Chavez-Arana Et Al., 2018
Chavez-Arana Et Al., 2018
Chavez-Arana Et Al., 2018
doi: 10.1093/jpepsy/jsy013
Advance Access Publication Date: 21 March 2018
Systematic Review
Abstract
Objective We investigated interventions, which aimed to improve cold and hot executive
functions (EFs) in children and adolescents with a diagnosis of acquired brain injury (ABI).
Methods The following electronic databases were searched: Medline, CINAHL, PsycINFO, and
Pubmed. The database filters limited the search to articles published between 1990 and July 2017
in English or Spanish, including children and adolescents. Articles were read and classified accord-
ing to the levels of evidence of the Australian National Health and Medical Research Council and
the Downs and Black checklist was used for Measuring Study Quality. Results Thirty studies are
reported in this systematic review. Level of evidence, quality of the studies, characteristics of the
participants, interventions implemented, and outcomes are described. Conclusions The study
of rehabilitation for executive dysfunction in children with ABI is emerging. Although few high-
quality intervention studies exist in this area, which limits conclusions regarding intervention effi-
cacy, results of existing studies suggest that education for parents may be an important component
of intervention. Moreover, caregiver involvement may improve the effectiveness of hot EFs rehabili-
tation interventions, while high intervention session frequency may be important in improving cold
EFs. Positive behavior supports and specific training based on a cognitive model provided some
promising findings, which require further evaluation.
Key words: acquired brain injury; adolescents; children; executive functions; intervention;
rehabilitation and systematic review.
Acquired brain injury (ABI) is a term that encom- Children and adolescents with ABI usually present with
passes damage to the brain that occurs after birth deficits in several cognitive domains such as attention,
Australian Institute of Health and Welfare (2007). processing speed, memory, language, and social
ABIs have diverse causes including traumatic brain problem-solving skills. In particular, executive func-
injury (TBI), cerebral vascular diseases, infections, tions (EFs) deficits are common and emerge with time
brain tumors, vestibular dysfunction, and postsurgi- postinjury as the child matures (Galvin, Lim, Steer,
cal complications (Ciuffreda & Kapoor, 2012). Edwards, & Lee, 2010; Kok et al., 2014).
C The Author(s) 2018. Published by Oxford University Press on behalf of the Society of Pediatric Psychology.
V
All rights reserved. For permissions, please e-mail: journals.permissions@oup.com 928
Interventions for Hot and Cold Executive Functions 929
EFs refer to a group of cognitive skills required for commitment therapy, which emphasizes acceptance
purposeful goal-directed activity and can be divided rather than behavior change or elimination only
into cold and hot domains (De Luca & Leventer (Morris, Johns, & Oliver, 2013). One more way to in-
(2008). Cold EF refers to purely cognitive EF, while volve caregivers is by teaching them positive behavior
hot EF refers to the affective aspects of these cognitive supports (PBS) to improve their child’s behavior (Carr
skills (Kerr & Zelazo, 2004). Cold EFs are more likely et al., 2002). Three main ideas support the inclusion
to be elicited by decontextualized problems such as of caregivers in a child’s rehabilitation: (1) caregivers
manipulation of abstract concepts, numbers, or letters are aware of the behavioral changes presented after in-
(Brock, Rimm-Kaufman, Nathanson, & Grimm, jury onset; (2) they are likely to be a stable resource
2009; Hongwanishkul, Happaney, Lee, & Zelazo, throughout child’s life, and (3) EFs development can
2005). Hot EFs refer to the socioemotional domain be affected by family factors (Catroppa et al., 2017;
and are more likely to be evoked by motivationally Lefley, 2009).
