Skip to main content

Michael R Marlo

Research Interests:
Research Interests:
Slides from my presentation at the workshop, Tone in African Languages, at Kenyatta University, May 16-17, 2016.
Research Interests:
Presentation from the workshop, Tone in African Languages, May 16-17, 2016, at Kenyatta University.
Research Interests:
This paper describes the process of studying the notoriously complex verbal tone systems of Bantu languages through the elicitation of systematic, paradigmatic data. The main thrust of this paper is the delineation of the factors known to... more
This paper describes the process of studying the notoriously complex verbal tone systems of Bantu languages through the elicitation of systematic, paradigmatic data. The main thrust of this paper is the delineation of the factors known to influence tonal outputs in Bantu languages, providing background on the micro-typology of Bantu verbal tone systems, with discussion of how these considerations impact the data-gathering process.
In this paper, we provide an overview of Kuria tone melodies, which are notable in the positions targeted by the melodic H, including the third and fourth moras of the verb macrostem. For some speakers, the moras of the word following the... more
In this paper, we provide an overview of Kuria tone melodies, which are notable in the positions targeted by the melodic H, including the third and fourth moras of the verb macrostem. For some speakers, the moras of the word following the verb are part of the representation when the melodic H is assigned to the verb. We also discuss certain complications to the melodic system in imperatives and some negative verb forms, which involve differences in the way in which otherwise regular patterns of spreading apply and in the realization of the melodic H on the final vowel.
This paper reports on verb tone melodies in Bakweri, which has 6 inflectionally-governed stem tone patterns. Depending on tense, a verb may have no tones beyond those in the lexicon, or may add L, H, HL, or LH to the stem. These melodic... more
This paper reports on verb tone melodies in Bakweri, which has 6 inflectionally-governed stem tone patterns. Depending on tense, a verb may have no tones beyond those in the lexicon, or may add L, H, HL, or LH to the stem. These melodic tones are linked to the final vowel, and melody-initial H spreads leftward to the lexical tone of the root. In addition, the reflexive contributes a LH melody which partially replaces the inherent melody determined by a verb’s tense, and which undergoes a special leftward spreading rule.
Research Interests:
This paper presents an overview of the verbal tone system of Lower Nyole (LN), a moribund and previously unrecognized variety of Nyole (JE35) in southern Busoga. LN has a reversive tone system with two tonal classes of verbs – (i) verbs... more
This paper presents an overview of the verbal tone system of Lower Nyole (LN), a moribund and previously unrecognized variety of Nyole (JE35) in southern Busoga. LN has a reversive tone system with two tonal classes of verbs – (i) verbs which are underlyingly toneless and (ii) historically *H-toned verbs which synchronically have an initial /L/ tone. All known verbal contexts of LN are inflected with a melodic tone pattern, i.e. there are no tonally uninflected constructions. Like other Luyia languages, LN has a large number of tonal inflections, characterized by assigning Hs to positions at the edges of the verb stem. An additional noteworthy property of LN verb tone is that long vowels that precede NC clusters are treated like short vowels by tone assignment principles.
Research Interests:
The Luyia languages of western Kenya and eastern Uganda have some of the most complicated systems of tonal morphology among Bantu languages. Luyia varieties commonly have 7-8 or more inflectional tonal patterns in verbs, and as many as 12... more
The Luyia languages of western Kenya and eastern Uganda have some of the most complicated systems of tonal morphology among Bantu languages. Luyia varieties commonly have 7-8 or more inflectional tonal patterns in verbs, and as many as 12 are reported in one Marachi dialect, possibly marking the peak complexity within Bantu.

Although segmental and lexical differences among Luyia varieties tend to be relatively minor, there is considerable diversity among Luyia verbal tonal systems. Eastern varieties like Idakho and Tachoni have historically conservative systems with a contrast between /H/ and /Ø/ verb roots. In these languages, some tenses are inflected with a ‘melodic H’ that links to a position at one of the edges of the stem, e.g. second mora, final vowel. Southwestern Luyia varieties like Khayo and Saamia have developed into so-called ‘predictable’ systems in which there are no lexical contrasts in verb roots, and all tenses are inflected with a melodic tone. Yet other Luyia varieties have hybrid properties, with a lexical contrast between two tonal types of roots, and a melodic tonal inflection in all verb forms. Some of these systems, e.g. Nyole and Nyala East, appear to have inverted root H tones as synchronically /L/, while others, e.g. Bukusu and Wanga, are more ambiguous as to whether historically *H verb roots are synchronically /H/ or /L/.

