (McWhinney, 2012, p. 92). MLT ratio is a related measure, calculated by dividing the focus child MLT over interlocutor MLT. Where the value is below 1.0 the interactant has a greater conversational burden, and greater than 1.0 means the child has a greater share (see McWhinney, 2012, p. 40). Mean length of utterance is a measure of morphosyntactic complexity and consists of the ratio of morphemes to utterances, where an utterance can range from a single token to a full clause. MLU has not been without its critics, but has been widely used to measure children’s language development despite criticisms relating to its lack of sensitivity to social context, and the unrepresentativeness of the early populations sampled by psycholinguists (e.g., Geneshi and Glupczynski, 2006). As one of a battery of measures though, it remains a valid technique. While we acknowledge that these measures are limited in their depth of analysis, the approach has been chosen to provide an initial comparison of language use to and by children in the communities. It is by no means exhaustive. But to date, there has been no quantitative, longitudinal study of children’s language development in these indigenous communities: this study is, thus, a first step in that direction. Extract 1: Melanie 4;3 and mother MM, T2. MM: dat lil julaka bin go na DET little bird PST go LOC na im-kayi mami 3S POSS mother now the little bird went to its mother. MM: yu luk deya 2S look there see there. MM: im- kayi mami i bin kraiin fo 3S POSS mother 3S PST cry for im 3S his mother was crying for him. MM: i bin ran wai dumuj i bin 3S PST run away because 3S PST git los get lost he ran away from because he got lost. Results Language Use Similarly, in Yakanarra, in Extract 2 Olivia and her mother are sharing a book and code-switching between FVKriol and Walmajarri (underlined). In both communities, the local Kriol variety was used overwhelmingly at T1 and T2 by both children and caregivers— well over 90% of morphemes. The exceptions were at T1 where Belinda used the Warumungu words karnanti (“mother”), kampaju (“father”), and kupunta (“burn”), repeatedly, and Melanie uttered only four morphemes, one of which was in Warumungu. Belinda’s great-grandmother, BM, is a fluent Warumungu speaker who uses Warumungu frequently, including in her interactions with children. Similarly in Yakanarra, children and their caregivers use the traditional language at very low rates overall, Frontiers in Psychology | www.frontiersin.org Extract 2: Olivia 2;7 and mother OM, T1. 7 OM: hu dijan? Who this one Who’s this one? Olivia: pirla ghost April 2015 | Volume 6 | Article 514 Vaughan et al. Child-caregiver interaction in Indigenous Australia OM: na parri det parri parri parri No boy DEM boy boy boy no boy, that’s a boy, boy, boy. Olivia: ei parri Yes boy yeah boy TABLE 2 | MLT observations for focus children and caregivers, Tennant Creek. Again, it is typically nouns that elicit Walmajarri. Indeed, Loakes et al. (2012) demonstrated that children in Yakanarra have a relatively good receptive knowledge of Walmajarri nouns related to the presence of these words in their input. Nouns are not the only targets of switching however. The traditional language is also used frequently for tag questions (e.g., payi “is that OK?” as in Extract 6 below). And in the following example, Emily’s mother (EM) issues her command first in Walmajarri and then in Kriol. This use of both languages in parallel utterances has also been documented in Tennant Creek speech styles (Disbray, 2008): yutanti Sit-down-IMP sit down EM: sidan Sit down sit down. Child (T2) CG (T1) CG (T2) Natalie and NM 2.0 1.4 4.1 24.6 Melanie and MM 2.0 4.3 6.3 26.3 Sarah and SM 1.8 5.5 6.6 35.3 Belinda and BM 1.9 5.2 8.3 15.1 Average 1.9 4.1 6.3 25.3 Time 1 Time 2 Natalie and NM 0.48 0.06 Melanie and MM 0.32 0.16 Sarah and SM 0.27 0.16 Belinda and BM 0.23 0.35 Average 0.33 0.18 Of course in cases of multilingual language acquisition there is the added complication that young children may not yet have learnt to discriminate between codes as the adult target does. This is not a question we will dwell on here, but it is important to acknowledge that the necessity of distinguishing codes for the purpose of quantitative analysis can obfuscate the social reality in terms of how speakers actually understand and manipulate codes. As we have seen, the contact varieties of Wumpurrarni English and FVKriol are the preferred languages for communication, but the traditional languages, Warumungu and Walmajarri, have a continued presence in interaction through a range of multilingual practices. Both communities appear to demonstrate similar uses of the traditional language in terms of the linguistic interaction of codes. However, an understanding of the social aspects of code use will require further research. In the next section we focus on how children’s productive language develops across the time periods. Receptive multilingualism, i.e., the use of one language per participant in an interaction is known to be a common practice in these communities (see e.g., the example in Loakes et al. (2013, p. 700) where a child speaks FVKriol to her grandmother and is responded to in Walmajarri). In Yakanarra it appears to be largely an intergenerational practice, and so perhaps is a product of language shift, but this is not the case in all communities (e.g., in Maningrida, Arnhem Land—see Elwell, 1982). Indeed, this kind of code-switching is known to have been a stable linguistic practice traditionally with distinct pragmatic and social functions (Wilkins and Nash, 2008; Singer and Harris, forthcoming). Code-switching raises a number of issues and questions for our analysis and for research of this kind more generally. Firstly, it presents a challenge for data analysis in that while it is uncontroversial in theory that code-switching differs from lexical borrowing (i.e., where a foreign word has been fully adapted into a host language), it is not always clear in practice (Poplack, 2004). For example, if the surface form is uninflected, if it occupies a slot shared by both languages, and/or if phonological cues are absent or unclear, the task is by no means a straightforward one. The challenge is mitigated to some extent by the fact that the researchers know the communities well and are able to make judgments based on their knowledge of the standard adult language and common community linguistic practices. Furthermore, the methodological issue begs a larger question regarding the social reality of code distinction: are speakers attending to the difference between codes? If not, should the researcher?8 Developmental Measures Mean Length of Turn (MLT) The MLT provides an impression of overall utterance length, while the related measure, MLT ratio, reflects the conversational load of each interactant. MLT is the average of the number of words used per turn, while conversational load is the ratio of child MLT over caregiver MLT. MLT values and the associated MLT ratios are shown in Tables 2, 3 below