Idenficaon
database searching through other sources
(n = 771) (n = 7)
(n = 56) (n = 26)
Included
Studies included in
qualitave synthesis
(n = 30)
After classifying the articles, quality was assessed by methods to assign participants to groups was common
two authors using the Downs and Black checklist for (five studies). Lack of a control group was a character-
Measuring Study Quality (DB; Downs & Black, istic of the studies with a level of evidence IV (seven
1998). The DB checklist consists of 27 items that as- studies), while using a blind RCT resulted in a level of
sess the quality of reporting (10 items), external valid- evidence II (three studies). On average, included stud-
ity (3 items), internal validity (7 items), confounders ies met 72.3% of the DB checklist criteria for study
(6 items), and power effect (1 item) (Downs & Black, quality. Most studies reported aims, outcomes mea-
1998; see Online Supplementary Table S3). Effect sured, characteristics of the participants, primary
sizes were not always reported in studies. Therefore, results, and interventions. Adverse events or absence
based on a previous systematic review (Knight, of adverse events and information regarding partici-
Scheinberg, and Harvey, 2013), we assigned a score of pants lost to follow-up were often not reported. On
1 to studies that reported effect sizes and 0 to studies average, studies were 62.8% adherent to external va-
First author Level of DB checklist total Name of Target of the Type of Delivery mode Duration Frequency Providers Attrition Outcome
(year) evidence score intervention intervention intervention (# of sessions) (duration of skills
ANHMRC each session)
Braga (2012) III-1 17 Metacognitive di- Metacognition SCT Group and indi- 3 months (26) Twice a week Trained psy- Metacognition þ
mension and self- vidual, FTF (2 hr) chology Self-esteem þ
program esteem students
Chan (2011) III-1 16 Problem-solving Problem-solving SCT Group, FTF 7 weeks (14) Twice a week 0% Metacognition þ
skills training and (3 hr) Problem-solving þ
program metacognition
Eve (2016) IV 14 Cogmed Working SCT Individual, 5–7 weeks (25) Weekly Cogmed cer- 22% Central executive
memory online (30–40 min) tified and visuospatial
coach sketchpad 0
Phonological
loop þ
Galbiati III-2 16 Attention remedia- Attention SCT Individual, FTF 6 months (104) Four times a 0% Attention þ
(2009) tion training week
program (45 min)
Kaldoja III-2 13 FORAMENRehab Attention SCT Individual, 6 weeks (10) Twice a week 0% Focused visual 0
(2015) online (30–50 min) Focus auditory
0 Sustained þ*
Complex —
Tracking 0
Seguin (2017) II 21 Ready! Set? Let’s Attention and SCT Individual, FTF 5 weeks (15) Three times a Trained psy- 10.5% Attention 0
Train! metacognition week (1 hr) chology Metacognition –
students Flexibility,
working memory
and inhibition þ
Sjö (2010) IV 10 Amat-C Attention and SCT Individual, FTF 6–9 months Every week- Supervised Attention —
memory (100) day (30–45 trainer Memory —
min) and
teacher
Thomas- III-1 13 TEACH-ware Attention, mem- SCT Individual, com- 8 weeks (—) Two times a Language 0% Attention 0
Stonell ory, and prob- puter-based week (1 hr) therapist Memory þ
(1994) lem-solving Problem-solving 0
van’t Hooft III-1 18 Amat-C Attention and SCT Children and 17 weeks Weekly 10% Sustained
(2005), memory coaches, FTF (17 and sessions (—); attention þ
(2007) and home/ practice at Daily work Selective
school home/school) (30 min) attention þ*
practice Memory þ/0
Note. ANHMRC ¼ Australian National Health and Medical Research Council; EF ¼ executive function; DB checklist ¼ Downs and Black checklist; FTF ¼ face-to-face—unable to determined or not reported..; SCT ¼ Specific
cognitive training. ANHMRC classifies case series, posttest, and pre–posttest as Level IV; comparative studies without controls are Level III-3; comparative studies with concurrent controls are considered Level III-2; pseudo-RCTs
are Level III-1; RCTs are Level II and systematic reviews of RCT are Level I. The total score from the DB checklist is reported. * Indicates changes maintains at follow-up; þ Indicates significant improvement in at least one outcome;
0 indicates no significant change; / was used in the outcome column when different results were found across measures of the same cognitive process.
Chavez-Arana et al.
