Skip to main content
Jill Vaughan
  • Monash University
    Clayton
    VIC 3800
Language naming systems are local ways of organising diversity, yet the language names used by linguists are sometimes incommensurable with the lived social reality of speakers. The process of assigning language names is not neutral,... more
Language naming systems are local ways of organising diversity, yet the language names used by linguists are sometimes incommensurable with the lived social reality of speakers. The process of assigning language names is not neutral, trivial or objective: it is a highly political process driven and shaped by understandings of group identity, similarity and difference. Closer attention to local perspectives on language naming offers important insights into ideologies around social and linguistic differentiation. This paper draws together accounts of diverse language naming practices from across Indigenous Australia and applies a close lens to the region of western Arnhem Land. Through an examination of three groups (speakers of Bininj Kunwok, Mawng, and Burarra), we describe the range of strategies speakers use to divide up their local language ecologies, practices for naming lects, and the role of variation in the processes of differentiation. Naming practices between these groups s...
Aboriginal Australia was, and is, fundamentally multilingual, and multilingualism is embedded in social, cultural, spiritual and economic life. Linguistic repertoires are typically highly complex, and deployed in diverse ways from... more
Aboriginal Australia was, and is, fundamentally multilingual, and multilingualism is embedded in social, cultural, spiritual and economic life. Linguistic repertoires are typically highly complex, and deployed in diverse ways from community to community. This chapter explores the shape and function of multilingual practices in Aboriginal Australia by considering the nature of individual language repertoires and community language ecologies, and by highlighting associated cultural phenomena.
The chapter traces the evolution of scholarly work on multilingualism in the region, and contrasts the contemporary context with a historical perspective on language practices in the region. Multilingualism in pre-colonial Australia is revealed to have been fundamentally implicated in territoriality, kinship, mobility and relationships between social groups. In contemporary Australia these connections persist, but the deployment of multilingual repertoires additionally reflects new communicative goals and is shaped by the emergence of widely spoken post-colonial contact varieties like Australian Kriol.
The wider sociolinguistic context of contemporary Indigenous Australia takes in a great diversity of local language ecologies, ranging from small remote communities where many traditional languages are spoken to large cities where... more
The wider sociolinguistic context of contemporary Indigenous Australia takes in a great diversity of local language ecologies, ranging from small remote communities where many traditional languages are spoken to large cities where Aboriginal Englishes are used within superdiverse contexts alongside other Englishes and languages. Prior to colonisation,
some 800 varieties – more than 250 distinct languages – were spoken across Australia. Since that time the region’s linguistic heritage has been decimated with fewer than 15 languages still learned by children, located mainly in the north and centre of Australia. Recent years have also seen, however, incredible resilience and creativity in the emergence of new varieties and in revitalisation efforts across the country.
The rapid and drastic social changes of the past two centuries have produced similarly seismic sociolinguistic shifts. Some sociolinguistic work has been possible within the small speaker communities of many Australian languages, but other approaches (e.g. variationist sociolinguistics) have only been possible where larger speech communities exist. As a result, the application of such methods has been somewhat limited. Nevertheless, recent years have seen sociolinguistic work in the Australian context increasingly engage with global methodological developments and ethical debates. Although social questions have long occupied a central location in linguistic work in the Australian context, the sociolinguistics of Australian Indigenous languages is now developing as a field in its own right, answering the call for more diverse data across many subfields of linguistics (e.g. Stanford 2016) with regular workshops and growing numbers of graduate researchers expanding the limits of the topic.
In this chapter, we provide an overview of key sociolinguistic research in Indigenous Australia, taking in both pre- and post-colonial contexts. Our discussion is structured around four interconnected strands which represent key gathering points of research endeavours: linguistic variation; multilingualism; language contact; and child language.
The names and borders of groups of people and their languages as conceived of by anthropologists and linguists are sometimes incommensurable with the lived social reality of the speakers themselves. The process of drawing such borders and... more
The names and borders of groups of people and their languages as conceived of by anthropologists and linguists are sometimes incommensurable with the lived social reality of the speakers themselves. The process of drawing such borders and assigning names is not neutral, trivial or objective: it is a highly political process driven and shaped by understandings of group identity, similarity and difference (Ionnàccaro & Dell’Aquila 2001; Léglise and Migge 2006). In the Australian context, the relationships between individuals, social groups and languages can be somewhat indirect. It is a common tendency (e.g. in northern Australia) for the individual to patrilineally inherit allegiance to a clan group, with each clan owning its own tract of land (e.g. Merlan 1981). Languages belong to the land rather than to clans directly, and so the relationship between individuals and ‘their’ languages is mediated. Languages are sometimes named after the clan associated with the land, or some aspect of that social group, but many other language naming strategies exist.
This paper builds an understanding of the diverse naming practices in Indigenous Australian communities through an examination of group and language naming in four regions: Cape York Peninsula in north-eastern Australia, the Western Desert region in western Australia, and among speakers of two language groups of coastal northern Australia (Yolŋu Matha varieties in eastern Arnhem Land and Burarra in north-central Arnhem Land). These descriptions draw from the literature and from the authors’ community-engaged fieldwork in Arnhem Land. We describe the range of strategies speakers use to divide up their local language ecologies, practices for naming lects, and the role of variation in the processes of differentiation and connection. Naming practices across these areas show some interesting similarities, but also striking differences.
In Maningrida, northern Australia, code-switching is a commonplace phenomenon within a complex of both longstanding and more recent language practices characterised by high levels of linguistic diversity and multilingualism.... more
In Maningrida, northern Australia, code-switching is a commonplace phenomenon within a complex of both longstanding and more recent language practices characterised by high levels of linguistic diversity and multilingualism. Code-switching is observable between local Indigenous languages and is now also widespread between local languages and English and/or Kriol. In this paper, I consider whether general predictions about the nature and functioning of code-switching account for practices in the Maningrida context. I consider: (i) what patterns characterise longstand- ing code-switching practices between different Australian languages in the region, as opposed to code-switching between an Australian language and Kriol or English? (ii) how do the distinctions observable align with general predictions and constraints from dominant theoretical frameworks? Need we look beyond these factors to explain the patterns? Results indicate that general predictions, including the effects of typological congruence, account for many observable tendencies in the data. However, other factors, such as constraints exerted by local ideologies of multilingualism and linguistic purism, as well as shifting socio-interactional goals, may help account for certain distinct patterns in the Maningrida data.
Language naming systems are local ways of organising diversity, yet the language names used by linguists are sometimes incommensurable with the lived social reality of speakers. The process of assigning language names is not neutral,... more
Language naming systems are local ways of organising diversity, yet the language names used by linguists are sometimes incommensurable with the lived social reality of speakers. The process of assigning language names is not neutral, trivial or objective: it is a highly political process driven and shaped by understandings of group identity, similarity and difference. Closer attention to local perspectives on language naming offers important insights into ideologies around social and linguistic differentiation.
            This paper draws together accounts of diverse language naming practices from across Indigenous Australia and applies a close lens to the region of western Arnhem Land. Through an examination of three groups (speakers of Bininj Kunwok, Mawng, and Burarra), we describe the range of strategies speakers use to divide up their local language ecologies, practices for naming lects, and the role of variation in the processes of differentiation. Naming practices between these groups show interesting similarities but also striking differences. We further highlight the interplay between two key processes which characterise local language naming strategies in the region: the ‘erasure’ of difference, typically from the perspective of a politically more powerful group, and the intentional creation of linguistic differentiation, or ausbau.
On Australia’s north-central coast, the regional hub of Maningrida is distinguished by a highly diverse language ecology, with local linguistic repertoires taking in elements of multiple traditional languages as well as localised... more
On Australia’s north-central coast, the regional hub of Maningrida is distinguished by a highly diverse language ecology, with local linguistic repertoires taking in elements of multiple traditional languages as well as localised Englishes, Kriol (an English-lexified creole), and a local alternate sign language system. Texts and talk in Maningrida are rarely monolingual. Youth culture in Maningrida is characterised by the interacting demands of traditional cultural life and an emergent urban subculture that increasingly connects to global phenomena. Young speakers are more, and differently, mobile compared to earlier generations, and their language practices are fundamentally implicated in community language change. While some Elders are concerned that young people are connecting less deeply to their languages’ traditional heartlands, young speakers themselves have different perspectives to offer.
This chapter explores the language practices of Maningrida youth and centralises local Indigenous perspectives on this phenomenon. Drawing on data collected through collaborative community language research, we consider the emergence of a restructured urban variety of the Burarra language, a process of ‘linguistic urbanisation’ in which youth are central. We further consider examples of the creative multilingual practices of young Maningrida speakers/signers, such as the recent success of an all-female band from the community who sing in five local languages. This chapter aims to illustrate how Maningrida youth create affordances within the diverse multilingual resources of their community, and the ways in which they draw on tradition and modernity to create new forms of meaning making.
Recent debates within the sociolinguistics of multilingualism have highlighted the challenges posed by new complexities in the semiotic space of ‘superdiversity’, where the linguistic repertoire cannot be neatly partitioned according to... more
Recent debates within the sociolinguistics of multilingualism have highlighted the challenges posed by new complexities in the semiotic space of ‘superdiversity’, where the linguistic repertoire cannot be neatly partitioned according to socially or politically constructed language boundaries. Some have noted, however, that discourses framing these complexities as ‘new’ tend to focus on the City and the West, and risk erasing diversities that have long been acknowledged as commonplace in other – for example, Indigenous – settings. This chapter explores core issues related to the ‘translinguistic turn’ within the context of Indigenous northern Australia. I highlight ‘the ordinariness of diversity’ in local translinguistic practice in this region, and demonstrate that the supposed ‘new complexities’ of globalisation have significant reflexes in the ‘peripheries’ of its reach.
In Arnhem Land, northern Australia, speakers of the Burarra language live and communicate within a highly multilingual and multilectal language ecology. This paper explores how regional ideologies of socio-cultural distinctiveness and... more
In Arnhem Land, northern Australia, speakers of the Burarra language live and communicate within a highly multilingual and multilectal language ecology. This paper explores how regional ideologies of socio-cultural distinctiveness and unity are projected into the linguistic space at the level of the language (within Maningrida's language ecology), as well as at the level of the lect (in terms of dialects and sociolects within the Burarra language). Drawing from current ethnography, naturalistic interactional and elicited language data, and other existing materials, the paper considers how speakers reproduce and evaluate language-internal variation within a linguistically diverse region. These processes are contextualised within the dynamics of long-term 'egalitarian' multilingualism which continue to shape contemporary practices and contemporary means of social meaning-making.
This chapter explores how speakers in Maningrida, a linguistically diverse Indigenous community in northern Australia, negotiate and evaluate their language practices within 'hybrid spaces' (i.e. spaces shaped by the interaction of... more
This chapter explores how speakers in Maningrida, a linguistically diverse Indigenous community in northern Australia, negotiate and evaluate their language practices within 'hybrid spaces' (i.e. spaces shaped by the interaction of diverse groups, institutions and ways of speaking). The analysis draws on data from two settings – a public school event, and a football match – and I consider the ways in which a translanguaging lens may provide insights into the interactional and socio-psychological realities of lived multilingualism in Maningrida. The major focus of the chapter pertains to Burarra/English mixing. I discuss the nature and functions of this language practice, and note that while speakers appear to 'soft assemble' their linguistic resources to fit the communicative situation at hand, there are also observable constraints exerted by the morphosyntax of the contributing codes. Issues of heteroglossia (e.g. in the use of vocabulary shared across language boundaries; in the treatment of adapted borrowings) are explored. These practices are further situated against the backdrop of long-standing multilingualism and language ideologies in the Arnhem Land region. The chapter evaluates translanguaging as a possible useful addition to the nomenclatural and analytical toolbox of researchers in the Australian Indigenous context, and as an important step towards decolonising understandings of local language practice, and further provides critiques and suggestions for strengthening the model's descriptive potential.
This chapter presents a case study of Yirrkala School, a remote Indigenous school in the Northern Territory of Australia that has succeeded in maintaining its long-standing bilingual programme in spite of great pressure to move towards a... more
This chapter presents a case study of Yirrkala School, a remote Indigenous school in the Northern Territory of Australia that has succeeded in maintaining its long-standing bilingual programme in spite of great pressure to move towards a mainstream, English-only model. Bilingual policy and education in the region is discussed, and the history and current realities of Yirrkala School itself are described. The chapter draws on a set of interviews conducted in 2015–16 with community members intimately involved with designing and implementing the local school curriculum, as well as on various community-based publications spanning several decades. These are used to articulate local perspectives on the role of language and culture in the classroom and the journey of two-way education in Yirrkala.
This chapter explores the ways a community-based bilingual school in the Tekoá Marangatu Indigenous community in Brazil supports and/or hinders the continuing development of the home language of Mbya Guarani children. It also highlights... more
This chapter explores the ways a community-based bilingual school in the Tekoá Marangatu Indigenous community in Brazil supports and/or hinders the continuing development of the home language of Mbya Guarani children. It also highlights how first- and third-grade Mbya Guarani children engage in translanguaging and look to their peers in making the sociolinguistic and sociocultural transition from home to the school context. The constant transformation of this reservation has been reshaping the social structures and activities the Guarani perform on a daily basis, yielding new forms of literacy. Even though Portuguese is the dominant language in the school context, both adults and children use Guarani as a way to navigate and resist the pressures of homogenization exerted by the outside world. Children are seen as social actors who transmit knowledge among themselves, the adults in their lives and the different contexts in which they live and experience bilingualism, biculturalism and biliteracy.
This book explores the experiences of Indigenous children and young adults around the world as they navigate the formal education system and wider society. Profiling a range of different communities and sociolinguistic contexts, this book... more
This book explores the experiences of Indigenous children and young adults around the world as they navigate the formal education system and wider society. Profiling a range of different communities and sociolinguistic contexts, this book examines the language ecologies of their local communities, schools and wider society and the approaches taken by these communities to maintain children’s home languages. The authors examine such complex themes as curriculum, translanguaging, contact languages and language use as cultural practice. In doing so, this edited collection acts as a first step towards developing solutions which address the complexity of the issues facing these children and young people. It will appeal to students and scholars of sociolinguistics, applied linguistics and community development, as well as language professionals including teachers, curriculum developers, language planners and educators.
Almost 2 million people in the North and South of Ireland identify as Irish speakers and an estimated 70 million around the globe can claim Irish heritage. While Irish ancestry may be distant for many, the Irish language is active in... more
Almost 2 million people in the North and South of Ireland identify as Irish speakers and an estimated 70 million around the globe can claim Irish heritage. While Irish ancestry may be distant for many, the Irish language is active in numerous locations in the diaspora, as documented in research profiling communities across the globe (e.g. Callahan, 1994; Garland 2008; Giles 2016; Kallen 1984, 1994; Noone, 2012a; Ó hEadhra, 1998;Ó Conchubhair 2008; Walsh & NíDhúda 2015 inter alia) and evidenced by the existence of many cultural and language groups. Census figures indicate that at least 25,000 people currently speak the language in Canada, the United States and Australia alone (Statistics Canada, 2013; United States Census Bureau, 2015; Australian Bureau of Statistics, 2012), yet very few in-depth general accounts of Irish-language use in the diaspora exist. Linguistic practices within Irish communities worldwide vary widely with regard to Irish-language use and language ideologies, with each community subject to distinct concerns, histories and discourses. As such, each has distinct possibilities for creating social and cultural meaning, possibilities that are fundamentally shaped by the socio-cultural and politico-historical contexts within which the Irish language has existed in the last 200 years. This paper investigates how the Irish language is recruited in constructions of cultural authenticity in three sites in the Irish diaspora: Boston, U.S.; Melbourne, Australia; and St. John’s, Newfoundland, Canada. Research is based on open-ended qualitative interviews with 41 learners and speakers regarding the Irish language and their own language practices, and in extensive participant observation of cultural and language-related activities in each site. Thematic content analysis of interview data provides the basis for ethnographic descriptions of each site. A Foucauldian understanding of discourse (e.g. Pennycook, 1994; Foucault, 1981, 1972) affords the identification and delineation of predominant discourses within which Irish-language use is implicated as a meaningful social act, and that are enacted or actively resisted within and across communities, as well as key subject positions made available within these discourses. This approach provides the basis for an exploration of (i) the processes of authenticating a cultural practice within discourse; (ii) how such processes shape the changing configurations of who is included and who is excluded within dominant politico-cultural discourses; and (iii) the various formations of community that exist within and across the diaspora space. The paper shows that the role of the Irish language in authenticating Irish cultural identity is subject to reworkings across time and space, as exemplified in the variety of local meanings it has taken on across the three diaspora sites featured.
This paper reports on a study in two remote multilingual Indigenous Australian communities: Yakanarra in the Kimberley region of Western Australia and Tennant Creek in the Barkly region of the Northern Territory. In both communities,... more
This paper reports on a study in two remote multilingual Indigenous Australian communities: Yakanarra in the Kimberley region of Western Australia and Tennant Creek in the Barkly region of the Northern Territory. In both communities, processes of language shift are underway from a traditional language (Walmajarri and Warumungu, respectively) to a local creole variety (Fitzroy Valley Kriol and Wumpurrarni English, respectively). The study focuses on language input from primary caregivers to a group of preschool children, and on the children's productive language. The study further highlights child-caregiver interactions as a site of importance in understanding the broader processes of language shift. We use longitudinal data from two time-points, approximately 2 years apart, to explore changes in adult input over time and developmental patterns in the children's speech. At both time points, the local creole varieties are the preferred codes of communication for the dyads in this study, although there is some use of the traditional language in both communities. Results show that for measures of turn length (MLT), there are notable differences between the two communities for both the focus children and their caregivers. In Tennant Creek, children and caregivers use longer turns at Time 2, while in Yakanarra the picture is more variable. The two communities also show differing trends in terms of conversational load (MLT ratio). For measures of morphosyntactic complexity (MLU), children and caregivers in Tennant Creek use more complex utterances at Time 2, while caregivers in Yakanarra show less complexity in their language at that time point. The study's findings contribute to providing a more detailed picture of the multilingual practices at Yakanarra and Tennant Creek, with implications for understanding broader processes of language shift. They also elucidate how children's language and linguistic input varies diachronically across time. As such, we contribute to understandings of normative language development for non-Western, non middle-class children in multilingual contexts.
Almost 2 million people in the North and South of Ireland identify as Irish speakers and an estimated 70 million around the globe can claim Irish heritage. While Irish ancestry may be distant for many, the Irish language is active in... more
Almost 2 million people in the North and South of Ireland identify as Irish speakers and an estimated 70 million around the globe can claim Irish heritage. While Irish ancestry may be distant for many, the Irish language is active in numerous diasporic communities, as documented in some limited research (e.g. Ihde 1994, Ó hEadhra 1998, Noone 2012a) and evidenced by the existence of cultural and language groups. Census figures indicate that over 30,000 people currently speak the language in Canada, the United States and Australia alone, yet no general account of Irish-language use in the diaspora exists. Linguistic practices within Irish communities worldwide vary widely with regard to Irish-language use, with each community subject to distinct concerns, histories and discourses (§4). As such each has different possibilities for creating social meaning through language use.