for Tennant Creek. As might be expected, the Tennant Creek children use slightly more than double the number of words per turn at T2 compared to T1. At T1 the children all appear to be at a similar stage, with results ranging from 1.8 to 2.0 words per turn on average. At Time 2, all of the children, except Natalie, have a 8 See work on code-mixing and “translanguaging” for further exploration of these points, especially (García, 2007; Creese and Blackledge, 2010; García and Wei, 2014). Frontiers in Psychology | www.frontiersin.org Child (T1) TABLE 3 | MLT ratios, Tennant Creek. Extract 3: EM’s direction to Emily 2;6, T1. EM: Tennant Creek 8 April 2015 | Volume 6 | Article 514 Vaughan et al. Child-caregiver interaction in Indigenous Australia marked increase in words usage, ranging from 4.3 to 5.5 words per turn. And as we see below, Natalie’s results may be task based. For the caregivers, MLT values are always higher than the children’s, especially at Time 2. At Time 1, the average MLT was 6.3 with a range of 4.1–8.3. However, the most striking result from this table is the average length of utterances used by the caregivers at Time 2, with an average of 25.3 words per turn, equating to four times as many words per turn on average than used at Time 1. Results for individual speakers range from 15.1 for Belinda’s great-grandmother to 35.3 for Sarah’s mother. Extract 4 is from Sarah’s mother at T2 and clearly shows the relatively monologic nature of her talk. She is telling a story and is not often interrupted by Sarah. This type of narration routine was observed in a previous study of Tennant Creek child-caregiver interaction (Disbray, 2008). Here adults tended to tell “elaborate” stories possibly as a strategy to maintain the children’s attention and to model story-telling. SM: In Extract 5, Natalie is playing with blocks, and her mother provides a series of requests and explanations. She draws the child’s attention to the construction depicted on the box containing the blocks, encouraging the child to build a similar construction. The especially long number of words per turn is typical of this session, yet markedly different to the length of utterance for the same speaker at T1. In this particular case, NM uses 32 words before Natalie’s one word response. Extract 5: NM’s interaction with Natalie 4;7, Time 2. Extract 4: SM’s interaction with Sarah 3;6, Time 2. SM: SM: deya im- kayi mami na There 3S POSS mother now there’s it’s mother now iya damob bin jeis-im na Here that group PST chase-TR now luk look here, that group were chasing it, now look. im dubala bin jeis-im bat 2 NOM PST chase-TR QUANT9 3S na now two of them were chasing it now. NM: yu gid dat pitja deya luk 2S get Dem picture there look you get that picture there, look. NM: si deya See there see there. NM: yu trai du dat 2S try do that you try to do that NM: luk, dem deya Look 3Pl there look, them there. NM: yu kin meik haus 2S can make house you can make a house. NM: kasel castle castle. NM: si tivi an bed yu kin meik im See TV and bed 2S can make 3S see the TV and bed, you can make them Sarah: na na dat dat Now now DEM DEM now now, that that. SM: stik bin pok-im im Stick PST poke-TR 3S the stick poked him. NM: luk what kain pants Look what kind pants look what kind of pants. kwik quickly quickly. NM: weya dat Harvey? Where that Harvey where’s that Harvey? SM: SM: SM: SM: stik bin pok-im im Stick PST poke-TR 3S the stick poked him. Natalie: dawan That one that one. im, gad, im still gad shangayi 3S got 3S still got shanghai he’s got, he’s still got the shanghai (sling-shot). The related MLT ratio (child/caregiver MLT) or conversational load is one way of representing the proportion of the utterance attributable to each speaker. We can expect this to be low at T2 given the long caregiver utterances as shown in Table 310 . These data suggest the conversational burden rests mostly with the caregivers in all instances, although T2 is more variable wan bala jeis-im One 3NOM chase-TR one of them chases it. 9 10 “bat” is an event quantifier indicating the event is not performed once by one individual. Frontiers in Psychology | www.frontiersin.org A ratio of 1 would indicate equality in conversational load, and any value less than 1 means the caregiver has a greater conversational burden than the child. 9 April 2015 | Volume 6 | Article 514 Vaughan et al. Child-caregiver interaction in Indigenous Australia TABLE 5 | MLT ratios, Yakanarra. TABLE 4 | MLT observations for focus children and caregivers, Yakanarra. Child (T1) Child (T2) CG (T1) CG (T2) Time 1 Time 2 Katherine and KM 5.3 2.5 10.2 7.6 Katherine and KM 0.52 0.33 Olivia and OM 3.1 5.2 7.6 8.9 Olivia and OM 0.41 0.58 Andrew and AM 2.3 5.1 27.3 14.8 Andrew and AM 0.08 0.34 Emily and EM 4.7 4.7 5.1 3.3 Emily and EM 0.92 1.42 Average 3.9 4.4 12.6 8.7 Average 0.48 0.67 than T1 with a low MLT for Natalie and her mother, reflective of Natalie’s minimal talk during the wooden block play, as seen in Extract 5 above. Melanie and Sarah have the same MLT ratio at T2 and Belinda is the only child to take on more of the conversational load at T2 compared to T1 increasing from 0.23 to 0.35 (still relatively low). The picture at Yakanarra is different, with more variability amongst participants, as shown in Table 4 which presents the MLT. For the Yakanarra child participants, there is little difference between T1 and T2, on average. At T1, their average MLT is around double the average of the Tennant Creek children (at 1.9), and at T2 their MLT of 4.4 is only marginally higher than at Time 1, and than the Tennant Creek children’s MLT at T2 (4.1). Thus, while Yakanarra children have a much higher average MLT at T1, by T2 they are very similar. It is likely that the MLT difference between Tennant Creek and Yakanarra children at T1 is linked to the difference in the average age between participants, since the Tennant Creek children are younger. At T1, the Yakanarra children’s MLT values correlate with their age: Katherine is the oldest participant and has an MLT of 5.3, but Andrew, the youngest only averages 2.3. At Time 2 however, Katherine has the lowest MLT. There is considerable variation in Yakanarra. Two children increase in MLT from T1 to T2 (Olivia and Andrew), and two do not—Katherine’s MLT is lower at T2, while Emily’s is the same. Thus, the range of values for MLT is quite broad at both times, between 2.3 and 5.3 words per turn at T1, and 2.5–5.2 at T2. This differs from the Tennant Creek results where the MLT fitted within a narrow range at each time period. For the caregivers, results are also different from Tennant Creek. At T1, the Yakanarra children have twice the average words per turn than Tennant Creek children, and this is also true for caregivers, where the average 12.6 words per turn is double those for Tennant Creek. At T2, Yakanarra caregivers have a lower MLT than at T1, while in Tennant Creek they were four times higher. Additionally, MLT values for the Tennant Creek caregivers fall within a relatively narrow range of values, whereas