First author Level of evi- DB Name of Target of the Type of Delivery mode Duration Frequency (dura- Providers skills Attrition Outcome
(year) dence checklist intervention intervention intervention (# of sessions) tion of each
ANHMRC total score session)
Brown (2014) II 22 SSTP combined Child behavior and PBS and ACT Group and individ- 10 weeks (8 group — (FTF 2 hr; phone Clinical psycholo- Post 16.66%; Behavior R þ*
with ACT emotion, ual, FTF com- sessions, 3 phone calls 1.5 hr) gists certified in Follow-up 20%; Emotion R þ
parent’s psycho- bined with calls) SSTP Total: 36.66% Parenting þ*
logical flexibility telephone
and experiential
avoidance
Pastore (2011) III-2 17 CBT Child behavior CBT Individual 4–8 months (—) 2–3 each week Therapist — Social
(45–60 min) abilities þ Behavior
Rþ
Emotion R þ
Tlustos (2016) III-1 17 CAPS Social competence CBT Family, online 6 months (—) 1–2 each week Clinical 6% Social
(at least 1 hr) psychologists abilities þ/0
Wade (2005) IV 18 FPS Cognition, coping, CBT Family, online — (8 core and 4 — (videoconference Research assistant 0% Social
and family optional) 45–60 min; —) and therapist abilities 0
communication Emotion R 0
EFs 0
Interventions for Hot and Cold Executive Functions
Family
functioning 0
Wade (2006) III-1 21 FPS Behavior and social CBT Family, online — (8 core and 6 Every 1 or 2 weeks Psychologist and 20% Social abilities 0
competence optional) (—) trained psych Behavior R 0/þ
graduate
Wade (2006) III-1 20 FPS Problem-solving CBT Family, online 6 months (7 core Every 1 or 2 weeks Trained psych 15.78% Behavior R þ
skills and 4 optional) (75–100 min) graduate Parental
stress 0
Wade (2008) III-1 20 TOPS EFs, language prag- CBT and meta- Family, online — (10 core and 4 Every 1 or 2 weeks Psych graduate 0% Behavior R 0
matics, and so- cognitive optional) (—) Emotion R þ
cial processing training Parental
stress 0 Family
functioning þ
Wade (2011) III-1 18 TOPS Behavior and par- CBT and meta- Family, online 6 months — — 20% Behavior R 0
ent–teen cognitive (10 core, 4 Family
conflicts training optional) conflict —
Wade (2014, III-1 20 CAPS Behavior CBT Family, online 6 months (8 core, — Trained Post: 12.30%; Behavior R and in-
2015) 4 supplemental, psychologists 18 months after hibition in
6 videoconference) baseline: 30.8% older
adolescents þ,
in younger adoles-
cents 0.
Wiseman- IV 11 Improving prag- Pragmatic skills Group, FTF 6 weeks (24) 4 each week (4 hr) Trained social — Social
Hakes matic skills worker sufficiency 0
(1998)
Woods, IV 14 Signpost Child behavior and PBS and CBT Individual, tele- 5 months (7 core and — Signpost certified — Behavior R þ/0
Catroppa, parenting phone, combined 2 supplemental) psychologist Parenting þ
Godfrey, & practices with home Parental
Anderson practice stress 0
(2014)
(continued)
933
pseudo-RCTs are Level III-1; SSTP ¼ stepping stones triple P; TOPS ¼ Teen online problem-solving; RCTs are Level II and systematic reviews of RCT are Level I. The total score from the DB checklist is reported. * Indicates changes maintains at follow
DB checklist ¼ Downs and Black checklist; EFs ¼ executive functions; FPS ¼ family problem-solving intervention; FTF ¼ face-to-face; PBS ¼ positive behavior supports; R ¼ regulation; RCT ¼ randomized control trial; signposts ¼ signposts for build-
Note. — Unable to determined or not reported; ACT ¼ acceptance and commitment therapy; ANHMRC: Australian National Health and Medical Research Council; CAPS ¼ counselor-assisted problem-solving; CBT ¼ cognitive behavior therapy;
ing better behavior; SSTP ¼ stepping stones triple P. ANHMRC classifies case series, posttest, and pre–post-test as Level IV; comparative studies without controls are Level III-3; comparative studies with concurrent controls are considered Level III-2;
Behavior R and pa-
rental stress chil-
Interventions for Both Hot and Cold EFs
risk þ, children
Outcome Table III describes the interventions in which the pri-
not at risk 0
Parenting þ
mary objective was to improve overall EFs. The types
dren at
of interventions applied by these studies were ABI edu-
cation, interactive video gaming, cognitive behavior
therapy, and specific cognitive training.
Attrition
Outcomes
Tables I, II, and III present the main outcomes of the
up; þ Indicates significant improvement in at least one outcome; 0 Indicates no significant change; / was used in the outcome column when different results were found across measures of the same cognitive process.
