ILLUSTRATIONS OF THE IPA
Cwyzhy Abkhaz
Samuel Andersson
Yale University
samuel. andersson@yale.edu
Bert Vaux
Cambridge University
bv230@cam.ac.uk
Zihni Pysipa (Şener)/Зиҳни Ҧысиҧа (Шьенер)
In this Illustration we describe the Cwyzhy (also Tswydzhy) dialect of Abkhaz, the native language of the third author. In Cwyzhy, the language Abkhaz is called /apHsaSWa/ [»apHsQSjQ]
аҧсашәа. 1 Abkhaz (ISO-639-3 abk) belongs to the Northwest Caucasian family of languages, and the Abkhaz dialects are related as shown in (1) (adapted from Chirikba 2012: 36):
(1) Relationships between Abkhaz varieties
Cwyzhy is a sub-dialect under the Sadz node in (1). All of the language varieties dominated
by the ‘Proto-Abkhaz’ node are mutually intelligible (Chirikba 1996b: ii). Though T’ap’anta
and Ashkharywa are spoken by a politically distinct Abaza people who live in the North
Caucasian republic of Karachaj–Cherkessia (ibid.: ii), linguists who work on Abkhaz (e.g.
Chirikba 1999, 2003; Hewitt 2006) generally consider them to be varieties of Abkhaz.
Abkhaz proper (i.e. all of the varieties under the node labelled ‘Southern dialects’
in (1), save for Ashkharywa) has four main dialects: Bzyp, Abzhywa, Sadz, and Ahchypsy
(Chirikba 1996a). The two literary dialects, Bzyp and Abzhywa, are relatively well-studied
(see Uslar 1887, Bgazhba 1964 on Bzyp; Aristava et al. 1968, Hewitt 1979, 1989, 1999, 2010,
Arsthaa & Chkadua 2002, Chirikba 2003, and Jakovlev 2006 on Abzhywa; Genko 1957,
1
Throughout this article we present Cwyzhy forms where possible in the order (i) /broad transcription/
– (ii) [narrow transcription] – (iii) Cwyzhy orthography employed by Mr. Pysipa. All glosses are based
on Mr. Pysipa’s variety, and sometimes differ significantly from the meanings of the same forms in the
standard dialects.
Journal of the International Phonetic Association, page 1 of 21
doi:10.1017/S0025100320000390
© The Author(s), 2021. Published by Cambridge University Press on
behalf of the International Phonetic Association
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. 31 Mar 2021 at 10:13:29, subject to the Cambridge Core terms of use.
2
Journal of the International Phonetic Association: Illustrations of the IPA
Chirikba 1994, 1996b, 2012 on the Abkhaz varieties collectively). Sadz has received limited
scholarly attention to date (Chirikba 1994, 1996a, b, 1997, 2014, to appear; Vaux & Pysipa
1997; Kilba 2000, 2012). According to Chirikba (1996a: 68 and to appear: 2), Sadz has two
subdialects, Khaltsys and Cwyzhy; the latter is the focus of the present article. Mr. Pysipa
mentions two other Sadz varieties surviving in Turkey, C’abal and Ahchypsy; Chirikba (2003:
ii) considers the former to be an archaic form of Abzhywa and the latter to form a dialect
branch separate from Sadz, as depicted in (1). In what follows, ‘Cwyzhy’ refers specifically
to the Cwyzhy dialect spoken by Mr. Pysipa unless stated otherwise.
Cwyzhy was originally spoken along the Kudepsta River, just north of the present border between Abkhazia and Russia (Chirikba 1996a: 67). After the exodus from the Russian
Empire to Anatolia in the 1860s, Cwyzhy was spoken until recently in three villages near
Bilecik in northwestern Turkey: Elmabaháe, Künceğiz, and Hasandere (Chirikba 1996a: 69).
Figure 1 shows where these three villages are located.
Figure 1 (Colour online) Map of villages in which Cwyzhy is spoken.
According to Mr. Pysipa, in 1993 Elmabaháe had 20 speakers of Cwyzhy and Künceğiz
65; Chirikba 1996a states that Elmabaháe no longer contains any Cwyzhy speakers.
Mr. Pysipa’s mother hails from Elmabaháe, and his father from Künceğiz; Mr. Pysipa himself
was born in 1954 and moved to the United States in 1990 before returning to Turkey in 2004.