The aim of this thesis is: (i) to explore Irish-language learners’ and speakers’ characterisations of patterns of language use and language-community formations between sites in the Republic and Northern Ireland (chiefly Galway, Dublin, Derry and Belfast) and in the diaspora (Melbourne, Australia; Boston, U.S.; and St. John’s, Newfoundland, Canada) (§5-7); and (ii) to examine the (Foucauldian) discourses within which Irish-language use is implicated as a meaningful social practice within and across these communities (§8).

Research is based on open-ended qualitative interviews with 86 learners and speakers regarding the Irish language and their own language practices, and in extensive participant observation of cultural and language-related activities in each site (§3). Thematic content analysis of interview data provides the basis for ethnographic descriptions of each site. Foucauldian discourse analysis is used to discover and delineate the predominant discourses (and counter-discourses) within which Irish-language use is implicated as a meaningful social act, and that are enacted or actively resisted within and across communities, as well as key subject positions made available within these discourses.

The research predominantly targets learners and teachers of Irish, and those involved in language maintenance in each of these communities, and focuses particularly on elective bilinguals – speakers who have learnt Irish in the classroom and who do not use Irish as their primary language. The focus on this kind of bilingual speaker is of paramount importance for two reasons: firstly, because attitudinal research has been largely silent on elective Irish bilinguals, and secondly, because elective bilinguals are likely to be crucial to the language’s survival. Patterns of bilingualism in Gaeltacht regions are shifting and changing, and, as such, circumstantial bilinguals make up decreasing proportions of the language’s speaker community. Urban language communities (largely made up of L2 speakers), however, are increasing in size and activity.