for the Yakanarra caregivers there is a wide range from 5.1 to 27.3 at T1 and from 3.3 to 14.8 at T2. Finally, three Yakanarra caregivers have similar MLT values to their children; the exception is Andrew and AM, with AM having exceptionally high MLT values (27.3 at T1, and 14.8 at T12). This is reflected in the MLT ratio, as shown in Table 5. Yakanarra children have, on average, a higher MLT than the Tennant Creek children at both time periods. This indicates Frontiers in Psychology | www.frontiersin.org that the Yakanarra children generally have a greater share of the conversation when interacting with their caregivers compared to the Tennant Creek children in these sessions. While neither group is approaching an equal conversational load, the Yakanarra children have a higher MLT ratio at T1 than the Tennant Creek children had at both T1 and T2, and they increase their conversational share at T2. A brief note about individual variation is important given these results. At T1, Andrew’s ratio is especially low at 0.08 indicating that he barely contributes, verbally, to the interaction. Emily, on the other hand, has an MLT ratio approaching 1.0 suggesting equal conversational load between her and her mother. At T2, Katherine and Andrew have similar, relatively low, MLT ratios although this is an increase for Andrew, but a decrease for Katherine. Emily is the only participant with a higher conversational burden than her mother. Emily’s input remains the same at T1 and T2, but her mother’s changes, with less input at T2 (see Extract 10 for an example of this pair’s interaction). Extracts 6 and 7 from Olivia’s interaction with OM are from an identical task where a book is being read. At T1, Olivia averaged three words per turn on average, and five at T2. OM averaged 7.6 words per turn at T1 and 9 at T2 yielding a slightly greater conversational load for Olivia at T2. Extract 6: OM’s interaction with Olivia 2;7, Time 1. 10 OM: en wat i bin du? And what 3S PST do And what has it done? OM: luk wat i bin du? Look what 3S PST do look, what has it done? OM: i bin fol dan, payi? 3S PST fall down isn’t it It’s fallen down, hasn’t it? Olivia: ye yes yeah. OM: oi i bin fol dan Oh 3S PST fall down oh, it’s fallen down. Olivia: no i rait. No 3S right no its OK. April 2015 | Volume 6 | Article 514 Vaughan et al. Child-caregiver interaction in Indigenous Australia M: en we det jikjik la is And where the chicken LOC 3S mami, payi? mother isn’t it and where is that chicken’s - its at his mother’s isn’t it? Olivia: im iya 3S here it’s here. differently to others. In Extract 8, they are looking at figurines, and the reasons for Andrew’s comparatively low MLT (5.1 to AM’s 14.8) as well as relatively low conversational load (0.38), are clear. Extract 8: AM’s interaction with Andrew 4;7, Time 2. AM: At T2, the development in Olivia’s interactional capability is clear. Here she has more, and longer turns, than at T1. Extract 7: OM’s interaction with Olivia 4;7, Time 2. OM: Olivia: Olivia: Olivia: OM: ei dis da mami wan fo det Hey Dem is mother NOM for Dem beibi baby hey this is the mother for that baby, si i garra bodul. See 3S get bottle see it’s got a bottle. en we dijan fo shanghai? And where Dem for shanghai and where is the shanghai (sling-shot). Andrew: de there there hu detwan? Who that one who’s that? AM: den dei tjeis-im bat im tjeis Then 3Pl chase-TR QUANT 3S chase then they chase it, chase ai dono hu det 1S don’t know who Dem I don’t know who that is. Andrew: im bat im tjeis-im bat im 3S but 3S chase-TR QUANT 3S it, chased it. xxx ting. Xxx thing [unintelligible word] thing AM: a pupala Ah poorthing ah poor thing. ye lil skul gel Yes little school girl yeah it’s a little school girl. AM: en diswan god lil And this one got little mobailfon mobile phone and this one has got a little mobile phone. Olivia: ye yes yeah. OM: dei bin ged im hepi 3PL PST get 3S happy They were happy. AM: yu luk-im lil mobailfon 2S look-TR little mobile phone you look, a little mobile phone. Olivia la nes i bin bi LOC nest 3S PST be it was in the nest Andrew: ye ai garram iya Yes 1S got here yeah I’ve got it here. Olivia: we is mami wan? Where POSS mother Det where is its mother? Olivia: de? there there? OM: na is mami bin go ged-im No POSS mother PST go get-TR no its mother has gone In this section, we have seen that utterance length is used differently in each of the two communities analyzed. In Tennant Creek, both children and caregivers tend to have an increase in MLT over time. As illustrated, the caregivers use exceptionally long utterances at Time 2, and this impacts on conversational load where we saw that Tennant Creek children take on less of the overall conversational burden at Time 2. The Yakanarra caregivers, by contrast, use shorter utterances on average at Time 2, while the children have slightly longer utterances. In Yakanarra, the children’s conversational burden is generally greater at Time 2, and one of the children actually has a higher conversational load than her caregiver. The caregivers in the two communities seem to respond differently with respect to quantity of talk. Tennant Creek caregivers bat mangarri bla im QUANT food for him to get food for it. Recall that Andrew is the youngest child in the Yakanarra cohort, and his interaction with his mother patterned somewhat Frontiers in Psychology | www.frontiersin.org 11 April 2015 | Volume 6 | Article 514 Vaughan et al. Child-caregiver interaction in Indigenous Australia use more talk at Time 2 perhaps recognizing their children’s increased receptive capacity, while the Yakanarra participants use less talk on average perhaps making room for their children’s productive capacity. It may be the age difference of the children, with the children older at Yakanarra, that elicits this response. i bin jamp of fom dat nes 3S Pst jump off from Dem nest it jumped off from that nest Melanie: an dei gada gid im na And 3Pl got to get 3S now ini? isn’t it and they have to get it now, don’t they? MLU MLU is a measure of morphological complexity, reflecting the ratio of morphemes to utterances, and is generally considered useful for comparing development across time points. MLU is often compared to standardized values, but this is not possible here as there are no standardized MLU values for Kriol and the traditional languages featured. However, values can be used comparatively across the corpus. In this study, MLU is measured using morphemes primarily from Kriol, with some traditional language morphemes also included in the analysis. This is justified because, as seen earlier, the number of traditional language morphemes used by participants was very minimal. MLU values for Tennant Creek are presented in Table 6 below. All children and their caregivers use morphologically more complex utterances at T2 with the children having the greatest increase from 1.7 to 5.3. At T2, both groups have a similar average MLU (5.3 and 5.6), reflecting similar grammatical complexity despite the marked differences in utterance length described