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. 31 Mar 2021 at 10:13:29, subject to the Cambridge Core terms of use.
Samuel Andersson, Bert Vaux & Zihni Pysipa: Cwyzhy Abkhaz
3
Consonants
kʰʲ ɡʲ
Plain
tsʰ dz
Labialized
tsʰʷ dzʷ
Palatalized
Plain
Ejective
tʃʰ dʒ
tʃʰʲ dʒʲ
pʼ
Labialized
tʼ
kʼ
qʼ
tʷʼ
kʷʼ
qʷʼ
kʲʼ
qʲʼ
Palatalized
Ejective
affricate
Plain
tsʼ
Labialized
tsʷʼ
tʃʼ
tʃ ʲʼ
Palatalized
Nasal
m
n
ɾ
Tap
f
Plain
Fricative
v
s
z
sʷ zʷ
Labialized
Palatalized
Approximant
Lateral
approximant
Pharyngeal
kʰʷ ɡʷ
Palatalized
Affricate
Uvular
kʰ ɡ
d
tʰʷ dʷ
Labialized
Velar
tʰ
Palatal
pʰ b
Postalveolar
Dental
Plosive
Labiodental
Bilabial
Plain
w
ʃ
ʒ
χ
ʁ
ʃʷ ʒʷ
χʷ ʁʷ
ʃʲ
χʲ
ʒʲ
ħ
ħʷ
ʁʲ
j
l
Below we illustrate near-minimal pairs for consonants in the frame /C0C(C)(C)/. Because
/0/ surfaces as [a] after /Â/, this consonant is shown with a following [a] instead. As the surface
representations demonstrate, the vowel /0/ has many allophones. The allophony is governed
by neighboring consonants and discussed further in the section Vowels. For all of the words
below, the suffix /-k'/ is the indefinite article. Some of these forms may be loans from standard
Abkhaz in Mr. Pysipa’s speech, and may differ slightly from the corresponding forms in other
varieties of Abkhaz (Chirikba, p.c., Hewitt p.c.). Cwyzhy has a larger consonant inventory
than Abzhywa due to additional coronal contrasts. For example, Abzhywa does not contrast
/sW SW/ as Cwyzhy does (Hewitt 1979). The larger coronal inventory is shared with Bzyp
(Chirikba 2003). Chirikba (2014: 298) states that Cwyzhy also possesses pharyngealized
voiceless uvular fricatives /X'/ and /XW'/, but Mr. Pysipa’s variety lacks these. Apart from
this the Cwyzhy consonants presented here are identical to those Chirikba (1996a) describes
for Sadz dialects.
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. 31 Mar 2021 at 10:13:29, subject to the Cambridge Core terms of use.
4
Journal of the International Phonetic Association: Illustrations of the IPA
BROAD
NARROW
SPELLING
TRANSLATION
pʰətʰ-k’
pʰətʰ
ҧыҭк
a little bit
bəɾɡ-k’
bəɾɡk’
быргк
a respected old man
p’əɾtʃ’-k’
p’əɾtʃ’
пырҿк
a chewing sound
məskʰʲ-k’
məskʰʲk’
мысқьк
a musk, good-smelling soap
fə-k’
fək’
фык
a lightning bolt
və-k’
vək’
вык
a letter v
tʰəʁʲ-k’
tʰɪʁ̞ʲk’
ҭыҕьк
a male goat
dəd-k’
dəd
дыдк
a thunderclap
tsʰəpʰχʲ-k’
tsʰəpʰχʲk’
цыҧxьк
a spark
dzə-k’
dzək’
ӡык
a (unit, e.g. cup, of) water
t’ə-k’
t’ək’
тык
an owl
ts’ədz-k’
ts’ədzk’
ҵыӡк
a louse
nəs-k’
nəsk’
ныск
a piece of evidence
ɾəmdz-k’
ɾəmdzk’
рымӡк
a pitcher, a (unit of) urine
sə-k’
tsʰək’
сык
a (unit of) snow
zə-k’
zək’
зык
a gall bladder
ləm-k’
лымк
a lion
tʰʷə-k’
ɫəmk’
꤮
tpʰək’
ҭәык
a full one
dʷə-k’
d͡bək’
дәык
a field
tsʰʷə-k’
tɕʰᶣək’
цәык
a bull
dzʷə-k’
ӡәык
a person
tʷ’ə-k’
dʑᶣək’
꤮
tp’ək’
тәык
a slave
tsʷ’ə-k’
tɕᶣ’ək’
ҵәык
a tip, point, skewer
sʷə-k’
ɕᶣək’
шәык
a freezing, a frostbitten one
zʷə-k’
ʑᶣək’
жәык
an old thing
tʃʰə-k’
tʃʰək’
ҽык
a horse
dʒə-k’
dʒək’
џык
a piece of dirt
tʃ’ətsʰ-k’
tʃ’ətsʰk’
ҿыцк
a new one
ʃədz-k’
ʃədzk’
шыӡк
an (outbreak of) typhoid fever
ʒə-k’
ʒək’
жык
an excavation
ʃʷə-k’
ʃᶣʏk’
шәык
a bruise
ʒʷə-k’
ʒᶣək’
жәык
a cow
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. 31 Mar 2021 at 10:13:29, subject to the Cambridge Core terms of use.