This thesis argues for an incorporation of post-structuralist, social constructionist approaches to identity within sociology of language, particularly with regards to elective bilinguals and in diasporic contexts. As such, in addition to contributing to a broader description of Irish-language communities worldwide, this thesis demonstrates the contributions that a critical discourse analytic approach can make in endeavouring to understand the changing position of linguistic minorities in post-modernity.
Research on the subjunctive in Australian English has typically relied on corpus interrogation to ascertain frequency of occurrence of the target forms across text types (e.g., Collins and Peters 2004, Peters 2009). Such work has been... more
Research on the subjunctive in Australian English has typically relied on corpus interrogation to ascertain frequency of occurrence of the target forms across text types (e.g., Collins and Peters 2004, Peters 2009). Such work has been valuable in confirming trends in usage, such as the increasing use of the mandative subjunctive and the decline of the were subjunctive, but has yet to explore in detail motivations behind the continued survival of what had widely been described as a moribund variant.
Based on syntactic evidence, we distinguish the plain form subjunctive, which includes the traditional mandative subjunctive, from the were subjunctive. Using existing corpus-based results as well as data from additional corpus searches, we suggest that there are two primary factors in the subjunctive’s continued existence in Australian English; firstly, the presence of a growing set of ossifying subjunctive frames within which the subjunctive is marked, and secondly, the role of the subjunctive in indexing formal-prestigious-standard English style, with the were subjunctive indexing literariness and the plain form subjunctive indexing a stance of power and epistemic authority.
The Australian Computational and Linguistics Olympiad (OzCLO) started in 2008 in only two locations and has since grown to a nationwide competition with almost 1500 high school students participating in 2013. An Australian team has... more
The Australian Computational and Linguistics Olympiad (OzCLO) started in 2008 in only two locations and has since grown to a nationwide competition with almost 1500 high school students participating in 2013. An Australian team has participated in the International Linguistics Olympiad (IOL) every year since 2009. This paper describes how the competition is run (with a regional first round and a final national round) and the organisation of the competition (a National Steering Committee and Local Organising Committees for each region) and discusses the particular challenges faced by Australia (timing of the competition and distance between the major population centres). One major factor in the growth and success of OzCLO has been the introduction of the online competition, allowing participation of students from rural and remote country areas. The organisation relies on the goodwill and volunteer work of university and school staff but the strong interest amongst students and teachers shows that OzCLO is responding to a demand for linguistic challenges.
Research Interests:
LOLspeak is a complex and systematic reimagining of the English language. It is most often associated with thepopular, productive and long-lasting Internet meme ‘LOLcats’. This style of English is characterised by the simultaneous... more
LOLspeak is a complex and systematic reimagining of the English language. It is most often associated with thepopular, productive and long-lasting Internet meme ‘LOLcats’. This style of English is characterised by the simultaneous playfulmanipulation of multiple levels of language.Using community-generated web content as a corpus, we analysesome of the common language play strategies (Sherzer 2002)used in LOLspeak, which include morphological reanalysis,atypical sentence structure and lexical playfulness. The linguistic variety that emerges from these manipulations displayscollaboratively constructed norms and tendencies providing astandard which may be meaningfully adhered to or subverted by users. We conclude with a discussion of why people may choose toparticipate in such language play, and suggest that the languageplay strategies used by participants allow for the construction of complex identity
It is a truth universally acknowledged that the subjunctive in modern English is on its deathbed. It has been described variously as “a vestigial survival” (Strang 1968), “a rather feeble and restricted device” (Foster 1976:220), “a... more
It is a truth universally acknowledged that the subjunctive in modern English is on its deathbed. It has been described variously as “a vestigial survival” (Strang 1968), “a rather feeble and restricted device” (Foster 1976:220), “a literary trick” (Jespersen 1962: 294) and even “probably simply a conspiracy” (Coffey 2000). Most grammar books published before 1975 concur: the subjunctive will soon no longer be. And yet, as early as the 1950s linguists had begun to highlight some curious goings-on in American English. Barber (1964: 133) notes “a surprising reversion” during and after World War II to older subjunctive forms and by the 1970s many studies were being carried out into subjunctive usage in American and British English, most indicating that the subjunctive was not so moribund as previously thought (e.g. Johansson 1979, Haegeman 1986). Interesting divergences were found to exist in its use in the two varieties. It would not be until Peters’ work in 1998 that the status of the subjunctive in Australian English would be examined in detail. This study offered clear evidence of the subjunctive in this variety also, but revealed that Australian usage differs from that of both American and British English. This thesis explores the status of the subjunctive in all its manifestations in contemporary Australian English. The scrutinising of data retrieved from the Australian component of the International Corpus of English allowed for quantitative analyses of subjunctive usage as well as a detailed investigation into the forms this usage takes. The study is intended to be both diachronic and synchronic; corpus data is both compared with previous findings on the use of subjunctive in Australian English and collated with data from parallel corpora representing usage in other World Englishes (namely British, American and New Zealand English). By examining not only cases where the subjunctive is employed, but also cases where an alternative structure is attested, this study aims to elucidate the factors influential in speakers choosing the subjunctive.
Arnhem Land, northern Australia, is a noted ‘hotspot’ for linguistic diversity – the region is home to many languages representing several diverse language families. Although a significant genetic boundary bisects the region, the... more
Arnhem Land, northern Australia, is a noted ‘hotspot’ for linguistic diversity – the region is home to many languages representing several diverse language families. Although a significant genetic boundary bisects the region, the languages form a Sprachbund evidencing grammatical, phonological and lexical convergence. Individual language repertoires in Arnhem Land are large, typically drawing on between three to six local languages, as well as English, a range of contact varieties, and local auxiliary sign language.
Data from Arnhem Land has periodically been used to exemplify a range of supposed ‘exceptionalisms’ (Koch 2014) which have been argued to characterise Australian languages more generally, such as high levels of lexical diversity, intensive borrowing (as well as unique motivations for it) (cf. Bowern & Atkinson 2012; Evans 2005; Harvey 2011), and ‘extraordinary’ levels of variation (Garde 2008). This paper considers two seemingly incongruous claims about the operation of variation and change in the region. First, it has been demonstrated that extensive and long-term grammatical, phonological and lexical diffusion has occurred (Heath 1978, 1981). It is suggested that this has been scaffolded by particular multilingual practices in the region, an apparent absence of social pressures to maintain linguistic distinction, and cultural/demographic factors such as small language groups and extensive cross-linguistic marriage (Heath 1978: 142-3, 1981: 365). Second, fine-grained systems of variation have been documented in many languages of Arnhem Land. Variation may be dimensionalised along diverse lines, including indexing patriclan (Garde 2008), moiety (Green 2003, Morphy 1977) and geographical oppositions (e.g. coastal vs. inland (McKay 2000)). Such variation is thought to be created and maintained by strong ideologies which value linguistic diversity, and possibly via the conscious promotion of particular variants (Evans 2003: 30; Garde 2008). Considered together, then, it would appear that Arnhem Land simultaneously demonstrates both significant convergence as well as systematic diversification/differentiation (cf. Gumperz and Wilson 1971, and Rumsey in prep on similar processes in the northern Kimberley region).
In this paper I consider evidence from north-central Arnhem Land to investigate the operation and interaction of these competing processes. Drawing on analyses of new data from Burarra, as well as existing documentation of Gurr-goni and Djinang (e.g. Green & Nimbadja 2015, Waters 1989), I explore patterns of convergence and systematic variation in these neighbouring varieties. I argue that both processes are indeed able to coexist, and that they demonstrably do so in the language ecology of this region. Reworking Labov’s (e.g. 1972) framework for categorising variables (stereotypes|markers|indicators), I suggest that differing levels of ‘salience’ are key to understanding how these apparently contradictory processes interact within a larger system. Finally, this paper will highlight the necessity of looking beyond the boundaries of single-language systems to broader language ecologies in understanding the functioning of variation, and its role in language change.
Linguistic complexity and linguistic diversity are not evenly distributed around the globe. In fact, hotspots of linguistic complexity and linguistic diversity often coincide. For example, some areas of the Amazon basin, Papua New Guinea... more
Linguistic complexity and linguistic diversity are not evenly distributed around the globe. In fact, hotspots of linguistic complexity and linguistic diversity often coincide. For example, some areas of the Amazon basin, Papua New Guinea and Northern Australia have a very large number of language families, and these areas are also home to languages with very high levels of linguistic complexity. Languages in areas like Arnhem Land (Northern Australia) have been claimed to have many phenomena that are low in functionality and also difficult to learn—such as extensive nominal classification (Sands 1995).
Communities that favour complexity are sometimes idealised as ‘small, isolated communities’ (Trudgill 2011, Dahl 2004) but communities in Arnhem Land are anything but isolated. The small number of speakers per language is misleading—language itself does not delimit social groups. In fact, people live in multilingual communities and social interaction is intense and occurs across an extensive network. Marriages, material and ceremonial exchange, and informal fostering create strong social ties and facilitate frequent interaction between people with a broad range of linguistic affiliations. Many small languages, from four language families, are used within a speech community characterised by ‘small-scale multilingualism’ (Evans 2010; Lüpke 2016).
Small-scale multilingualism is a term used to describe communities in which people are highly multilingual, but where there are no lingua francas or polyglossia. That is, local languages cannot be assigned unique domains of use, nor can patterns of language use be explained by placing languages on some hierarchy of social status. Instead, people may use different languages with different people, across similar contexts. The interlocutors, their linguistic repertoires, and their socio-linguistic affiliations are the main determinants of language choice in interaction (Singer & Harris 2016).
Explanations for the growth of linguistic complexity either attribute it to isolation and low-contact or to contact involving child bilingualism (Dahl 2004). Nichols (1992) identifies areas with many language families as a context that favours complexification because “diversity among neighbouring languages fosters complexity in each language” (Nichols 1992:192). Nichols’ model predicts that high linguistic diversity should coincide with high linguistic complexity. This predicts the situation in Arnhem Land, but we still do not know the nature of language contact on the ground and how this fosters diversity and complexity.
Research in Arnhem Land so far has reported a range of diverse language practices, including, for example, both code-switching and linguistic purism in different contexts (Vaughan and Carew 2016), receptive multilingualism (Singer and Harris 2016, Rehbein et al. 2012) and ongoing learning of new languages throughout the life span. This flexibility seems to be balanced by allegiance to the patrilect (father’s language), creating what Stanford (2009) calls ‘communities of descent’. In certain contexts, such as formal speeches or child- language socialisation, the affiliation with the patrilect comes to the fore, a bulwark against the multitudinous options for multilingual practices observed in other contexts.
This talk will present analyses of new data collected on language use and language ideologies in Arnhem Land, northern Australia. This includes corpus data from recordings of multilingual interactions as well as ethnographic data from interviews and participant observation. Drawing on these analyses, we propose a model of small-scale multilingualism in Arnhem Land that demonstrates how language practices and ideologies in this area support complexification and diversification. Juxtaposing the study of multilingual practices with research on language ideologies enables greater insights into the nature of language contact
as well as the social conditions that constrain these interactions, and prevent any one language spreading at the expense of the others (cf. Epps forthcoming).
In Maningrida, northern Australia, codeswitching is a commonplace phenomenon within a complex of both longstanding and more recent multilingual practices. Fourteen Indigenous languages representing three language families are spoken among... more
In Maningrida, northern Australia, codeswitching is a commonplace phenomenon within a complex of both longstanding and more recent multilingual practices. Fourteen Indigenous languages representing three language families are spoken among 2500 people, alongside increasing use of English and contact varieties (such as Kriol, spoken further south in the Maningrida region, and right across northern Australia). Individual linguistic repertoires are typically large, but strong ideologies exist dictating rights/responsibilities around language ownership and use. A variety of code-mixing practices is observable between local Indigenous languages, and is now also widespread between local languages and English. Codeswitching has been a feature of the longstanding stable ‘egalitarian’ multilingual ecology of the region (Singer & Harris 2016), yet the practice is also symptomatic of a changing local language ecology, shaped by the large-scale incursion of English and implicated in the emergence of a local urban koine.
In this paper I consider whether general predictions about the nature and functioning of codeswitching account for practices in the Maningrida context, and for patterns attested across the diverse multilingual contexts of northern Australia more generally. I consider: (i) what patterns characterise (intraclausal) codeswitching practices between different Australian languages in the region, as opposed to codeswitching between an Australian language and Kriol or English? What (if any) generalisations can be made?; and (ii) are the distinctions observable accounted for by general predictions and constraints from dominant theoretical frameworks, or by the typological congruence of the languages implicated? Need we look beyond these factors to explain the patterns?
First I survey historical and contemporary descriptions of multilingual practices and codeswitching in the region. Little published work addresses codeswitching between Indigenous languages only, so I draw together available accounts from northern Australia (e.g. Coleman n.d.; Evans 2010; Haviland 1982; O’Keeffe 2016; Sutton 1978) to identify any tendencies, constraints and particularities. I consider to what extent observable patterns may be accounted for by established general principles, and whether regional language ideologies may also shape these outcomes of long-term contact. These data are compared with data from work on contemporary codeswitching between Indigenous languages, Kriol and/or English (e.g. Meakins 2012; McConvell 2002; O’Shannessy 2012) and I explore possible reasons for divergences in these practices. To this discussion I contribute data from multilingual interactions in Maningrida, where codeswitching is evident both between local languages and also with English. Results indicate that general predictions, along with the effects of typological congruence, account for many observable tendencies in the data (e.g. switch-sites). However other factors, such as constraints exerted by local ideologies of multilingualism and linguistic purism, as well as shifting socio-interactional goals, may help account for certain distinct patterns in the Maningrida data at least (e.g. lower incidence of certain types of codeswitching between Indigenous languages, despite higher typological congruence and shared multilingual repertoires). Data from this context present an opportunity to test theories and to explore the impact of social-psychological pressures of divergence and convergence in cultural and linguistic practice.