Throughout Bantu, nominal tone is less well studied than verbal tone, due to the fact that nouns are morphologically simpler and tend not to have inflectional alternations. However, emerging research suggests that at least some Luyia noun tone systems rival their verbal tone systems in complexity. For instance, eight distinct lexical tonal patterns are attested in disyllabic noun stems in the central Luyia variety Wanga—a historical doubling of the four patterns reconstructed for Proto-Bantu (LL, HH, LH, HL). Two patterns surface all L in phrase-final position: toneless omu-limi ‘farmer’ vs. L-final axasì ‘maternal cousin’. These two patterns distinguish themselves phrase-medially. Three other tone patterns are characterized by a single H that occupies different positions: eʃi-láaro ‘shoe’ vs. omu-laáme ‘heir’ vs. i-koofyá ‘hat’. Three further patterns are characterized by two Hs, again differing in the positions of the Hs: eʃi-muúꜜná ‘squirrel’ vs. eʃíꜜ-túúyu ‘rabbit’ vs. liíꜜ-téété ‘grasshopper’.
As seems to be the norm in Luyia, there is also significant internal diversity in the lexical tonal patterns of nouns.

This talk aims to begin the challenging task of explaining the diversification and complexification of modern Luyia tone systems.
Research Interests:
This paper offers an overview of verbal tone melodies within Luyia, a cluster of Bantu languages spoken in Kenya and Uganda. Luyia tone is diverse, possessing three types of verbal tonal systems: ‘conservative’, ‘predictable’, and... more
This paper offers an overview of verbal tone melodies within Luyia, a cluster of Bantu languages spoken in Kenya and Uganda. Luyia tone is diverse, possessing three types of verbal tonal systems: ‘conservative’, ‘predictable’, and ‘reversive’. We illustrate the general tonal characteristics of each type of system with an exemplar language variety, describing the complex interactions of lexical and melodic tones.
In this poster, we compare the principles of tone assignment in Bukusu nouns and verbs.
Research Interests:
In this talk, we present an overview of nominal and verbal tone patterns in Bukusu, a Bantu language of western Kenya. In Bukusu, tone distinguishes lexical items and is also a major feature of teh marking of grammatical distinctions such... more
In this talk, we present an overview of nominal and verbal tone patterns in Bukusu, a Bantu language of western Kenya. In Bukusu, tone distinguishes lexical items and is also a major feature of teh marking of grammatical distinctions such as tense. We discuss data from a variety of morphological and syntactic contexts and illustrate some of the ways in which components of grammar interact to produce complex surface tonal patterns.
This paper describes and analyzes the primary tonal patterns found in Bukusu nouns and their alternations in a phrasal context.
Research Interests:
This chapter discusses the symbiotic relationship of linguistic description and comparative research: linguistic typology relies on detailed studies of individual languages, and the grammatical description of individual languages,... more
This chapter discusses the symbiotic relationship of linguistic description and comparative research: linguistic typology relies on detailed studies of individual languages, and the grammatical description of individual languages, including endangered languages, benefits from the prior and concurrent study of closely related languages and the identification of parameters along which these languages are similar and different. This view is supported by discussion of phenomena in Bantu languages including tone and reduplication which have considerable micro-parametric variation, particularly in the behavior of object markers. Two case studies are presented: (i) exceptional tonal properties of the 1sg object prefix N‑ and the reflexive marker di‑i‑ in Yao and (ii) exceptional patterns of reduplication involving /i/-initial verbs in Buguumbe Kuria which suggest a connection with the reflexive. In each case, knowledge of analogous patterns in other languages informs the description and analysis of the individual language. In addition, each case expands knowledge of the typology of patterns of exceptional object marking in Bantu languages, which improves the quality of the description of other languages in the future.
This article provides an overview of the numerous exceptional morphosyntactic and phonological properties of the reflexive compared to other object prefixes in Bantu languages. These properties are considered in light of Marlo's (2014,... more
This article provides an overview of the numerous exceptional morphosyntactic and phonological properties of the reflexive compared to other object prefixes in Bantu languages. These properties are considered in light of Marlo's (2014, 2015, to appear) micro-typological studies of Bantu object marking, and the proposal that the 1sg object prefix and the reflexive may be in a position closer to the verb stem than other object prefixes.
This paper describes the morphosyntactic diversity among Bantu languages in the number of pre-stem and post-stem object markers (OMs) that are allowed and discusses the factors responsible for this diversity. The number of OMs is... more
This paper describes the morphosyntactic diversity among Bantu languages in the number of pre-stem and post-stem object markers (OMs) that are allowed and discusses the factors responsible for this diversity. The number of OMs is sometimes highly restricted and sometimes seemingly unrestricted. Some languages have only pre-stem marking of objects; some have only post-stem marking; and some have both pre-stem and post-stem marking. Person-number, animacy, and locative noun classes are the basis for a number of morphosyntactic asymmetries in individual systems in whether objects are marked before or after the stem, though other factors such as grammatical relations are also involved. A handful of types of OMs, including locatives and the 1sg and reflexive markers, commonly allow languages to circumvent restrictions on object marking, allowing one more OM than usual. The number of OMs is also restricted by certain syntactic configurations such as passivization. A number of other issues that are important general considerations in the syntax of Bantu languages are shown to have at most a secondary role in determining the number of OMs that are allowed.
Presentation at the workshop, Tone in African Languages, May 16-17, 2016, at Kenyatta University.
Research Interests:
This presentation provides information about Michael Marlo's experience as a Fulbright Core US Scholar at Kenyatta University in Kenya in 2016.
Research Interests:
In this presentation, we will look at an University of Missouri project to document the linguistic diversity and history of a group of languages spoken near Lake Victoria in western Kenya and eastern Uganda.
Research Interests:
In this informal presentation (to an audience at Kenyatta University), I describe the background, goals, and preliminary results of work on my Fulbright grant, and my related research project, Structure and Tone in Luyia. This project... more
In this informal presentation (to an audience at Kenyatta University), I describe the background, goals, and preliminary results of work on my Fulbright grant, and my related research project, Structure and Tone in Luyia. This project involves the development of foundational descriptive linguistic materials on the phonology, morphology, and syntax of four varieties of Luyia: Bukusu, Logoori, Tiriki, and Wanga. A special emphasis is the tonal system of each language. As a Fulbright scholar, I am also contributing to the training of students in the documentation and description of understudied African languages in an effort to combat language endangerment.
Research Interests:
This paper presents an overview of the tone system of Lower Nyole (LN), a moribund and previously unrecognized variety of Nyole (JE35) spoken in southern Busoga. After providing a preliminary study of tonal patterns in nouns, we show that... more
This paper presents an overview of the tone system of Lower Nyole (LN), a moribund and previously unrecognized variety of Nyole (JE35) spoken in southern Busoga. After providing a preliminary study of tonal patterns in nouns, we show that LN has a 'reversive' tone system with two tonal classes of verbs – (i) verbs which are underlyingly toneless and (ii) historically *H-toned verbs which synchronically have an initial /L/ tone. All known verbal contexts of LN are inflected with a melodic tone pattern, i.e. there are no tonally uninflected constructions. Like other Luyia languages, LN has a large number of tonal inflections, which are characterized by the assignment of Hs to positions at the edges of the verb stem. An additional noteworthy property of LN verb tone is that long vowels that precede NC clusters are treated like short vowels by tone assignment principles.
Studies of Bantu noun tone are relatively few compared to those on verb tone. Even seminal research on the subject is arguably limited in scope, tending to focus on the behavior of mono- and disyllabic nouns and on the connection between... more
Studies of Bantu noun tone are relatively few compared to those on verb tone. Even seminal research on the subject is arguably limited in scope, tending to focus on the behavior of mono- and disyllabic nouns and on the connection between synchronic tonal melodies and those reconstructed for Proto-Bantu cognates. This paper adds to a recent trend of looking beyond these few noun shapes to consider the how tone is manifested on a wider variety of Wanga nouns including those that are morphologically complex in various ways. Drawing on the tonal behavior of approximately 2,100 nouns in our amassed lexicon, we have identified eight major noun tone patterns.