Samuel Andersson, Bert Vaux & Zihni Pysipa: Cwyzhy Abkhaz
tʃʰʲəɡʷ-k’
tʃʰʲɪɡʷk’
чыгәк
a haystack
dʒʲə-k’
dʒʲɪk’
џьык
an oak tree
tʃ ʲ’əɡʷ-k’
tʃʲ’ɪɡʷk’
ҷыгәк
a well-organized person
ʃʲə-k’
ʃʲək’
шьык
a shushing
ʒʲə-k’
ʒʲək’
жьык
a piece of meat
jə-ˈχa
jɪˈχa
иыха
his head
ɥə-k’
ɥʏk’
ҩык
a wine
kʰəzʷ-k’
kʰɪʑᶣk’
қыжәк
a male sheep
ɡə-k’
ɡək’
гык
a deficiency
k’ə-k’
k’ək’
кык
a one
kʰʷətsʰ-k’
kʰʷɵtsʰk’
қәыцк
a wiping out/off
ɡʷə-k’
ɡʷək’
гәык
a heart
kʷ’əχʲ-k’
kʷ’ɪχʲk’
кәыхьк
cold leftover food, a cold person
wə-k’
wɵk’
уык
a tall one
kʰʲəʃʷ-k’
kʰʲɪʃᶣk’
қьышәк
a lip
ɡʲə
ɡʲɪ
гьы
and, also, even
kʲ’əb-k’
kʲ’ɪbk’
кьыбк
a ladder
q’əz-k’
q’əzk’
ҟызк
a goose
χə-k’
χək’
хык
a bullet
ʁəʁ-k’
ʁəʁk’
ҕыҕк
a dry brittle thing
qʷ’əd-k’
qʷ’ɵdk’
ҟәыдк
a bean
χʷə-k’
χʷɵk’
хәык
a meal
ʁʷə-k’
ʁʷɵk’
ҕәык
a board
qʲ’ə-k’
qʲ’ɪk’
ҟьык
a turd, pile of shit, shitty person
χʲə-k’
χʲɪk’
хьык
a gold one
ʁʲə-k’
ʁʲɪk’
ҕьык
a very fast one
ħəˈbəq’awa
ħaˈbəq’oː
ҳабыҟoy
we are
ħʷə-k’
ħᶣək’
ҳәык
a curled one
5
Realization of consonants
In word-initial position the voiced stops to our ears sound slightly imploded in Mr. Pysipa’s
speech, and the non-ejective voiceless stops and affricates are heavily aspirated. In word-final
position, voiced stops may be devoiced and aspirated, so that /a-kHalbad/ ақалбад ‘sock’ can
surface as [akHaë»batH]. Some final stops, particularly ejectives, may have a relatively weak
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. 31 Mar 2021 at 10:13:29, subject to the Cambridge Core terms of use.
6
Journal of the International Phonetic Association: Illustrations of the IPA
final release (noted for Sadz dialects by Chirikba 1996a: 67). The labialized anterior fricatives
and affricates, /sW zW tsHW tsW' dzW/, are slightly palatal, as seen in the transcriptions above.
Mr. Pysipa’s /R/ has a degree of pharyngealization, and may also surface as an approximant,
especially when geminated. The fricatives which we transcribe /S SJ/ for Cwyzhy Abkhaz
correspond to /ß S/ respectively in the literary Abzhywa dialect. Unlike Abzhywa, Cwyzhy
does not have any appreciable retroflexion in either of these consonants.