In Maningrida (north-central Arnhem Land), fourteen languages representing three language families are spoken alongside English and Kriol. This linguistic diversity reflects long-standing practices of multilingualism and diversification... more
In Maningrida (north-central Arnhem Land), fourteen languages representing three language families are spoken alongside English and Kriol. This linguistic diversity reflects long-standing practices of multilingualism and diversification akin to ‘small-scale’ or ‘egalitarian’ multilingualism, and now accommodates more recent linguistic arrivals, the impact of newer patterns of mobility, and the reification of certain local languages in community institutions. Using a corpus of recent naturalistic language use and linguistic biography interviews, this paper considers how local valorisations of multilingualism and diversity contrast with the bilingualism-as-deficit discourses common in certain community and governmental institutions.
In Maningrida, north central Arnhem Land, fourteen Indigenous languages are spoken, alongside English and a range of contact varieties, in a community of 2500 people. Interaction is characterised by high levels of individual... more
In Maningrida, north central Arnhem Land, fourteen Indigenous languages are spoken, alongside English and a range of contact varieties, in a community of 2500 people. Interaction is characterised by high levels of individual multilingualism, as well as receptive multilingualism and code-mixing practices (Carew 2016; Elwell 1982; Singer & Harris 2016). Local language ideologies dictate that variation is dimensionalised according to patrilineal land-holding units, into what might roughly be called ‘dialects’, with linguistic boundaries “couched principally in the idiom of local geography” (Sutton 1991).
Work on dialects in contact has largely been concerned with monolingual or migrant communities in western contexts, where it has been (arguably) feasible to approach dialects as varieties of some superordinate language, and to consider linguistic accommodation to be the primary/only driver in shaping new varieties (Labov 2001, Trudgill 2008). While several important exceptions exist (e.g. Gumperz & Wilson 1971, Stanford 2007), many questions remain regarding variation in multilingual, non-western or Indigenous contexts. In this talk, I consider the case of variation and dialect contact within Burarra, the language with the largest speaker community in Maningrida. Burarra is commonly described by speakers as consisting of four major ‘dialects’: An-barra, Martay, Maringa (not always distinguished as a distinct dialect) and Gun-nartpa (often considered a distinct language), with each label referencing groups of clan estates further east in Arnhem Land. Variation within Burarra is chiefly lexical, with further more minor phonological and syntactic differences also evident. Against this backdrop, a newer variety of Burarra has emerged in the urban context of Maningrida, a product of dialect levelling, partially stabilised code-switching with English, and the privileging of shared cross-linguistic lexical stock in the region. While this newer variety is now the first language for some, the traditional ‘dialects’ still co-exist with it and are central identity categories, reproduced through “communities of descent” (Stanford 2009).
Using recent interactional and elicited language data, as well as interviews and more general ethnographic work in the community, I discuss the extent to which the extant and socially salient variation within the Burarra linguistic space can be said to be reflexes of the various attested Burarra cultural categories, and the means by which these broader categories are performed interactionally. These patterns of variation are contextualised within the ‘total semiotic fact’ (Nakassis 2016, following Silverstein 1985) of cultural and social identity practice in the Maningrida region. Ultimately I will argue that the nature and functioning of variation in the region issues a challenge to assumptions of indexicality and lectal coherence (Guy & Hiskens 2016) in the dialectology literature, and point to the need to problematise the role of accommodation in language contact more generally.
In Maningrida, northern Australia, codeswitching is a commonplace phenomenon within a complex of both longstanding and more recent multilingual practices. Fourteen Indigenous languages representing three language families are spoken among... more
In Maningrida, northern Australia, codeswitching is a commonplace phenomenon within a complex of both longstanding and more recent multilingual practices. Fourteen Indigenous languages representing three language families are spoken among 2500 people, alongside increasing use of English and contact varieties (such as Kriol, spoken further south in the Maningrida region, and right across northern Australia). Individual linguistic repertoires are typically large, but strong ideologies exist dictating rights/responsibilities around language ownership and use. A variety of code-mixing practices is observable between local Indigenous languages, and is now also widespread between local languages and English. Codeswitching has been a feature of the longstanding stable ‘egalitarian’ multilingual ecology of the region (Singer & Harris 2016), yet the practice is also symptomatic of a changing local language ecology, shaped by the large-scale incursion of English and implicated in the emergence of a local urban koine.
In this paper I consider whether general predictions about the nature and functioning of codeswitching account for practices in the Maningrida context, and for patterns attested across the diverse multilingual contexts of northern Australia more generally. I consider: (i) what patterns characterise (intraclausal) codeswitching practices between different Australian languages in the region, as opposed to codeswitching between an Australian language and Kriol or English? What (if any) generalisations can be made?; and (ii) are the distinctions observable accounted for by general predictions and constraints from dominant theoretical frameworks, or by the typological congruence of the languages implicated? Need we look beyond these factors to explain the patterns?
First I survey historical and contemporary descriptions of multilingual practices and codeswitching in the region. Little published work addresses codeswitching between Indigenous languages only, so I draw together available accounts from northern Australia (e.g. Coleman n.d.; Evans 2010; Haviland 1982; O’Keeffe 2016; Sutton 1978) to identify any tendencies, constraints and particularities. I consider to what extent observable patterns may be accounted for by established general principles, and whether regional language ideologies may also shape these outcomes of long-term contact. These data are compared with data from work on contemporary codeswitching between Indigenous languages, Kriol and/or English (e.g. Meakins 2012; McConvell 2002; O’Shannessy 2012) and I explore possible reasons for divergences in these practices. To this discussion I contribute data from multilingual interactions in Maningrida, where codeswitching is evident both between local languages and also with English. Results indicate that general predictions, along with the effects of typological congruence, account for many observable tendencies in the data (e.g. switch-sites). However other factors, such as constraints exerted by local ideologies of multilingualism and linguistic purism, as well as shifting socio-interactional goals, may help account for certain distinct patterns in the Maningrida data at least (e.g. lower incidence of certain types of codeswitching between Indigenous languages, despite higher typological congruence and shared multilingual repertoires). Data from this context present an opportunity to test theories and to explore the impact of social-psychological pressures of divergence and convergence in cultural and linguistic practice.
Work on dialects in contact has largely been concerned with monolingual or migrant communities in western contexts, where it has been (arguably) feasible to approach dialects as varieties of some superordinate language, and to consider... more
Work on dialects in contact has largely been concerned with monolingual or migrant communities in western contexts, where it has been (arguably) feasible to approach dialects as varieties of some superordinate language, and to consider linguistic accommodation to be the primary/only driver in shaping new varieties (Labov 2001, Trudgill 2008). While several important exceptions exist (e.g. Gumperz & Wilson 1971, Stanford 2007), many questions remain regarding variation in multilingual, non-western or Indigenous contexts. I consider a case of dialect contact in Maningrida, Arnhem Land, where approximately 14 languages are spoken in a community of 2500 people. Interaction is characterised by high levels of individual multilingualism and code-mixing practices. Variation is traditionally dimensionalised according to land-holding units, into what might roughly be called ‘dialects’, with linguistic boundaries “couched principally in the idiom of local geography” (Sutton 1991). Within this highly multilingual context, three dialects of the Burarra language have contributed to an ‘urban’ koine. While this newer variety is now the first language for some, the traditional ‘dialects’ still co-exist with it and are central identity categories, reproduced through “communities of descent” (Stanford 2009). Using interactional and elicited language data, as well as interviews and more general ethnographic work in the community, I describe the nature and interpretation of current dialectal variation within the Burarra linguistic space. I further consider the continuity of practices and ideologies driving diversification and the maintenance of difference in this context, and demonstrate the need to problematise the role of accommodation in language contact more generally.
In Maningrida, a remote, largely Indigenous, community in northern Australia, code-switching is a commonplace phenomenon that sits within a complex of both longstanding and more recent multilingual practices. Approximately fourteen... more
In Maningrida, a remote, largely Indigenous, community in northern Australia, code-switching is a commonplace phenomenon that sits within a complex of both longstanding and more recent multilingual practices. Approximately fourteen languages are spoken among Maningrida's 2500 inhabitants, and interaction is characterised by high levels of individual multilingualism and various code-mixing practices. While a unified communilect does not exist, convergences are observable in terms of shared community language features. The task of describing the nature and functions of code-switching in such a context is complicated by a number of factors: code-switching has been described as a feature of stable 'egalitarian' multilingualism in Australia, and as such may constitute a reflex of the maintenance of linguistic boundaries, shaped by broader ideologies privileging linguistic diversity and distinctiveness. However, it can also be a practice symptomatic of a changing local language ecology, a precursor to language shift or the emergence of a contact variety. Further complicating factors in Maningrida include the presence of shared lexical items which can blur the lines between borrowing and code-switching, and the nature of dialectal variation in the region, which may rely partly on the indexical values of 'shibboleth' forms. This paper will focus in particular on Burarra-English code-switching in Maningrida, which is implicated, along with dialect-levelling processes and shared cross-linguistic stock, in the formation of an urban koine variety of Burarra. We draw on long-term linguistic and ethnographic fieldwork in the community to describe the shape and functions of this form of partially-conventionalised code-switching, as well as constraints (both grammatical and social) on its use. Burarra-English code-switching within this context provides an opportunity to explore competing social-psychological pressures of divergence and convergence in cultural and linguistic practice. As such, we demonstrate what this particular site of linguistic practice can reveal about the complexities of code-switching on the ground.
In remote northeast Arnhem Land, children grow up in a highly multilingual and multidialectal environment. Yirrkala is a community of approximately 700 people where most speak a form of Yolŋu Matha, a pan-dialectal cover term... more
In remote northeast Arnhem Land, children grow up in a highly multilingual and multidialectal environment. Yirrkala is a community of approximately 700 people where most speak a form of Yolŋu Matha, a pan-dialectal cover term encapsulating approximately 30 Indigenous language varieties. Clan groups each have their own varieties (as well as their own homelands, totems, ceremonies, songs etc.) which, while in linguistic terms might be considered dialects of Yolŋu Matha, are typically understood by speakers to be distinct languages (Wilkinson 1991: 5). These clanlects are mutually intelligible, distinguished largely by grammatical morphology and some minor, though highly salient, lexical differences (Amery 1993), and shaped by a kind of long-term ausbau process (Garde 2008). Speakers of at least 18 of these clanlects currently reside at Yirrkala.
Traditionally, children learn their mother’s and grandmother’s varieties at a young age. As they become young adults, they begin learning their father’s language, their own clan variety. The learning of these clanlects is a crucial step in learning about rom (ceremonial law/customs) and culture, and becoming strong in Yolŋu identity. Children learn to identify an interlocutor’s clan affiliation by their speech, which helps them determine their relationship with others.
This paper explores how children and young adults understand and deploy dialectal variation within the Yolŋu Matha linguistic space, and examines how linguistic coherence is created among multiple variable phenomena (Guy and Hinskens 2016). We consider how these practices are impacted by the current language ecology of Yirrkala where a further, newer Yolŋu Matha variety (not a clanlect), the koiné language Dhuwaya, is now spoken by all community members, regardless of clan affiliations. Relying on linguistic and ethnographic fieldwork, as well as targeted interviews in the community, we attempt to elucidate the practices and ideologies that shape the role and nature of variation in this context.
In this paper we report on the design and implementation of an interview-based study into caregiver attitudes and beliefs about child-language acquisition in two remote Aboriginal Australian communities. Language socialisation plays a... more
In this paper we report on the design and implementation of an interview-based study into caregiver attitudes and beliefs about child-language acquisition in two remote Aboriginal Australian communities. Language socialisation plays a crucial role in children's cognitive, social and cultural development, and the child-caregiver relationship is a fundamental site in this process. As in any study of language socialisation, a key focus is to gain insight into the underlying values and beliefs that govern social interaction within a given community. Identifying these can be difficult for cultural-insiders because they must 'denaturalise' normal behaviour, as well as for cultural outsiders who must do away with their own preconceived, deep-seated assumptions about child development and caregiver/child interactions. Adopting an approach that avoids these twin pitfalls, then, can be challenging. This paper discusses the development of an appropriate methodology for undertaking a language socialisation study with sensitivity to cultural and individual differences in two communities in Aboriginal Australia. We focus in particular on interviews targeting parents' and caregivers' attitudes and beliefs about child-language acquisition and development; both shared community ideologies, and individuals' beliefs and values guiding language socialisation routines. The two communities included in our study represent very different language ecologies, but together exemplify the sociolinguistic variation that typifies Aboriginal Australia. The first is Barunga, a small community in Central Northern Australia where Kriol is the predominant language spoken. The second is Maningrida, a community of around 2000 people in Arnhem Land where around fourteen distinct languages are spoken, with most individuals able to speak between two and six (Elwell 1982; Handelsmann 1996). Both communities featured have recently undergone/are currently undergoing distinct configurations of language loss, and they differ significantly in the levels of multilingualism observable. We argue that the method should include open-ended questions, designed to elicit narrative-like descriptions, as well as questions and prompts to direct the interview along specific themes. To identify the most meaningful themes we considered features found in language socialisation literature from various cultural backgrounds (e.g. Ochs and Sheiffelin 1984; Kulick and Schieffelin 2004), themes arising from literature on child development in Aboriginal Australia (e.g. Hamilton 1981; Philpott 2003; Byers et al 2012; Kruske et al 2012) and, crucially, we relied on the outcomes of local community collaboration in designing the study. The resulting interview structure ranges across four main themes: (i) the 'development of learning/milestones'; (ii) multi-party vs. dyadic input and competence; (iii) accommodation to the infant, (verbal and environmental); and (iv) autonomy of the infant. As well as discussing the development of our interview methodology, we present initial findings from the interviews themselves. In particular, we discuss the implications of the findings for the ongoing maintenance of creoles and endangered languages as home languages. We explore insights into local family language policies HOLM 2016 Abstracts – Oral Presentations