0%
Signpost certified
Cold EF Outcomes
Interventions that were effective in improving atten-
Frequency (dura-
(15–20 min)
session)
8 phone calls)
Child behavior,
Hot EF Outcomes
Six studies measured functional domains related to so-
Signpost
17
ANHMRC
dence
III-2
Table II. Continued
Anderson
Godfrey,
(2014)
First author Level of DB Name of Target of the Type of Delivery mode Duration Frequency Providers skills Attrition Outcome
(year) evidence checklist intervention intervention intervention (# of sessions) (duration of
ANHMRC total score each session)
Dise-Lewis IV 13 BrainSTARS Cognitive Psycho- Group, FTF 4 months (3) Every 6 or — — Behavior R 0
(2009) development education and self- 8 weeks (—) EFs 0
guided
De Kloet IV 17 Therapy Wii Cognitive, so- Interactive Individual 12 weeks (2 — (sessions 1 Occupational 10% Attention þ/0
(2012) cial, physical video gaming FTF and prac- hr, home therapist, Working
activity and tice at home) practice 2 hr physical memory þ
QOL a week) therapist and Social abilities
trained þ/0
teachers
Kurowski II 26 CAPS Problem solv- CBT Family, 6 months (7 Every 1 or 2 Psychologist 6-months: EFs in older
(2013), ing, commu- Online core and 4 weeks (—) 5.97%; teens þ*
(2014) nication and optional) 18-months: EFs in younger
self- 70.14% teens 0
Interventions for Hot and Cold Executive Functions
regulation
Treble- III-2 16 AIM Attention and SCT Individual, 10 weeks (10) Weekly sessions Trained 41% Inhibition 0
Barna EFs FTF com- (60–90 min); psychologists Flexibility 0
(2015) bined with homework two Planning 0
home online to four times Behavior R
practice per week þ/0/0
(20–40 min)
Wade III-1 21 TOPS EFs CBT and meta- Family, online — (10 core and — Psychologist 20% EFs severe
(2010) cognitive 4 optional) and trained TBI þ
training psych EFs moderate
graduates TBI 0
EFS reported by
parents 0
Note. —unable to determined or not reported; ANHMRC ¼ Australian National Health and Medical Research Council; BrainSTARS ¼ Brain Injury: strategies for teams and reeducation for students;
CBT ¼ cognitive behavior therapy; DB checklist ¼ Downs and Black checklist EFs ¼ executive functions; FTF ¼ face-to-face; QOL ¼ quality of life; R ¼ regulation; RCT ¼ randomized control trial;
SCT ¼ specific cognitive training. ANHMRC classifies case series, posttest, and pre–post-test as Level IV; comparative studies without controls are Level III-3; comparative studies with concurrent con-
trols are considered Level III-2; pseudo-RCTs are Level III-1; RCTs are Level II and systematic reviews of RCT are Level I. The total score from the DB checklist is reported. * Indicates changes maintains
at follow-up; þ Indicates significant improvement in at least one outcome; 0 Indicates no significant change; / was used in the outcome column when different results were found across measures of the
same cognitive process.
935
Family Factor Outcomes because of the high prevalence of child TBI in compar-
The studies that taught parents PBS were effective in ison with other types of ABI and there being more
improving parenting practices. Improvements in pa- male participants in this clinical group (Thurman,
rental distress were not presented in most of the stud- 2016). Total 61.5% of the studies reported in this sys-
ies. Family functioning presented mixed results. tematic review focused in studying participants with
All the studies that targeted metacognition, work- TBI. The absence of studies with young children is sur-
ing memory, and parenting practices found at least prising considering that ABI is the leading cause of in-
some improvement. Some improvement in emotional jury in children <5 years of age (Thurman, 2016).
regulation, family functioning, behavior regulation, Taking into account that cognitive sequelae of brain
and attention were present in half or more of the stud- injury in children become more evident over time
ies targeting those outcomes. Effective results were (Anderson, Spencer-Smith, & Wood, 2011), it is
limited in the studies that measure overall EFs, social expected that family and school staff’s concerns about
abilities, parental stress, and planning.
was often not assessed. Only two studies found main- how to deconstruct or measure social skills as a cogni-
tenance of some improvement in attention at 6 months tive process; therefore, comparison among studies is
(van’t Hooft, et al., 2007) and 1.63 years (Kaldoja not possible. The model from Beauchamp and
et al., 2015) after the intervention was completed. Anderson (2010) describes the cognitive processes in-
Interventions that aim to improve attention were volve in social skills development, which can support
greatly influenced by the attention treatment model of the development of assessments tools and interven-
Sohlberg and Mateer (1987). This model has a specific tions for social skills.
process approach and considers five levels of atten- The concepts from Ylvisaker and Feeney (2009)
tion: focused, sustained, selective, alternating, and di- strongly influenced the rehabilitation of hot EFs in
vided. Tasks for each level of attention are applied children with ABI. They propose that the adults in-
based on its difficulty, going from basic to more com- volve in the daily routines of the child should partici-
plex tasks (Sohlberg & Mateer, 1987). The specific pate in the intervention (Ylvisaker & Feeney, 2009).
require hot and cold EFs to work together toward Hispanic population. Future studies could examine
adaptive function (Zelazo & Carlson, 2012). In addi- the feasibility and effectiveness of interventions in
tion, impairments in either hot or cold EFs can have Spanish-speaking countries.