Abkhaz has long consonant clusters, which we illustrate in the spectrograms and waveforms below with CCC clusters consisting only of obstruents. They are representative of a
general pattern in the language where each consonant is released separately, and consonants
are not typically elided in these circumstances. However, in fast speech, there may sometimes
be no acoustic evidence for particular consonants. Examples can be seen in the narrow transcriptions in the list of minimal pairs above. The absence of acoustic evidence does not imply
that no articulatory movements for the relevant consonants are being performed.
The waveforms in Figures 2 and 3 show that compared to aspirated stops, ejectives
typically have a louder and shorter release burst.
Labialization
Labialization has three different realizations in Cwyzhy:
i. The labialized coronal stops, /tHW dW tW'/, are coarticulated with a labial stop, as in
꤮
/tW'0-k'/ тәык ‘a slave’, realized as [tp'0k'].
ii. The labialized coronal fricatives and affricates, /sW zW SW ZW tsHW dzW tsW'/, and /ÂW/, have
a front rounded secondary articulation, as in /zW0-k'/ жәык ‘an old thing’, realized as
[Ôj0k'].
iii. All other labialized segments have a back rounded secondary articulation, as in
/qW'0d-k'/ ҟәыдк ‘a bean’, realized as [qW'8dk'].
A fourth type of labialization, labiodentalization, is found in some varieties (see Catford
1972, Chirikba, to appear), but is absent from Mr. Pysipa’s speech.
Labial–coronal double articulations as shown in (i) are extremely rare cross-linguistically,
and have previously been hypothesized not to exist (Maddieson 1983, 1987). Ladefoged &
Maddieson (1996: 343–348), however, argue that true labial–coronal sounds occur either
allophonically or phonemically in a handful of languages. Concerning the Caucasian cases,
including Abkhaz, they say that ‘it might be more justifiable to consider this gesture as phonetically a secondary articulation’ (Ladefoged & Maddieson 1996: 344), a conclusion that we
do not think follows from the descriptions cited. Catford is explicit that these stops involve
‘complete labial closure’ (Catford 1972: 681), and the closure is also described as ‘complete’
by Hewitt (1979: 256). As the degree of stricture is the same at labial and coronal places of
articulation, we feel that the term ‘double articulation’ is justified.
Catford does mention that the closure is between the inner surfaces of the lips
(endolabio–endolabial in Catford’s (1977) terminology), unlike for plain labial stops, where
the contact is typically between the outer surfaces (exolabio–exolabial). These descriptions
match our impression of the labial–dental stops in Cwyzhy, from video footage of Mr. Pysipa
꤮
and his mother. We illustrate the contrast between Mr. Pysipa’s [p'] and [tp'] in Figure 4.
The closure of the lips on the left appears slightly less dark. As the closure for [p'] is
between the outer surfaces, the typical dark vermilion zone of the lips is obscured. When the
inner surfaces form the closure, as on the right, the vermilion zone is more visible, leading
to a darker appearance. However, this visual evidence is also consistent with an incomplete
꤮
closure for [tp']. Articulatory work is necessary to shed additional light on exactly how [p']
꤮
and [tp'] are distinguished.
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. 31 Mar 2021 at 10:13:29, subject to the Cambridge Core terms of use.
Samuel Andersson, Bert Vaux & Zihni Pysipa: Cwyzhy Abkhaz
Figure 2 (Colour online) Waveform and spectrogram of [tsH0pHXJk'] ‘a spark’.
Figure 3 (Colour online) Waveform and spectrogram of [m0skHJk'] ‘a musk, good-smelling soap’.
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. 31 Mar 2021 at 10:13:29, subject to the Cambridge Core terms of use.
7
8
Journal of the International Phonetic Association: Illustrations of the IPA
꤮
Figure 4 (Colour online) Left: lip closure for [p'] in [»da˘jza˘p'] ‘he came’. Right: lip closure for [tp'] in
꤮
[iːuː ˈzəmɪtʃʰʲħaɾatpʼə] ‘as hard as he could’.