earlier. At T1 the caregivers tend to have a much higher MLU than their children (besides Natalie and NM), but at T2 they are almost at parity. The exception is Melanie and MM, with MM having a higher MLU than Melanie. As with MLT, the Tennant Creek data again falls within a relatively narrow range, although Natalie and NM have somewhat lower MLUs, and Melanie and MM slightly higher values. Extract 5 gave an example of Natalie and NM’s interaction at T2 and Extract 9 is an example from Melanie and MM’s (see also Extract 1). Verbal morphemes (preceded by an underscore) help to explain the higher MLU. Both participants use relatively long utterances providing a useful illustration of average turn length. Yakanarra results for MLU are shown below. In Yakanarra, MLU is narrower than Tennant Creek between 2.4 and 4.4, with caregivers having a higher MLU than the children at both times. Emily and EM are the exception to this, with Emily’s MLU at T2 slightly higher than EM’s. MLU increases at T2 for the children, and decreases slightly for the caregivers. Individual variation is fairly minimal as with the other measures analyzed in this study. Average values point to an increase in child MLU across the time periods, with three of the four children showing higher MLU values at Time 2 (an increase of 1.2 in each case). Katherine is the exception here, with a slightly higher MLU at Time 1 (3.0 compared with 2.6). For the caregivers, average MLU decreased slightly across the time periods. Results in Table 7 show that this is the case for all except OM, who has a slightly higher MLU at Time 2 (4.4 compared with 5.0). Examples of Olivia (and OM’s) speech are given in extracts 2, 6, and 7. Extracts 6 (T1) and 7 (T2) in particular illustrate Olivia’s increase in morpho-syntactic complexity across the time periods. Extract 10 shows Emily interacting with EM, which demonstrates EM’s relatively low MLU T2, given the demonstrably low morpho-syntactic complexity of her turn (it is also indicative of the pair’s MLT ratio, as discussed earlier). Extract 10: EM’s interaction with Emily 4;6, Time 2. Emily: en de det beed And Dem Dem bird and there, that bird. an dei bin keriy im, And 3Pl Pst carry 3S and they were carrying it, EM: yu luk det nes 2Pl look Dem next you look, that nest. keriy-im bat im na dat nes, carry-TR QUANT 3S from Dem nest carrying it from that nest - Emily Extract 9: MM’s interaction with Melanie 4;3, Time 2. MM: TABLE 6 | MLU, Tennant Creek. en det nes And Dem nest and that nest. TABLE 7 | MLU, Yakanarra. Child (T1) Child (T2) CG (T1) CG (T2) Natalie and NM 1.8 3.7 1.4 3.5 Katherine and KM 3 Melanie and MM 2 6.3 3.9 7.5 Olivia and OM 2.6 Sarah and SM 1.5 5.4 4 5.6 Andrew and AM 2.1 Belinda and BM 1.6 5.9 4.1 5.9 Emily and EM Average 1.7 5.3 3.4 5.6 Average Frontiers in Psychology | www.frontiersin.org Child (T1) 12 Child (T2) CG (T1) CG (T2) 2.6 5.1 4.4 3.8 4.4 5 3.3 4.8 4.3 2 3.2 3.1 2.7 2.4 3.2 4.4 4.1 April 2015 | Volume 6 | Article 514 Vaughan et al. Emily: Child-caregiver interaction in Indigenous Australia language capacity was not clear for Tennant Creek when we analyzed MLT and MLT ratio, we might infer that the especially long turns used by their caregivers is a response to their greater receptive knowledge at T2. While not an explicit focus of this study, work on Baby Talk registers in Australian languages formed part of the theoretical grounding of register (see Laughren, 1984; Bavin, 1992, 1993; Kral and Ellis, 2008; O’Shannessy, 2011; Jones and Meakins, 2013; Turpin et al., 2014). We are, thus, able to reflect on the language input to the children in our study with this work in mind. Certain features noted in previous studies were observable in our data (largely use of repetition (see, e.g., Extract 2) and slower speech rate), but not the full range of features described in the studies above. Further research is required to fully investigate the existence and nature of this register in Tennant Creek and Yakanarra. As discussed in the introduction, relatively little is known about child language development outside middle-class Western societies, and measures of analysis have tended to develop from languages spoken in these environments. As such, it is worth reflecting on our results to determine the validity of the measures we used for analysing contact varieties. One measure in particular, MLU, was used in this study in quite broadly and language types were grouped together (i.e., both Kriol and the traditional language) in calculating it. However, use of the traditional language was quantitatively minimal and so this is unlikely to have had much impact on results. A crucial issue, which warrants further investigation, is that of variation. Heavy Kriol varieties have a more complex morphology and use grammatical and case-marking morphemes not used in English. At T1, where children ranged between 1;8 and 3;6, there was little evidence that they used such morphology. However, the children use a wider variety of verbal and nominal morphemes, and more of them, at T2 as reflected in the higher MLU for both cohorts. This could be for a number of reasons—the data may indicate acquisition of morphology, but it may also be an indication that children are using a heavier style of Kriol at T2. Semantic case-marking is also an optional feature, often occurring with relatively low frequency, so it may simply be that case-marking did not appear in the T1 data. In future work, it will be important for researchers to analyse individual speaker styles closely, and to relate them to children’s stylistic use. Cross-sectional data from older children in Tennant Creek show use of complex morphology, through the use of transitive marking on verbs, and semantic case-marking (Disbray, 2009), particularly in heavy WE styles. These results are useful for contributing to knowledge on the types of values to expect for Kriol varieties where MLU is concerned, and a more detailed analysis, beyond the scope of this paper, may throw further light on this. The results of this study support those of Hoff (2006) who observed considerable variation in type and amount of direct language input children in these age groups received. These communities are undergoing, or have undergone in recent year, sizeable shifts in the languages spoken, commensurate with increasing variability in the input the children receive (see Loakes et al. (2013) for further discussion of this phenomenon). en de i flaiing And there 3S flying and there it’s flying. These MLU results indicate that in both communities the children use grammatically more complex utterances at T2. At both times, caregivers use more complex utterances than their children, but in Yakanarra the caregivers use less complex utterances at T2 than at T1. In other words, while children’s language is developing over the 2-year period, as would be expected, caregiver responses pattern differently in each community. Discussion and Conclusion This study compared language use in two remote Indigenous Australian communities. Reflecting