Language names and borders as conceived of by linguists are sometimes incommensurable with the social reality of language groupings lived by speakers. The process of drawing such borders and assigning names is not neutral or trivial: it... more
Language names and borders as conceived of by linguists are sometimes incommensurable with the social reality of language groupings lived by speakers. The process of drawing such borders and assigning names is not neutral or trivial: it is driven and shaped by understandings of group identity, similarity and difference (Ionnaccaro & Dell'Aquila 2001, Leglise and Migge 2006). Typically a language gains wider public acknowledgement through 'artefactualisation' as a dictionary or grammar, referred to as " the 'birth certificates' of a language " by Blommaert (2008). However, the linguist's conception of a discrete language, essentially a 'doculect' (Cysouw & Good 2013), may not reflect the ways in which speakers divide up their own local language ecology. Australia is no exception, with linguists working on Australian languages usually classifying and naming language and codes in a community (or relying on someone else's classification), and then getting on with whatever else it is that they want to research. It is easy to forget, or fail to realise, however that the people who identify as speakers of Australian Indigenous languages often name and classify codes in very different ways to linguists. Understanding local perspectives on language naming offers insight into ideologies around social and linguistic differentiation. In this talk we will attempt to build an understanding of the diverse naming practices in Arnhem Land through an examination of language naming practices in three groups: Mawng speakers at Warruwi Community, speakers of Burarra and Ndjébbana at Maningrida and speakers of Bininj Kunwok in all its varieties. The data used is drawn from corpora of conversational and elicited data, open-ended interviews and participant observation. We describe the range of strategies speakers use to divide up their local language ecologies, practices for naming lects, and the role of variation in the processes of differentiation and connection. Naming practices between these groups show some interesting similarities but also striking differences. The term Bininj Kunwok, for example, was invented by linguists as a cover term for six related dialects: Kunwinjku, Kuninjku, Kundjeyhmi, Kundedjnjenghmi, Kune and Mayali. These dialect names are also often used by speakers of the languages but not consistently so across social groupings. For example, speakers of the largest dialect, Kunwinjku, who are also the most powerful politically, do not always recognise six distinct dialects but sometimes only recognise the nearest dialect to theirs as different (Kundjeyhmi) but include all other variation within their dialect, Kunwinjku. This perspective contrasts with the views of speakers of other dialects who recognise 5-7 distinct varieties. This erasure of difference, from the perspective of a politically more powerful group is something that Irvine and Gal discuss as a " process in which ideology, in simplifying the sociolinguistic field renders some persons or activities (or sociolinguistic phenomena) invisible " (2000:38). Erasure is just one of a number of processes which linguistic anthropologists have identified as the means by which nation states use language to consolidate and naturalise their position of power, and sociolinguistic distinctiveness from other nations. Garde (2008) discusses how the process of ausbau 'intentional linguistic differentiation' is used to shore up political differences between clans who speak varieties of Bininj Kunwok. These processes of erasure and ausbau, often assumed to be associated only with nation-states, are also relevant to the linguistic anthropology of Arnhem Land. This talk will consider the extent to which such processes can also account for the naming practices observable among Mawng speakers at Warruwi, and Burarra and Ndjébbana speakers at Maningrida. Our findings emphasise the value of bringing front-and-centre speakers' conceptions of differentiation and divisions of the language ecology, and the importance of attending to the views of all relevant social actors in grasping locally salient sociolinguistic processes.
In this paper we report on the design and implementation of an interview-based study into caregiver attitudes and beliefs about child language acquisition in two remote Aboriginal Australian communities. Language socialisation plays a... more
In this paper we report on the design and implementation of an interview-based study into caregiver attitudes and beliefs about child language acquisition in two remote Aboriginal Australian communities.
Language socialisation plays a crucial role in children’s cognitive, social and cultural development, and the child-caregiver relationship is a fundamental site in this process. As in any study of language socialisation, a key focus is to gain insight into the underlying values and beliefs that govern social interaction within a given community (Ochs & Sheiffelin 1984).
The two Arnhemland communities included in our study
The first is Barunga, a small community where a variety of Kriol is the predominant language spoken. The second is Maningrida, a community of around 2000 people where fourteen distinct languages are spoken, with most individuals able to speak between two and six (Elwell 1982; Handelsmann 1996).
The interview structure ranges across four main themes: (i) the ‘development of learning/milestones’; (ii) multi-party vs. dyadic input and competence; (iii) accommodation to the infant, verbal and environmental; and (iv) autonomy of the infant. These themes were identified from Language Socialisation literature (e.g. Kulick and Schieffelin 2004) and literature on child development in Aboriginal Australia (e.g. Philpott 2003; Hamilton 1981; Kruske et al 2012; Byers et al 2012; Lowell et al 1996).
Thus far the interviews have indicated that the stages of childhood development can be characterized in locally salient ways, for example beibiwan ‘baby’; biganini ‘small child’ and bigwanbigwan ‘grown/adult young person’ at Barunga. These terms are defined in terms of physical development as well as the child’s capabilities, such as clear speech, being able to care for ones self, as well as specific cultural milestones such as Initiation Ceremonies. We found that interview questions worked best when specific children from target age range were referred to elicit developmental stage e.g. “Would you use baby talk to ‘children like John?’ (children aged 2-3)”. We also found that questions that contained inherent comparison or judgments of other people were not appropriate, for example the following two questions were deleted after workshopping with co-researchers in the community:
1. What kinds of things do mums and dads have to think about that people without kids don’t need to think about?
represent very different
language ecologies, but together exemplify the sociolinguistic variation that typifies Aboriginal
Australia.
  220
2. Are some kids better talkers than others? What are they like?
We also found that some of the most successful questions were those that elicited spontaneous hypothetical (or real) narratives e.g. “When you see a baby (1-2 yrs old) John’s age do something dangerous what would you say to them? Can you give an example story?”
As well as discussing the development of our interview methodology, we present findings from the interviews themselves and contextualise this study as a starting point for an ongoing broader language socialisation study in both these communities.
Multilingual practices, such as code-switching/mixing and receptive multilingualism, enable speakers to draw upon the range of codes within their linguistic repertoire to construct linguistic and social meaning in interaction. This is... more
Multilingual practices, such as code-switching/mixing and receptive multilingualism, enable speakers to draw upon the range of codes within their linguistic repertoire to construct linguistic and social meaning in interaction. This is particularly the case in Aboriginal Australia, one of the most multilingual regions in the world, where traditional languages continue to be spoken alongside newer contact languages (e.g. Kriol and mixed languages), Aboriginal English and Standard Australian English (see, e.g., Koch & Nordlinger 2014). As such, speakers in these settings have many codes to choose from in their linguistic meaning-making.
This paper investigates the social and pragmatic functions of the linguistic codes of Maningrida, Arnhem Land, a predominantly Aboriginal region of Australia’s Northern Territory. Maningrida has extremely high levels of multilingualism due to its role as a linguistic and socio-political nexus of many distinct groups from further afield in Arnhem Land. Indeed, Maningrida is in some ways a microcosm of traditional Arnhem Land affiliations and hostilities, many of which are still apparent in ongoing interactions and attitudes. At last report (in 1996), fourteen distinct languages are spoken there, with most people able to speak between two and six (Elwell 1982; Handelsmann 1996).
In focusing particularly on the practices of code-switching and mixing, we investigate which languages are currently being used in Maningrida, how distinct codes are drawn upon in interaction, and the particular social meanings they may have for speakers. In particular, we consider a small set of multilingual texts collected from both private and public domains in the community in order to elucidate indexical properties and grammatical constraints (where relevant) of code-switching/mixing as practiced in Maningrida. Close examination of the choice and strategic use of codes in interaction is combined with data from qualitative interviews with speakers (regarding language ideologies and practices), as well as broader ethnographic work in the community, to elucidate the indexicalities of the codes in question (e.g. Bucholtz & Hall 2008). This provides a targeted means of investigating how social meanings are constructed more generally.
These findings are considered within the context of existing work on code-switching/mixing both in Aboriginal Australia (e.g. McConvell 2008; Meakins 2012) and beyond. This study further contributes to understanding the enduring role of ‘traditional’ linguistic practices that contribute to ongoing stable multilingualism in Aboriginal Australia, as well as the inextricable link between language, land and identity in this region, wherein sociolinguistic variation is dimensionalised into the geographical and social cosmos (Evans 2013). These extant yet traditional practices now interact with newer cultural and linguistic pressures, such as the increasing use of Standard Australian English, in a variety of ways, and within this context code-switching/mixing can be a symptom of language in a state of change but can also be characteristic of an enduring multilingual language ecology.
Language socialisation plays a fundamental role in children’s cognitive, social and cultural development. Learning language, and being able to use it to manage the world, is critical to the healthy cognitive and linguistic development of... more
Language socialisation plays a fundamental role in children’s cognitive, social and cultural development. Learning language, and being able to use it to manage the world, is critical to the healthy cognitive and linguistic development of all children. Two of the most important sites of language socialisation are the home and the school classroom. In Aboriginal Australia, children typically enter school with a wide range of linguistic codes and, for the most part, are expected to adjust to the Standard Australian English (SAE)–dominant environment of the classroom (Lowell & Devlin 1998, Rhydwen 1992). A small body of research has addressed the kinds of issues Aboriginal children subsequently face in classroom participation, in part resulting from this transition, such as the discussion around whether children are equipped to deal with the nature of questions typical of school interactions (see, e.g., Christie 1985, Malcolm 1982, Moses & Yallop 2008).
This paper presents insights from research in four Aboriginal communities: Murray Downs and Ltyentye Apurte in Central Australia, Wurrumiyanga on the Tiwi Islands, and Yirrkala in north-east Arnhem Land. These communities represent diverse language ecologies, and together exemplify the sociolinguistic variation that typifies Aboriginal Australia. We first consider the linguistic codes that children in each of these communities use at home, and how these pattern onto expected code-use in the classroom. These repertoires vary significantly across the sites due to: (i) the different traditional language spoken in each (Alyawarr, Arrernte, Modern Tiwi and Yolngu Matha respectively); (ii) the differing role and nature of Kriol in each (i.e. whether it is used at all, whether varieties are acrolectal/basilectal); and (iii) the linguistic environment of the school (the differing role of SAE, e.g. Yirrkala School has a bilingual program).
We further discuss the effect of children’s linguistic repertoires on their ability to participate effectively in the classroom, and the key issues faced by children in each community during and beyond this transition. In the early years of schooling, children worldwide acquire more developed linguistic skills (such as advanced use of pronouns, determiners and demonstratives and more complex syntactic structures) as well as increased pragmatic and metalinguistic competence. This process is complicated when there is the additional challenge of acquiring a new code. The linguistic situation faced by children in much of Aboriginal Australia is distinct, however, from what has typically been considered in the literature on early education and L2 acquisition (e.g. the experiences of immigrant children learning SAE). In contrast, the creoles and mixed languages many Aboriginal children speak at home have some features in common with SAE, and children usually have prior exposure to the standard code (Heath 1983, Siegel 1992). As such, the experiences of Aboriginal children in this regard constitute an under-researched area that this study will contribute to illuminating.
This work builds on findings from the first phase of the Aboriginal Child Language Acquisition project (ACLA1, see Simpson & Wigglesworth 2008), which detailed the complex linguistic environment children are raised in by investigating children’s language input in three Aboriginal communities (Yakanarra in the Kimberley, Dagaragu in the Victoria River District, and Tennant Creek). In combination, these two groups of studies provide a much clearer picture of how children are exposed to and learn to manage their linguistic repertoires across a variety of contexts in Aboriginal Australia, and the ways in which the first language (acrolectal vs. basilectal creoles, mixed languages, traditional languages) impacts language behaviour and integration in the school environment.
This paper reports on a study in a remote multilingual Indigenous Australian community – Yakanarra in the Kimberley region of Western Australia – which is undergoing language shift from the traditional language, Walmajarri, to Fitzroy... more
This paper reports on a study in a remote multilingual Indigenous Australian community – Yakanarra in the Kimberley region of Western Australia – which is undergoing language shift from the traditional language, Walmajarri, to Fitzroy Valley Kriol (also known as Kimberley Kriol). The study focuses on language input by primary caregivers to a group of four preschool children, and also focuses on the children’s productive language. We use longitudinal data from two time-points, approximately two years apart, to describe changes in adult input over time and developmental patterns in the children’s speech, as well as to contribute to an understanding of broader patterns of community language use in Yakanarra.