Consonant length and syllabicity
Unlike the literary dialects, Cwyzhy possesses a robust phonemic length contrast in
consonants:
(2)
Consonant gemination
BROAD
NARROW SPELLING TRANSLATION
/a-ʃə-ˈɾa/
[aʃəˈɾa]
/a-ʃːə-ˈɾa/ [aʃːəˈɾa]
ашыра
get angry
ашшыра
fence in, build a fence around something
Chirikba (1996a: 74; 2014: 300–302) claims that the only consonants which exist as geminates are /l pH s S SW tsH X XJ XW/. However, Mr. Pysipa’s variety features geminates of all
but 21 consonants.2 It is possible that some or all of these 21 consonants can also appear as
geminates, and merely fail to do so in the data available to us.
Geminates reduce to singletons word-initially (pace Chirikba 1996a: 74), producing
alternations like the following:
(3)
Word-initial degemination
BROAD
NARROW
SPELLING
TRANSLATION
/a-ˈlːa/
[aˈɫːa]
алла
the dog
/adʷaˈtʃʼə ˈlːakʼ əzˈbəjtʼ/ [ad͡baˈtʃʼə ˈɫakʼ əzˈbiːtʼ] Aдәаҿылaк ызбит I saw a dog
outside
2
The 21 consonants are /pʰ b tʰ d tsʰʷ dʒ pʼ m n v sʷ zʷ j kʰ ɡ ɡʲ kʲʼ qʲʼ ʁ ʁʲ ħʷ/.
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. 31 Mar 2021 at 10:13:29, subject to the Cambridge Core terms of use.
Samuel Andersson, Bert Vaux & Zihni Pysipa: Cwyzhy Abkhaz
9
In some consonant clusters, consonants may be syllabic. This applies to both sonorants and
obstruents:
(4) Syllabic consonants
BROAD
NARROW
SPELLING
TRANSLATION
/mts’-k’/
[mts’k’]
̩
мҵк
a type of fly
/ʒʲ-k’/
[ʒʲk’]
̩
жьык
a piece of meat
In words such as these, there is also the possibility of epenthesizing a schwa, which then
serves as the syllable nucleus. The same word can thus be pronounced with a syllabic
consonant at one time and with an epenthetic vowel at another (see Spruit 1986: 83):
(5) Variable epenthesis
BROAD
NARROW
SPELLING
TRANSLATION
/ʒʲ-k’/
[ʒʲək’], [ʒʲ̩ k’]
жьык
a piece of meat
Lateral allophony
Andersson (2017) reports on an acoustic study of lateral allophony in Georgian and Cwyzhy,
on which the current section builds. Variation in /l/ has not previously been reported for
Abkhaz, but impressionistically [l] and [ë] both occur. We therefore decided to study how
the backness of intervocalic /l/ is affected by the backness of preceding and following vowels. The data are taken from previous Cwyzhy recordings (Vaux & Pysipa 2020). While
the recordings do contain full sentences, most of the data consists of single words elicited
orthographically from English and Turkish translations. The style is therefore most closely
comparable to wordlist speech. Nevertheless, it is important to note that the data are less controlled than single words recorded in a carrier sentence, for example. Care must therefore be
taken when interpreting the data, which we have not been able to control for factors such as
utterance length and phrasal position.
We extracted 54 tokens of the lateral, with as many preceding and following vowel qualities as possible. Only intervocalic tokens were studied. Because of the small vowel inventory
of Abkhaz, as well as the nature of vowel coloring (see the Vowels section), it is relatively
rare to find /l/ in certain contexts. For example, the sequence [i˘li˘] requires an underlying
sequence of five segments /0jl0j/. Because of this it was necessary to include some repeated
tokens of the same word. The 54 tokens come from a set of 33 words.
Praat (version 6.0.29, Boersma & Weenink 2017) was used to analyse the recordings.
The segmentation criteria employed for the lateral are given in (7). The list draws heavily
on Skarnitzl’s (2009) work on Czech, which Andersson (2017) found to be applicable to
Georgian and Cwyzhy.
(7) Segmentation criteria for the lateral
a.
b.
c.
d.
e.
f.
weak F2 (relative to surrounding vowels)
lower intensity in higher frequency ranges (2 kHz and above)
amplitude dip in the waveform
simplification of the shape of the waveform
antiformant between 2 kHz and 3 kHz
visible release of tongue tip contact
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. 31 Mar 2021 at 10:13:29, subject to the Cambridge Core terms of use.