a loss of the traditional languages of the area, creole varieties—Wumpurrarni English and Fitzroy Valley Kriol, respectively—are used predominantly by the child-caregiver pairs studied in Tennant Creek and Yakanarra. The individual variation in language use observed was consistent with our previous study in Yakanarra where traditional language tends to be reserved for use by, and with, older participants aged 50+ (e.g., Loakes et al., 2013). McConvell (2008) also discusses a general trend for language shift in Indigenous Australia, where children appear to be largely monolingual (speaking a variety of Kriol). This compares to the situation previously in Australia, where community members tended to be multilingual in two or more traditional languages as well as English (e.g., Brandl and Walsh, 1982; Singer and Harris, forthcoming). While our results point to a general decline in multilingualism and its attendant linguistic practices in Indigenous Australia, we have noted a range of uses for the surviving elements of the traditional languages featured. Code-switching and receptive multilingualism have important social and pragmatic functions, even, or perhaps especially, in cases of language change. Indeed, it can be the case that low-frequency linguistic forms carry significant pragmatic force (i.e., contributing to “markedness”). McConvell (2008) notes that such uses of traditional languages may have strong social meaning as “acts of identity” (see also Le Page and Tabouret-Keller, 1985). As found in the early studies of caregiver talk (e.g., Snow, 1972, 1977; Sachs et al., 1976; Ferguson, 1977) for the caregivers in Tennant Creek, speech increases in MLT with age, reflecting similar patterns of development and we may assume, consequently, that caregivers make similar adjustments in terms of fine-tuning to their speech as those found in previous studies such as Vosoughi (2010) and that this is a function of the child’s age. In contrast, the Yakanarra caregivers tend to use shorter and fewer turns at T2, and may respond to their children’s language development by making room for their productive language capacity. For caregivers then, the main difference across the communities is the quantity of talk used. In both Tennant Creek and Yakanarra we observed the general developmental characteristics of focus children using data collected approximately 2 years apart. The children displayed more developed language skills at T2, an unsurprising result given that they were on average 2 years older than at T1. While productive Frontiers in Psychology | www.frontiersin.org 13 April 2015 | Volume 6 | Article 514 Vaughan et al. Child-caregiver interaction in Indigenous Australia Tennant Creek were younger at the start of the study than those in Yakanarra. In tandem with Loakes et al.’s (2013) findings for Yakanarra, this research has contributed to a depiction of both language input to children, as well as their own language production. The former study elucidated how this language varies synchronically (according to interlocutor age), while the current study has focused on how children’s language varies diachronically across time points 2 years apart. These results are a start in developing a picture of how children acquire language in these two multilingual Indigenous Australian communities. This study has provided an initial insight into child-caregiver interaction in two remote Indigenous communities. Despite the different community structures—with Yakanarra being relatively closed, and Tennant Creek having more varied demographics— results are similar in terms of general developmental patterns, and the language types used. The creole variety local to each community is the preferred language of communication at both time points. For measures of morphosyntactic complexity (MLU), children in both communities use more complex utterances at T2 than at T1, and child-caregiver averages are similar to each other at T2. For measures of utterance length (MLT), there are more notable differences between the two communities for both children and caregivers at T2; for children in Tennant Creek, MLT doubled on average between T1 and T2, while in Yakanarra children were more variable. Strikingly, caregivers’ MLT quadrupled between T1 and T2 in Tennant Creek, while in Yakanarra caregivers’ MLT was lower at T2 than T1. We note, however, that the samples used here were convenience samples, and very small ones with only four dyads in each community. We may attribute some of the different results also to the fact that the children in Acknowledgments We would like to thank all the participants, children and adults who contributed data to this study. Thanks also to the anonymous reviewers for their insightful comments, and to Jane Simpson for assistance with the interlinear glossing. The study was supported by Australian Research Council Discovery Grants (DP0343189 and DP0877762). References Creese, A., and Blackledge, A. (2010). Translanguaging in the bilingual classroom: a pedagogy for learning and teaching? Mod. Lang. J. 94, 103–115. doi: 10.1111/j.1540-4781.2009.00986.x De Houwer, A. (1999). “Environmental factors in early bilingual development: the role of parental beliefs and attitudes,” in Bilingualism and Migration, eds G. Extra and L. Verhoeven (New York, NY: Mouton de Gruyter), 75–96. De Houwer, A. (2005). “Early bilingual acquisition: focus on morphosyntax and the separate development hypothesis,” in Handbook of Bilingualism: Psycholinguistic Approaches, eds J. F. Kroll and A. M. B. De Groot (Oxford: Oxford University Press), 30–48. De Houwer, A. (2007). Parental language input patterns and children’s bilingual use. Appl. Psycholinguist. 28, 411–424. doi: 10.1017/S0142716407 070221 Díaz-Campos, M. (2005). “The emergence of adult-like command of sociolinguistic variables: a study of consonant weakening in Spanish-speaking children,” in Studies in the Acquisition of the Hispanic Languages: Papers from the 6th conference on the acquisition of Spanish and Portuguese as first and second languages, ed D. Eddington (Somerville, MA: Cascadilla), 56–65. Disbray, S. (2008). “Story-telling styles: a study of adult-child interactions in narrations of a picture book in Tennant Creek,” in Children’s Language and Multilingualism: Indigenous Language Use at Home and School, eds J. Simpson and G. Wigglesworth (London: Continuum International Press), 56–98. Disbray, S. (2009). More than One Way to Catch a Frog: Children’s Discourse in a Contact Setting. Ph.D. dissertation, The University of Melbourne. Disbray, S., and Simpson, J. (2004). The expression of possession in Wumpurrarni English, Tennant Creek. Monash Univ. Linguist. Papers 4, 65–86. Eades, D. (1996). “Aboriginal english,” in Atlas of Languages of Intercultural Communication in the Pacific, Asia, and the Americas, eds S. A. Wurm, P. Mühlhäusler, and D. T. Tryon (Berlin; New York: Mouton de Gruyter), 133–141. Egan, A. (1986). Pintaru-kurlu “The Quail”. Yuendumu, NT: Bilingual Resources Development Unit. Elwell, V. M. R. (1982). Some