At both time points, Fitzroy Valley Kriol is the preferred language of communication for the dyads in this study. Results show that for measures of morphosyntactic complexity (MLU), children and caregivers used more complex utterances at Time 2 than at Time 1, as was expected, but that at Time 2 the children’s utterances were mostly more complex than their caregiver’s. Regarding measures of utterance length (MLT), on average children’s words per turn increased a small amount between Times 1 and 2, although there was variability between participants, while caregivers’ average MLT was in fact lower at Time 2 than Time 1. When overall conversational load was considered (MLT ratio), the children’s contributions were found to constitute a relatively large share of the interactions, with one child even having a greater conversational burden than her caregiver. We might infer from this that the Yakanarra caregivers, who use shorter and fewer turns at Time 2, respond to their children’s language development by making room for their productive language capacity.

This study will contribute to a fuller understanding of child-caregiver interactions in remote Indigenous communities. It will also elucidate the contribution of intergenerational transfer to language maintenance in a community where the language practices of the youthful population are a core contributing factor to the rapidity of language shift from Walmajarri to Fitzroy Valley Kriol.
This paper addresses the ways in which the Irish language is recruited as a means of resistance in communities in Ireland and the Irish diaspora: resistance to enforced or presumed identities, to pressures of cultural assimilation, and to... more
This paper addresses the ways in which the Irish language is recruited as a means of resistance in communities in Ireland and the Irish diaspora: resistance to enforced or presumed identities, to pressures of cultural assimilation, and to prevailing and hegemonic discourses.
Almost 2 million people in the North and South of Ireland identify as Irish speakers and an estimated 70 million around the globe can claim Irish heritage. While Irish ancestry may be distant for many, the Irish language is active in numerous communities, as documented in some limited research (e.g. Ihde 1994, Ó hEadhra 1998, Noone 2012) and evidenced by the existence of cultural and language groups. Census figures indicate that over 30,000 people currently speak the language in Canada, the United States and Australia alone, yet no general account of Irish-language use in the diaspora exists. Linguistic practices within Irish communities worldwide vary widely with regard to Irish-language use, with each community subject to distinct concerns, histories and discourses, and hence with different possibilities for creating social meaning through language use. This paper is therefore located within a broader research project providing a synchronic sociology of the Irish language worldwide, and examining constructions of socio-cultural identity among those learning and using the language in different communities.
Research was carried out in 7 contrasting field sites spanning the Republic of Ireland (Galway and Dublin), Northern Ireland (Derry and Belfast), and the diaspora (Melbourne, Australia; Boston, U.S.; St. John’s, Newfoundland, Canada). Data is drawn largely from semi-structured interviews with 93 Irish-language users across the field sites, and is supplemented by questionnaires and participant observation in each. Discourse analysis (e.g. Hook 2001) of the data’s emergent themes allows for an exploration of the dominant discourses which influence language choices in these communities, as well as in distinct communities of practice within them. Positioning theory (e.g. Harré et al. 2009, Tirado & Gálvez 2007) is used to investigate how Irish speakers situate themselves within these discourses by taking up or resisting available subject positions, and thus how use of the Irish language is made meaningful as a social action in local environments.
This paper examines the ways in which resistance is enacted within discourses specific to each field site, as well as discourses that span across communities. I discuss how Irish takes on local meaning in each location: for example, providing a tool to construct oppositional identity in Newfoundland (in opposition to Newfoundlanders descended from English migrants; in opposition to a generalised Canadian identity); expressing resistance to presumed majority identities in Northern Ireland; and providing a means to symbolically resist pressures to assimilate culturally in diasporic contexts in Boston and Melbourne. I also present ways in which Irish use may be recruited more generally as an act of resistance to dominant discourses via the language’s position within counter-discourses. As such, in addition to contributing to a broader description of Irish-language communities worldwide, this paper argues for an incorporation of social constructionist approaches to identity within sociology of language, particularly with regards to elective bilinguals and in diasporic contexts.
It is estimated that 70 million people around the globe can claim Irish heritage and, although Irish ancestry may be distant for many, the Irish language is active in numerous communities worldwide. The current vitality of Irish as a... more
It is estimated that 70 million people around the globe can claim Irish heritage and, although Irish ancestry may be distant for many, the Irish language is active in numerous communities worldwide. The current vitality of Irish as a minority language within Ireland has been addressed in many studies, however research on Irish-language use within diasporic communities is relatively scarce, documented in some limited research but widely evidenced by the existence of cultural and language groups. This paper reports on research conducted in Ireland (North and South) and in several communities in the Irish diaspora (in Australia, the U.S. and Canada) on constructions of socio-cultural identity among those learning and using the language.