10
Journal of the International Phonetic Association: Illustrations of the IPA
In some cases, these criteria did not clearly identify the beginning and end points of the
lateral. In such cases, a conservative segmentation was chosen, giving the lateral a shorter
duration rather than a longer one. The surrounding vowels were segmented using standard
segmentation criteria (Peterson & Lehiste 1960).
The measure chosen for backness was F2 at the midpoint of the lateral (see Recasens,
Fontdevila, & Dolors Pallarés 1995, Andrade 1999, Carter & Local 2007, Yuan & Liberman
2009). For the surrounding vowels, F2 at the midpoint was again used. Measurements were
made automatically in Praat. Each token was inspected manually to confirm that Praat’s formant tracking was accurate. Where it was not, the formant tracking settings were adjusted
to obtain an accurate automatic measurement, following the methodology used by Frisch &
Wodzinski (2016).
It is clear that there are some tokens with relatively clear [l] (brighter shade), especially
when preceding and following vowels are front (top right of Figure 5). Similarly, when both
preceding and following vowels are back (bottom left of Figure 5), a more velarized [ë]
(darker shade) appears. Example waveforms and spectrograms illustrating clear and velarized
laterals are shown in Figures 6 and 7.
Figure 5 F2 (Bark) of the lateral (color gradient) by F2 (Bark) of preceding (x-axis) and following (y-axis) vowels. Figure made in
R (R Core Team 2019) with a modified version of the function CGSPlot from Brinda (2020).
Vowels
Varieties of Abkhaz and other Northwest Caucasian languages are usually analysed with two
underlying vowel phonemes, viz. /a/ and /0/ (see Spruit 1986: 81–82 for discussion), and
Cwyzhy is no different in this respect (see Chirikba 1996a on Sadz dialects generally). If
unaffected by neighboring consonants, these two vowels surface as [a] and [0] (see below for
more detailed phonetic data).
However, each vowel typically acquires the additional vocalic features [high], [back], [round]
from the features of the immediately preceding and following consonants, if they are present.
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. 31 Mar 2021 at 10:13:29, subject to the Cambridge Core terms of use.
Samuel Andersson, Bert Vaux & Zihni Pysipa: Cwyzhy Abkhaz
11
Figure 6 (Colour online) Spectrogram of a velarized lateral between central vowels in [a˘»g0ëaRa] ‘to stop’. The duration of the
[0ëa] sequence is 266 ms.
Figure 7 (Colour online) Spectrogram of a clear, non-velarized lateral between front vowels in [a»nejli˘k'a˘] ‘the message’.
The duration of the [e˘li˘] sequence is 302 ms.
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. 31 Mar 2021 at 10:13:29, subject to the Cambridge Core terms of use.
12
Journal of the International Phonetic Association: Illustrations of the IPA
This gives rise to a much larger inventory of vowels on the surface. Some works do not transcribe these allophones (e.g. Spruit 1986), but where details are given (e.g. Hewitt 2010), they
generally agree with what we present for Cwyzhy below. Very similar patterns of allophony
can also be found in other Northwest Caucasian languages (e.g. Kabardian; Kuipers 1960,
Allen 1965, Anderson 1978, Choi 1991, Colarusso 1992, Wood 1994).
BROAD
NARROW
SPELLING
TRANSLATION
/wəj/
[wiː]
yыи
3.SG PERSONAL/DEMONSTR. PRONOUN
/ˈsə-χʲədz/
[ˈsɪχ ʲIdz]
cыxьыӡ
my name
/aˈkʼːə-ɥ-ba/
[aˈkʼːyːbæ]
aккыҩбa
a few, some
/tsʷʼaˈɥə/
[tɕᶣʼæˈɥY]
ҵәaҩы
curve, turn
/ajˈħa/
[ejˈħa]
eиҳa
more
/ˈtsʰəpʰχʲadza/
[ˈtsʰəpʰχʲædza] цыҧxьaӡa
per …, for each …
/ˈa-χaɾa/
[ˈɑχɑɾɑ]
axapa
far
/aχʷ ʃaˈqʼawzj/
[aχʷ ʃaˈq’ɔːzɪ]
ахә шaҟayзи how much is the price?