secondary consequences to other aspects of EF (Blair
& Diamond, 2008). Therefore, balance between cog- Limitations
nition and emotion is required for self-regulation de- The present study has several limitations. The way in
velopment (Blair & Diamond, 2008). which EFs are deconstructed and measured varied
Some of the interventions described are adaptations among studies; therefore, conclusions were drawn
of interventions previously used in other populations, based on the qualitative synthesis of the studies. In ad-
such as children with neurodevelopmental disorders dition, the keywords used in the search did not specify
or adults with ABI. For this reason, we suggest that the variety of brain injuries. As a consequence, rele-
adapting interventions from different populations can vant studies may have been overlooked and not
D’Zurilla, T. J., & Nezu, A. M. (1999). Problem-solving Kok, T. B., Post, W. J., Tucha, O., de Bont, E. S., Kamps, W.
therapy: A social competence approach to clinical inter- A., & Kingma, A. (2014). Social competence in children
vention (2nd ed.). New York: Springer. with brain disorders: A meta-analytic review.
Eve, M., O’Keeffe, F., Jhuty, S., Ganesan, V., Brown, G., & Neuropsychology Review, 24, 219–235.
Murphy, T. (2016). Computerized Working-Memory Kurowski, B. G., Wade, S. L., Kirkwood, M. W., Brown, T.
Training for Children Following Arterial Ischemic Stroke: M., Stancin, T., & Taylor, H. G. (2013). Online problem-
A Pilot Study With Long-Term Follow-Up. Appl solving therapy for executive dysfunction after child trau-
Neuropsychol Child, 5(4), 273–282. doi:10.1080/ matic brain injury. Pediatrics, 132, e158–e166.
21622965.2015.1055563 Kurowski, B. G., Wade, S. L., Kirkwood, M. W., Brown, T.
Feeney, T. J. (2010). There’s always something that works: M., Stancin, T., & Taylor, H. G. (2014). Long-term bene-
Principles and practices of positive support for individuals fits of an early online problem-solving intervention for ex-
with traumatic brain injury and problem behaviors. ecutive dysfunction after traumatic brain injury in
Seminars in Speech and Language, 31, 145–161. children: a randomized clinical trial. JAMA Pediatrics,
unrecognised and undervalued? Neuropsychological acquired brain injuries demonstrated in a controlled study.
Rehabilitation, 19, 904–927. doi: 10.1080/ Brain Injury, 19(7), 511–518. doi:10.1080/
09602010903040691 02699050400025224
Robinson, K. E., Kaizar, E., Catroppa, C., Godfrey, C., & van’t Hooft, I., Andersson, K., Bergman, B., Sejersen, T., von
Yeates, K. O. (2014). Systematic review and meta-analysis Wendt, L., & Bartfai, A. (2007). Sustained favorable
of cognitive interventions for children with central nervous effects of cognitive training in children with acquired brain
system disorders and neurodevelopmental disorders. injuries. NeuroRehabilitation, 22(2), 109–116.
Journal of Pediatric Psychology, 39, 846–865. doi: Wade. (2015). Long-term Behavioral Outcomes following a
10.1093/jpepsy/jsu031 Randomized Clinical Trial of Counselor-Assisted
Rubia, K. (2011). “Cool” inferior frontostriatal dysfunction Problem-Solving for Adolescents with Complicated Mild
in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder versus “hot” to Severe Traumatic Brain Injury. doi:10.1089/neu.20
ventromedial orbitofrontal-limbic dysfunction in conduct Wade, Carey, J., & Wolfe, C. R. (2006). The efficacy of an
disorder: A review. Biological Psychiatry, 69, e69–e87. online cognitive-behavioral family intervention in improv-
Yeates, K. O., Swift, E., Taylor, H. G., Wade, S. L., Drotar, rehabilitation: a review and implications for intervention.
D., Stancin, T., & Minich, N. (2004). Short- and long- Pediatric Rehabilitation, 5, 51–70.
term social outcomes following pediatric traumatic brain Zelazo, P. D., & Carlson, S. M. (2012). Hot and cool executive
injury. Journal of The International Neuropsychological function in childhood and adolescence: Development and
Society, 10, 412–426. plasticity. Child Development Perspectives, 6, 354–360.
Ylvisaker, M., & Feeney, T. (2009). Apprenticeship in self- Zelazo, P. D., Qu, L., & Kesek, A. C. (2010). Hot executive
regulation- supports and interventions for individuals with function: Emotion and the development of cognitive con-
self-regulatory impairments. Developmental trol. In S. D. Calkins, M. A. Bell, S. D. Calkins, & M. A.
Neurorehabilitation, 12, 370–379. Bell (Eds.), Child development at the intersection of emo-
Ylvisaker, M., & Feeney, T. (2002). Executive functions, tion and cognition (pp. 97–111). Washington, DC:
self-regulation, and learned optimism in pediatric American Psychological Association.