Research was conducted through qualitative interviews with Irish-language users and participant observation in a variety of Irish-language environments. Data collected provides the basis for a synchronic sociology of the Irish language worldwide, revealing differing patterns of usage in distinct communities of practice, some regionally specific and some determined by other factors. Content analysis of the data’s emergent themes allows for an exploration of dominant competing discourses that impact on Irish and minority-language use, including discourses of ethnicity, tradition and minority. These discourses are examined in their historical and political contexts and considered as frames within which the Irish language is made meaningful as a social action in local environments.
LOLspeak is a complex and systematic reimagining of the English language. It is most often associated with the popular, productive and long-lasting internet meme ‘LOLcats’. This style of English is characterised by the simultaneous... more
LOLspeak is a complex and systematic reimagining of the English language. It is most often associated with the popular, productive and long-lasting internet meme ‘LOLcats’. This style of English is characterised by the simultaneous playful manipulation of multiple levels of language in order to perform an authentic ‘cat’ voice, and serves both as an entertaining in-group practice and as a cultural index which is recruited in the construction of identity.
Using community-generated web content as a corpus, we analyse some of the common speech play strategies (Sherzer 2002) used in LOLspeak, which include morphological reanalysis, atypical sentence structure and lexical playfulness. The linguistic variety that emerges from these manipulations displays collaboratively constructed norms and tendencies providing a standard which may be meaningfully adhered to or subverted by users.
Building on this, we use Bucholtz and Hall’s (e.g. 2005) interactional and ethnographic approach to linguistic analysis to examine how the speech play strategies used by participants allow for the simultaneous construction of two identities: firstly the identities of the cats that they claim to be speaking for, and secondly their own identity as savvy members of an online community of practice (Jones & Schieffelin 2009).
Research Interests:
This paper reports on research conducted within the Irish language community in Australia on constructions of socio-cultural identity among those learning and using the language. This study forms part of a broader research project aiming... more
This paper reports on research conducted within the Irish language community in Australia on constructions of socio-cultural identity among those learning and using the language. This study forms part of a broader research project aiming to provide a synchronic account of identity in Irish language learning and usage in Ireland and the Irish diaspora.