/ˈa-χʷaʃʷ/
[ˈaχʷɔɔʃᶣ]
axәaшә
medicine
cыгәaҟoyт
I’m in a hurry
cҭaxәыyп
I want, I want it/them
/sə-ˈɡʷaqʼa-wa-jtʼ/ [səˈɡʷaqʼoːtʼ]
/s-tʰaˈχʷə-wpʼ/
[stʰaˈχʷuːpʼ]
/ɾə-tʃʰʲˈχʷəɾtʰa/
[ɾɪtʃʰʲˈχʷɵɵɾtʰa] pычxәыpҭa
their toilet
/a-aːˈɡəla-ɾa/
[aːˈɡəəɫaɾa]
to stop
aaгылapa
We extracted three tokens of each of the 13 words above from the materials in Vaux &
Pysipa 2020. These are subject to the same constraints and limitations as the lateral data
from the same source that were discussed earlier. As some vowels appear only after back
consonants, words were generally chosen which had the relevant vowel after a velar or uvular.
Occasional exceptions had to be made, either due to restrictions based on coarticulation (see
below), or because of a lack of relevant words in our materials. In almost all cases, we used
three repetitions of the same word but in some cases morphologically related forms of a
word were used to get three tokens. The vowels were segmented in Praat using standard
segmentation criteria for vowels (Peterson & Lehiste 1960). F1 and F2 were measured at the
midpoint of each vowel in Praat. All tokens were manually inspected to verify the automatic
measurements. Where errors were present, the formant tracking settings were adjusted so
that an accurate automatic measurement could be made. Figure 8 shows the realization of the
surface vowels of Cwyzhy.
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. 31 Mar 2021 at 10:13:29, subject to the Cambridge Core terms of use.
Samuel Andersson, Bert Vaux & Zihni Pysipa: Cwyzhy Abkhaz
13
Figure 8 (Colour online) F1–F2 plot (Bark scale) for Cwyzhy vowels.
The relationship between the two contrastive vowels, and the thirteen realizations that we
have identified, is shown in Table 1.
Table 1 Variation in vowels conditioned by surrounding consonants.
Phoneme(s)
Allophone
Environment(s)
Adjacent to palatalized consonants
[Q]
Unstressed and word-final
[ç]
After back labialized consonants
[e]
Before /j/ when not preceded by another /a/ or a /Â/ (may result in [e˘])
/aa/
[a]
When the two /a/ phonemes are separated by a word boundary
/aw/
[ç˘]
When not preceded by another /a/ or a /Â/
/a-wa(-j)/
[o˘]
When /-wa-/ is the dynamic verb suffix, and when not preceded by /Â/
/a/
After /j/
[ R]
/0 /
Adjacent to palatalized consonants
After /w/
[8]
Adjacent to labialized consonants where labialization is realized as [W]
After /Á/
[Y]
Adjacent to labialized consonants where labialization is realized as [j]
[a]
After /Â/
/0j/
[i˘]/[R˘]
In all environments
/0w/
[u˘]/[U˘]
In all environments
/0Á/
[y˘]/[Y˘]
In all environments
Note that the allophony of Cwyzhy is variable, and as our phonetic transcriptions throughout the article suggest, for many of the given environments, the [a] and [0] qualities may also
appear.
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. 31 Mar 2021 at 10:13:29, subject to the Cambridge Core terms of use.
14
Journal of the International Phonetic Association: Illustrations of the IPA
In some cases the glides /j w Á/ appear to surface as long vowels when syllabic: [iː]/[ɪː],
[u˘]/[U˘], and [y˘]/[Y˘], respectively. For example, when /-j/ ‘and’ is attached to a consonantfinal noun, the result is a long vowel:
(8)
Apparent syllabic glides
BROAD
NARROW
SPELLING
TRANSLATION
/bəɾtʰ/
[bəɾtʰ]
Бырҭ
Bert
/bəɾtʰ-j/
[ˈbəɾtʰiː]
Бырҭи
... and Bert
However, these syllabic glides occur in precisely the environments where we would independently expect epenthetic [0] (see Spruit 1986: 83). An alternative analysis, therefore, would
derive these long vowels from the sequence [0] + glide which arises from epenthesis. Vowel
coloring by glides is independently motivated in this environment (see Table 1), and produces
the correct surface forms with long vowels.