We divide Wanga noun tone melodies into pairs (A/G, B/C, E/F, D/H) based on their surface characteristics. We illustrate, however, that nouns exhibiting one melody vs. the other in a given pair appear to have different origins. Some Wanga noun tone melodies have a direct mapping to those reconstructed for Proto-Bantu and mirror the diachronic behavior of certain other Lacustrine languages; Wanga Pattern A largely corresponds to Proto-Bantu *LL. Other melodies are innovative (such as Pattern H), while still others maintain prosodic characteristics of the language from which they were borrowed; Wanga has reinterpreted Swahili’s pattern of antepenultimate stress as HØ in its Pattern F nouns. We show further evidence for sub-patterns within some melodies that have emerged due to interactions between a noun’s lexical tonal melody and other tones contributed either by the augment (pre-prefix) or, in some instances, other prefixes and even bases, in instances of reduplication.

Of particular interest to us are connections that Wanga’s nominal tonology reveal to the verb tone system, as described elsewhere. For example, both noun and verb tone provide strong yet oftentimes covert evidence for an underlying three-way tonal contrast between /H/, /L/, and /Ø/. The fact that these contrasts, particularly between /L/ and /Ø/ (as in Patterns A vs. G and Pattern E vs. F), are revealed only in certain phrasal contexts may have implications for the eventual leveling of these distinctions altogether. Also, like in the verb system, the presence of a L tone in a noun tone melody has the ability to block tonal alternations due Reverse Meeussen’s Rule (HH  ØH) which, in turn, has an influence on still other outcomes that we and others attributed to Prefix Hop. L tone can also block anticipatory H spreading when a noun appears phrase-medially, just as it does in Wanga verbs. These parallels are not absolute, however, as Wanga nouns witness unbounded rightward H spreading and Meeussen’s Rule (HH  HØ), both of which are absent in the verb system.