social factors affecting multilingualism among Aboriginal Australians: a case study of Maningrida. Int. J. Soc. Lang. 36, 83–103. doi: 10.1515/ijsl.1982.36.83 Ferguson, C. A. (1977). “Baby talk as a simplified register,” in Talking to Children: Language Input and Acquisition, eds C. E. Snow and C. A. Ferguson (New York, NY: Cambridge University Press), 219–235. Andrews, J. (2008). “Bringing up our yorta yorta children,” in Contexts of Child Development: Culture, Policy and Intervention, eds G. Robinson, U. Eickelkamp, J. Goodnow, and I. Katz (Darwin: Charles Darwin University Press), 23–35. Bavin, E. (1992). “The acquisition of Warlpiri,” in The Crosslinguistic Study of Language Acquisition, Vol. 3, ed D. I. Slobin (Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum), 309–371. Bavin, E. (1993). “Language and culture: socialization in a Warlpiri community,” in Language and Culture in Aboriginal Australia, eds M. Walsh and C. Yallop (Canberra, ACT: Aboriginal Studies Press), 85–96. Bavin, E., and Shopen, T. (1985). “Warlpiri and English: language in contact,” in Australia, Meeting Place of Languages, ed M. Clyne (Canberra, ACT: Pacific Linguistics), 81–94. Berko Gleason, J. (eds.). (2005). The Development of Language, 6th Edn. Boston: Pearson/Allyn & Bacon. Bialystok, E., and Feng, X. (2011). “Language proficiency and its implications for monolingual and bilingual children,” in Language and Literacy Development in Bilingual Settings, eds A. Durgunogle and C. Goldenberg (New York, NY: Guilford), 121–138. Bornstein, M. H., Haynes, M. O., and Painter, K. M. (1998). Sources of child vocabulary competence: a multivariate model. J. Child Lang. 25, 367–394. doi: 10.1017/S0305000998003456 Brandl, M. M., and Walsh, M. (1982). Speakers of many tongues: toward understanding multilingualism among Aboriginal Australians. Int. J. Sociol. Lang. 36, 71–81. Brown, P. (2001). “Learning to talk about motion UP and DOWN in Tzeltal: is there a language-specific bias for verb learning?” in Language Acquisition and Conceptual Development, eds M. Bowerman and S. C. Levinson (Cambridge: Cambridge University Press), 512–543. Butcher, A. R. (2008). Linguistic aspects of Australian Aboriginal English. Clin. Linguist. Phon. 22, 625–642. doi: 10.1080/02699200802223535 Conboy, B. T., and Thal, D. J. (2006). Ties between the lexicon and grammar: crosssectional and longitudinal studies of bilingual toddlers. Child Dev. 77, 712–735. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2006.00899.x Crago, M. B., Allen, S. E. M., and Hough-Eyamie, W. P. (1997). “Exploring innateness through cultural and linguistic variation: an Inuit example,” in The Biological Basis of Language, ed M. J. Gopnik (Oxford: Oxford University Press), 70–90. Frontiers in Psychology | www.frontiersin.org 14 April 2015 | Volume 6 | Article 514 Vaughan et al. Child-caregiver interaction in Indigenous Australia Kaye, K. (1980). Why we don’t “baby talk” to babies. J. Child Lang. 7, 498–507. doi: 10.1017/S0305000900002804 Kral, I., and Ellis, E. M. (2008). “Children, language and literacy in the Ngaanyatjarra lands,” in Children’s Language and Multilingualism, eds J. Simpson and G. Wigglesworth (London: Continuum), 154–172. Kulick, D. (1992). Language Shift and Cultural Reproduction. Socialization, Self and Syncretism in a Papua New Guinean Village. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Lanza, E. (1997). Language Mixing in Infant Bilingualism: A Sociolinguistic Perspective. Oxford: Clarendon Press. Laughren, M. (1984). Warlpiri Baby Talk. Aust. J. Linguist. 4, 73–88. doi: 10.1080/07268608408599321 Lee, J. (1987). Tiwi Today: A Study of Language Change in a Contact Situation. Canberra, ACT: Pacific Linguistics. Lee, J. (2004). Kriol-Ingglish Dikshenri. SIL Darwin: Summer Institute of Linguistics. Le Page, R., and Tabouret-Keller, A. (1985). Acts of Identity: Creole-Based Approaches to Language and Ethnicity. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. LeVine, R. A. (2004). “Challenging expert knowledge: findings from an African study of infant care and development,” in Childhood and Adolescence: CrossCultural Perspectives and Applications, eds U. P. Gielen and J. Roopnarine (Westport, CT: Praeger), 149–165. Lieven, E. V. M. (1994). “Crosslinguistic and crosscultural aspects of language addressed to children,” in Input and Interaction in Language Acquisition, eds C. Galloway and B. J. Richards (Cambridge: Cambridge University Press), 56–73. Loakes, D., Moses, K., Simpson, J., and Wigglesworth, G. (2012). Developing tests for the assessment of traditional language skill: a case study in an Indigenous Australian community. Lang. Assess. Q. 9, 311–330. doi: 10.1080/15434303.2011.653918 Loakes, D., Moses, K., Wigglesworth, G., Simpson, J., and Billington, R. (2013). Children’s language input: a study of a remote multilingual Indigenous Australian community. Multilingua 32, 683–711. doi: 10.1515/mult-2013-0032 Luykx, A. (2005). “Children as socializing agents: family language policy in situations of language shift,” in Proceedings of the 4th International Symposium on Bilingualism, eds J. Cohen, K. T. McAlister, K. Rolstad, and J. MacSwan (Somerville, MA: Cascadilla), 1407–1414. Makihara, M. (2005). Rapa Nui ways of speaking Spanish: language shift and socialization on Easter Island. Lang. Soc. 34, 727–762. doi: 10.1017/S004740450505027X Malcolm, I. (1996). Observations on the variability in the verb phrase in Aboriginal English. Aust. J. Linguist. 16, 145–165. doi: 10.1080/072686096085 99536 Malcolm, I., and Kaldor, S. (1991). “Aboriginal English – an overview,” in Language in Australia, ed S. Romaine (Cambridge: Cambridge University Press), 67–83. Malcolm, I. G., and Sharifian, F. (2002). Aspects of Aboriginal English oral discourse: an application of cultural schema theory. Discourse Stud. 4, 169-181. Malcolm, I. G., and Sharifian, F. (2007). “Multiwords in Aboriginal English,” in Phraseology and Culture in English, ed P. Skandera (Berlin, New York: Mouton De Gruyter). Marchman, V., Martinez-Sussman, C., and Dale, P. S. (2004). The languagespecific nature of grammatical development: evidence from bilingual language learners. Dev. Sci. 7, 212–224. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2004.00340.x McConvell, P. (2008). “Language mixing and language shift in Indigenous Australia,” in Children’s Language and Multilingualism: Indigenous Language use at Home and School, eds J. Simpson and G. Wigglesworth (London: Continuum International Press), 237–260. McConvell, P., and Meakins, F. (2005). Gurindji Kriol: a mixed language emerges from code-switching. Aust. J. Linguist. 