Many studies have addressed the current vitality of Irish as a minority language within Ireland (e.g. Ó Flatharta et al. 2009, Ó Giollagáin et al. 2007, O Néill 2005), however research on the status of Irish in the Irish diaspora is relatively scarce. It is estimated that 70 million people around the globe can claim Irish heritage and, while Irish ancestry may be distant for many, the Irish language is active in numerous communities, as documented in some limited research (e.g. Ihde 1994) and evidenced by the existence of numerous cultural and language groups. Data from the last two Australian censuses show a slight increase in Irish speaker numbers from 828 in 2001 to 918 in 2006, but the status of Irish in Australia is similar to the situation Ihde noted in the U.S., that is to say characterised by small pockets of native speakers that will vanish unless replenished by new immigration, and also by larger groups of language learners who in general will not reach a high level of Irish language competence.

The current study investigates the role of the Irish language in the construction of diasporic identity, as well as reporting on the role of diasporic language use in revitalisation efforts in Ireland. Research is conducted primarily through questionnaire-based interviews with learners and users of the Irish language in Australia, where information is elicited regarding informants’ demographic, Irish language use, language attitudes, ethno-cultural activities, ancestry/migration history and personal identification, as well as the motivating factors in their language use. Identity in this study is viewed as a dynamic and emergent social construction, which relies on the negation and negotiation of the ‘other’ (Woodward 2002, Bucholtz and Hall 2005) and which, within the context of the diaspora, is subject to “necessary heterogeneity and diversity” (Hall 1993: 401) and heightened contingency, indeterminacy and conflict (Gilroy 1997). Ager’s (2001) model of motivation in language planning, with components of motives, attitudes and goals/needs, provides a framework for accounting for motivating factors in language use.

Results demonstrate that established narratives and discourse within the Irish diaspora that tend to privilege the migrant, and especially the Irish-speaking migrant, as white, Catholic, nationalist, straight and working-class may not reflect the reality of the demographic, and that prevalent notions of diasporic nostalgia, with attendant phases of nationalism and the fetishising and repeating of the past do not provide a complete account of factors in Irish language use. Comparisons are drawn between identity constructs identified in this study and those established in other surveys conducted in Ireland and key areas of the Irish diaspora (Boston, U.S.A.; Newfoundland, Canada).

Ager, Dennis. 2001. Motivation in language planning and language policy. Clevedon: Multilingual Matters Ltd.
Bucholtz, Mary and Kira Hall. 2005. Identity and interaction: a sociocultural linguistic approach. Discourse Studies, 7(4-5): 585-614.
Gilroy, David. 1997. Diaspora and the detours of identity. In Kathryn Woodward (ed) Identity and difference. London: Sage. 299-343
Hall, Stuart. 1993. Cultural Identity and Diaspora. In Patrick Williams and Laura Chrisman (eds) Colonial Discourse and Post-Colonial Theory: a Reader.  Hemel Hempstead: Harvester Wheatsheaf. 392-401.
Ihde, Thomas. 1994. The Irish language in the United States: a historical, sociolinguistic and applied linguistic survey. Westport, CT: Bergin & Garvey.
Ó Flatharta, Peadar, Caoilfhionn Nic Pháidín, Colin Williams, Francois Grin, and Joseph Lo Bianco. 2009. 20-Year Strategy for the Irish Language. Prepared for the Department of Community, Rural and Gaeltacht Affairs. Dublin: Fiontar, Dublin City University.
Ó Giollagáin, Conchúr , Seosamh Mac Donnacha, Fiona Ní Chualáin, Aoife Ní Shéaghdha, and Mary O’Brien. 2007. Comprehensive linguistic study of the use of Irish in the Gaeltacht : principal findings and recommendations : a research report. Prepared for The Department of Community, Rural and Gaeltacht Affairs. Dublin: National University of Ireland, Galway (Acadamh na hOllscolaíochta Gaeilge).
O’Néill, Diarmuid. 2005. Rebuilding the Celtic languages: Reversing Language Shift in the Celtic Countries. Talybont: Y Lolfa Cyn.
Woodward, Kathryn. 2002. Understanding Identity. London: Arnold.

Keywords: language endangerment, language and identity, language attitudes, Irish, diaspora
Many studies have addressed the current vitality of Irish as a minority language within Ireland, however research on Irish-language use within diasporic communities is relatively scarce. It is estimated that 70 million people around the... more
Many studies have addressed the current vitality of Irish as a minority language within Ireland, however research on Irish-language use within diasporic communities is relatively scarce. It is estimated that 70 million people around the globe can claim Irish heritage and, while Irish ancestry may be distant for many, the Irish language is active in numerous communities, as documented in some limited research (e.g. Ihde 1994) and evidenced by the existence of cultural and language groups. This paper reports on research conducted in Ireland (North and South) and in several communities in the Irish diaspora (in Australia, the U.S. and Canada) on constructions of socio-cultural identity among those learning and using the language.

Research was conducted primarily through questionnaire-based interviews with Irish-language users. Data collected provides the basis for a synchronic sociology of the Irish language worldwide, revealing differing patterns of usage in distinct communities of practice, some regionally specific and some determined by other factors. Qualitative analysis of the data’s emergent themes allows for an exploration of dominant competing discourses that impact on Irish and minority-language use, including discourses of ethnicity, tradition and minority. Positioning theory (e.g. Harré et al. 2009) provides a means to investigate how Irish speakers situate themselves by taking up or resisting subject positions within these discourses in their historical and political contexts and how use of the Irish language is made meaningful as a social action in local environments.

Ihde, Thomas. 1994. The Irish language in the United States: a historical, sociolinguistic and applied linguistic survey. Westport, CT: Bergin & Garvey.
Harré, Rom, Fathali M. Moghaddam, Tracey Pilkerton Cairnie, Daniel Rothbart and Steven R. Sabat. 2009. Recent Advances in Positioning Theory. Theory & Psychology, 19(1): 5-31.
Speakers of Burarra (north-central Arnhem Land) identity four dialects of their language, but much variation that distinguishes them survives predominantly in older speakers, outside of the urban centre, Maningrida. This project will... more
Speakers of Burarra (north-central Arnhem Land) identity four dialects of their language, but much variation that distinguishes them survives predominantly in older speakers, outside of the urban centre, Maningrida. This project will produce a stratified corpus of naturalistic language use across a range of genres from speakers across the Burarra region, and will gather rich data about multilingual practices and language ideologies. As well as providing a significant record of cultural, mythological and local territorial knowledge, the project’s fundamental focus on dialectal and other variation will enhance our understandings of the linguistic construction of difference in a highly multilingual context.

Access the collection here: http://hdl.handle.net/2196/8ed9691a-9033-4ba7-bd89-ff70e621b142