25, 9–30. doi: 10.1080/07268600500110456 McConvell, P., and Thieberger, N. (2001). State of Indigenous Languages in Australia – 2001. Australia State of the Environment Technical Paper Series (Natural and Cultural Heritage), Series 2. Canberra, ACT: Department of the Environment and Heritage. McGregor, W. B. (2004). The Languages of the Kimberley, Western Australia. London: Routledge. Foulkes, P., Docherty, G. J., and Watt, D. (1999). Tracking the emergence of structured variation: realisations of (t) by Newcastle children. Leeds Work. Pap. Linguist. Phon. 7, 1–25. Foulkes, P., Docherty, G. J., and Watt, D. (2005). Phonological variation in child-directed speech. Language 81, 177–206. doi: 10.1353/lan.2005.0018 Gafaranga, J. (2010). Medium request: talking language shift into being. Lang. Soc. 39, 241–270. doi: 10.1017/S0047404510000047 García, O. (2007). “Foreword,” in Disinventing and Reconstituting Languages, eds S. Makoni and A. Pennycook (Clevedon: Multilingual Matters), xi-xv. García, O., and Wei, L. (2014). Translanguaging: Language, Bilingualism and Education. New York, NY: Palgrave Macmillan. Garrett, P. B. (2005). What a language is good for: language socialization, language shift, and the persistence of code-specific genres in St. Lucia. Lang. Soc. 34, 327–361. doi: 10.1017/S0047404505050128 Garrett, P. B., and Baquedano-López, P. (2002). Language socialization: reproduction and continuity, transformation and change. Annu. Rev. Anthropol. 31, 339–361. doi: 10.1146/annurev.anthro.31.040402.085352 Geneshi, C., and Glupczynski, T. (2006). “Language and literacy research: multiple methods and perspectives,” in Handbook of Complementary Methods in Education Research, eds J. L. Green, G. Camilli, and P. B. Elmore (Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum), 657–680. Hamilton, A. (1981). Nature and Nurture: Aboriginal Child-rearing in Northcentral Arnhem Land. Canberra, ACT: Australian Institute of Aboriginal Studies. Harkness, S. (1977). “Aspects of the social environment and first language acquisition in rural Africa,” in Talking to Children: Language Input and Acquisition, eds C. E. Snow and C. A. Ferguson (New York, NY: Cambridge University Press), 309–318. Harris, J. (1993). “Losing and gaining a language: the story of Kriol in the Northern Territory,” in Language and Culture in Aboriginal Australia, eds M. Walsh and C. Yallop (Canberra, ACT: Aboriginal Studies Press), 155–168. Hart, B., and Risley, T. R. (1995). Meaningful Differences in the Everyday Experience of Young American Children. Baltimore, MD: Brookes. Heath, S. B. (1983). Ways With Words: Language, Life and Work in Communities and Classrooms. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Hoff, E. (2001). Language Development. Belmont, CA: Wadsworth. Hoff, E. (2003). The specificity of environmental influence: socioeconomic status affects early vocabulary development via maternal speech. Child Dev. 74, 1368–1378. doi: 10.1111/1467-8624.00612 Hoff, E. (2006). How social contexts support and shape language development. Dev. Rev. 26, 55–58. doi: 10.1016/j.dr.2005.11.002 Hoff, E., Core, C., Place, S., Rumiche, R., Señor, M., and Parra, M. (2012). Dual language exposure and early bilingual development. J. Child Lang. 39, 1–27. doi: 10.1017/S0305000910000759 Hoff, E., and Naigles, L. (2002). How children use input to acquire a lexicon. Child Dev. 73, 418–433. doi: 10.1111/1467-8624.00415 Hoff-Ginsberg, E. (1998). The relation of birth order and socioeconomic status to children’s language experience and language development. Appl. Psycholinguist. 19, 603–629. doi: 10.1017/S0142716400010389 Hudson, J. (1985). Grammatical and Semantic Aspects of Fitzroy Valley Kriol. Darwin, Summer Institute of Linguistics. Huttenlocher, J., Haight, W., Bryk, A., Seltzer, M., and Lyons, T. (1991). Early vocabulary growth: relation to language input and gender. Dev. Psychol. 27, 236–248. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.27.2.236 Huttenlocher, J., Vasilyeva, M., Cymweman, E., and Levine, S. (2002). Language input at home and at school: relation to child syntax. Cogn. Psychol. 45, 337–374. doi: 10.1016/S0010-0285(02)00500-5 Huttenlocher, J., Vasilyeva, M., Waterfall, H. R., Vevea, J. L., and Hedges, L. V. (2007). The varieties of speech to young children. Dev. Psychol. 43, 1062–1083. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.43.5.1062 Jones, C., and Meakins, F. (2013). The phonological forms and perceived functions of janyarrp, the Gurindji “baby talk” register. Lingua 134, 170–193. doi: 10.1016/j.lingua.2013.07.004 Kaldor, S., and Malcolm, I. (1982). “Aboriginal English in country and remote areas: a Western Australian perspective,” in English and the Aboriginal Child, eds R. D. Eagleson, S. Kaldor, and I. Malcolm (Canberra, ACT: Curriculum Development Centre), 75–112. Frontiers in Psychology | www.frontiersin.org 15 April 2015 | Volume 6 | Article 514 Vaughan et al. Child-caregiver interaction in Indigenous Australia Poplack, S. (2004). “Code-Switching,” in Sociolinguistics. An International Handbook of the Science of Language and Society, 2nd Edn., eds U. Ammon, N. Dittmar, K. J. Mattheier, and P. Trudgill (Berlin: Walter de Gruyter), 589–596. Pye, C. (1992). “The acquisition of K’iche’ (Maya)”, in The Crosslinguistic Study of Language Acquisition, Vol. 3, ed D. I. Slobin (Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum), 221–308. Richman, A. L., Miller, P. M., and LeVine, R. A. (1992). Cultural and educational variations in maternal responsiveness. Dev. Psychol. 28, 614–621. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.28.4.614 Rondal, J. A. (1980). Fathers’ and mothers’ speech in early language development. J. Child Lang. 7, 353–369. doi: 10.1017/S0305000900002671 Rowe, M. L. (2008). Child-directed speech: relation to socioeconomic status, knowledge of child development and child vocabulary skill. J. Child Lang. 35, 185–205. doi: 10.1017/S0305000907008343 Sachs, J., Brown, R., and Salerno, R. A. (1976). “Adults’ speech to children,” in Baby Talk and Infant Speech, eds W. von Raffler-Engel and Y. Lebraun (Lisse: Swets & Zeitlinger), 240–245. Sandefur, J. R. (1979). An Australian Creole in the Northern Territory: A Description of Ngukurr-Bamyili Dialects Part 1. Work Papers of SIL-AAB Series B, Vol. 3, Darwin: Summer Institute of Linguistics - Australian Aborigines Branch. Saxton, M. (2009). “The inevitability of child-directed speech,” in Language Acquisition, ed S. H. Foster-Cohen (London: Palgrave Macmillan), 62–86. Saxton, M. (2010). Child Language: Acquisition and Development. London: Sage. Sharifian, F. (2005). Cultural conceptualisations in English words: a study of Aboriginal children in Perth. Lang. Educ. 19, 74–88. doi: 10.1080/09500780508668805 Simpson, J., and Wigglesworth, G. (2008). Children’s Language and Multilingualism: Indigenous Language use at Home and School. London: Continuum International Press. Singer, R., and Harris, S. (forthcoming). What practices and ideologies support small-scale multilingualism? A case study of unexpected language survival in an Australian Indigenous community. J. Socioling. Slobin, D. (eds.). (1997). The Crosslinguistic Study of Language Acquisition, Vol. 4. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. Smith, J., Durham, M., and Fortune, L. (2007). Community, caregiver and child in the acquisition of variation in a Scottish dialect. Lang. Var. Change 19, 63–99. doi: 10.1017/S0954394507070044 Smith, J., Durham, M., and Richards, H. (2013). The social and linguistic in the acquisition of sociolinguistic norms: caregivers, children, and variation. Linguistics 51, 285–324. doi: 10.1515/ling-2013-0012 Smith-Hefner, N. J. (1998). The linguistic socialization of Javanese children in two communities. Anthropol. Linguist. 30, 166–198. Snow, C. E. (1972). Mothers’ speech to children learning language. Child Dev. 43, 549–565. doi: 10.2307/1127555 Snow, C. E. (1977). “Mothers’ speech research: from input to interaction,” in Talking to Children: Language Input and Acquisition, eds C. E. Snow and C. A. Ferguson (New York, NY: Cambridge University Press), 31–49. Snow, C. E. (1995). “Issues in the study of input: fine-tuning, universality, individual and developmental differences, and necessary causes,” in Handbook of Child Language, eds P. Fletcher and B. MacWhinney (Oxford: Blackwell), 180–193. ten Thije, J. D., Rehbein, J., and Verschik, A. (2012). Receptive Multilingualism— Introduction. Int. J. Biling. 16, 245–247. doi: 10.1177/1367006911426468 ten Thije, J. D., and Zeevaert, L. (eds.). (2007). Receptive Multilingualism: Linguistic Analyses, Language Policies and Didactic Concepts. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. Turpin, M., Demuth, K., and Campbell, A. N. (2014). “Phonological aspects of Arandic baby talk,” in Language Description Informed by Theory, eds R. Pensalfini, M. Turpin, and D. Guillemin (Amsterdam: John Benjamins), 49–80. Vosoughi, S. (2010). Interactions of Caregiver Speech and Early Word Learning in the Speechome Corpus: Computational Explorations. Cambridge, MA. Massachusetts Institute of Technology MSc thesis. Wigglesworth, G., and Simpson, J. (2008). “The language learning environment of preschool children in Indigenous communities,” in Children’s Language and Multilingualism: Indigenous Language use at Home and School, eds J. Simpson and G. Wigglesworth (London: Continuum International Press), 13–29. Wilkins, D. P., and Nash, D. (2008). “The European “discovery” of a multilingual Australia: the linguistic and ethnographic successes of a failed expedition,” in McWhinney, B. (2012). The Childes Project: Tools for Analyzing Talk, Vol. 1, 3rd Edn. New York; East Sussex: Psychology Press. Meakins, F. (2008). “Unravelling languages: multilingualism and language contact in Kalkaringi,” in Children’s Language and Multilingualism: Indigenous Language use at Home and School, eds J. Simpson and G. Wigglesworth (London: Continuum), 283–382. Meakins, F. (2011). Case-marking in Contact: The Development and Function of Case Morphology in Gurindji Kriol. Amsterdam: John Benjamins Publishing Company. Morrison, B., and Disbray, S. (2008). “Warumungu children and language in Tennant Creek,” in Warra wiltaniappendi = Strengthening languages. Proceedings of the Inaugural Indigenous Languages Conference (ILC) 2007 (Adelaide, SA: Australia), 107–111. Moses, K. (2009). How do Dinosaurs hug in the Kimberley? The Use of Questions by Aboriginal Caregivers and Children in a Walmajarri Community. Ph.D. dissertation, University of Melbourne. Moses, K., and Yallop, C. (2008). “Questions about questions,” in Children’s Language and Multilingualism: Indigenous Language use at Home and School, eds J. Simpson and G. Wigglesworth (London: Continuum International Press), 30–55. Naigles, L., and Hoff-Ginsberg, E. (1998). Why are some verbs learned before other verbs? Effects of input frequency and structure on children’s early verb use. J. Child Lang. 25, 95–120. doi: 10.1017/S0305000997003358 Nicoladis, E., and Genesee, F. (1997). Language development in preschool bilingual children. J. Speech Lang. Pathol. Audiol. 21, 258–270. O’Shannessy, C. (2005). Light warlpiri: a new language. Aust. J. Linguist. 25, 31–57. doi: 10.1080/07268600500110472 O’Shannessy, C. (2011). “Young children’s social meaning making in a new mixed language,” in Growing Up in Central Australia: New Anthropological Studies of Aboriginal Childhood and Adolescence, ed U. Eickelkamp (New York, NY; Oxford: Berghan Books), 131–155. O’Shannessy, C. (2012). The role of code-switched input to children in the origin of a new mixed language. Linguistics 50, 305–340. doi: 10.1515/ling2012-0011 Ochs, E., and Schieffelin, B. B. (1994). “Language acquisition and socialization: three developmental stories and their implications,” in Language, Culture and Society: A Book of Readings, 2nd Edn., ed B. G. Blount (Prospect Heights, IL: Waveland Press Inc.), 470–512. O’Shannessy, C. (2004). The Monster Stories: A Set of Picture Books to Elicit Overt Transitive Subjects in Oral Texts. Nijmegen: Max Planck Institute for Psycholinguistics. O’Shannessy, C. (2013). The role of multiple sources in the formation of an innovative auxiliary category in Light Warlpiri, a new Australian mixed language. Language 89, 328–354. doi: 10.1353/lan.2013.0025 Pan, B. A., Rowe, M. L., Singer, J. D., and Snow, C. E. (2005). Maternal correlates of growth in toddler vocabulary production in low-income families. Child Dev. 76, 763–782. doi: 10.1111/1467-8624.00498-i1 Paradis, J., and Genesee, F. (1996). Syntactic acquisition in bilingual children: autonomous or interdependent? Stud. Second Lang. Acquis. 18, 1–25. doi: 10.1017/S0272263100014662 Patterson, J. L. (1998). Expressive vocabulary development and word combinations of Spanish-English bilingual toddlers. Am. J. Speech Lang. Pathol. 7, 46–56. doi: 10.1044/1058-0360.0704.46 Patterson, J. L., and Pearson, B. Z. (2004). “Bilingual lexical development: influences, contexts and processes,” in Bilingual Language Development and Disorders in Spanish-English Speakers, ed B. A. Goldstein (Baltimore: Brookes), 77–104. Paugh, A. L. (2005). Multilingual play: children’s code-switching, role play, and agency in Dominica, West Indies. Lang. Soc. 34, 63–86. doi: 10.1017/S0047404505050037 Pearson, B. Z., and Fernandez, S. C. (1994). Patterns of interaction in the lexical development in two languages of bilingual infants. Lang. Learn. 44, 617–653. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-1770.1994.tb00633.x Pearson, B. Z., Fernandez, S. C., and Oller, D. K. (1993). Lexical development in bilingual infants and toddlers: comparison to monolingual norms. Lang. Learn. 43, 93–120. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-1770.1993.tb00174.x Philips, J. R. (1973). Syntax and vocabulary of mothers’ speech to young children: age and sex comparisons. Child Dev. 44, 182–185. doi: 10.2307/1127699 Frontiers in Psychology | www.frontiersin.org 16 April 2015 | Volume 6 | Article 514 Vaughan et al. Child-caregiver interaction in Indigenous Australia The History of Research on Australian Aboriginal languages, ed W. McGregor (Canberra, ACT: Pacific Linguistics), 485–507. Copyright © 2015 Vaughan, Wigglesworth, Loakes, Disbray and Moses. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms. Conflict of Interest Statement: The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest. Frontiers in Psychology | www.frontiersin.org 17 April 2015 | Volume 6 | Article 514