Some may feel that a two-vowel analysis is needlessly abstract. Why, for example,
would we think that [s `»tsHo˘t'] cыцoит ‘I am going’ is really derived from /s-tsHa-»wa-jt'/
(Chirikba 1996a: 76)? It is worth pointing out that while many allophonic rules assume fairly
abstract underlying forms, vowel coloring is productive across morpheme boundaries and is
sometimes found even across word boundaries:
(9)
Justification for vowel coloring
BROAD
NARROW
SPELLING
TRANSLATION
/ˈamaɾa/
[ˈamaɾa]
амара
the sun
/ˈamaɾa-j/
[ˈamaɾej]
амараи
… and the sun
/jaˈχʲa/
[jæˈχʲæ]
иaxьa
today
/aˈɾəj jaˈχʲa jtsʰaɾˈməʃʲ/
[aˈɾɪː jæˈχʲe ͜ jtsʰaɾˈmɪʃʲ]
apи иaxьa
Will this go
иыцapмишь out today?
Some have attempted to reduce the vowel inventories of Northwest Caucasian languages
further, to one vowel (Allen 1965 on Abaza, with discussion of Kabardian and Abkhaz,
Anderson 1978 on Abaza and Kabardian), or even no vowels at all (Kuipers 1960 on
Kabardian). We use two vowel phonemes in light of minimal pairs for the two vowels, both
of which contrast with the absence of a vowel:
(10)
Minimal pairs for vowels
BROAD
NARROW
SPELLING
TRANSLATION
/ˈa-χaɾa/
[ˈaχaɾa]
axapa
far
/ˈa-χɾa/
[ˈaχɾa]
axpa
the forest (standard Abkhaz [ˈabna])
/a-ˈχʷːa/
[aˈχʷːa]
axәxәa
the peak
/a-ˈχʷːə/
[aˈχʷːɵ]
axәxәы
the feather
/a-z/
[az]
aз
brown
/a-ˈzə/
[aˈzə]
aзы
for it
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. 31 Mar 2021 at 10:13:29, subject to the Cambridge Core terms of use.
Samuel Andersson, Bert Vaux & Zihni Pysipa: Cwyzhy Abkhaz
15
For further discussion of the contrast between /0/ and the absence of a vowel, and other
ways of accounting for it, see the section on stress, below. Some argue for a third vowel
phoneme, long /a˘/, in the related Circassian languages (Appelbaum & Gordon 2013: 14 and
references therein). Cwyzhy also has a phonetic long [a˘], and there are some phonological arguments for analysing it as a single phoneme rather than a sequence /aa/. While the
sequences [aj] or [aw] do not appear because of vowel coloring, [a˘j] and [a˘w] appear freely:
(11)
Differences between long and short /a /
BROAD
/ˈamaɾa-j/
/jəsˈzaːjtʼ/
/aχʷ ʃaˈqʼawzj/
/sajˈləwkʼaːw/
NARROW
[ˈamaɾej]
[jɪsˈzaːjtʼ]
[aχʷ ʃaˈq’ɔːzɪ]
[sejˈluːkʼaːw]
SPELLING
амараи
иыcзaaит
ахә шaҟayзи
ceилыyкaay
TRANSLATION
… and the sun
it came to me
how much is the price?
did you (M.SG) understand me?
Despite this, we believe that [a˘] should be analysed as a sequence /aa/ at the phonological
level. In some cases, the underlying geminate status is clear from morphological considerations. For example, in [a˘»g0ëaRa] aan‡R˛apa ‘to stop’, the first /a/ is a separate morpheme,
which together with a suffix /Ra/ marks the verb as nonfinite. Moreover, the second /a/ of an
/aa/ sequence can bear stress independently of the first, as in /a»alag0jt'/ [a»aëagi˘t'] aa˛an‡RsT
‘. . . began.’ This form would begin with /j-/ ‘it, they’ in isolation, but when the subject of the
verb immediately precedes it, this agreement marker is not present.
Stress3
Word-level stress is contrastive in Abkhaz, as in the minimal pair in (12):
(12)
Minimal pair for stress
BROAD
NARROW SPELLING TRANSLATION
/ˈa-χʷːa/ [ˈaχʷːa]
ахәхәа
the ash(es), the handle
(Abzhywa /a-χʷa/ has only the first meaning;
‘the handle’ is a-χʷə́)
/a-ˈχʷːa/ [aˈχʷːa]
ахәхәа
the peak
(Abzhywa /a-χʷə/ ‘the top’(Hewitt, p.c.),
‘the hill’ (Chirikba, p.c.))
Stress in Abkhaz has not previously been studied acoustically, although intensity is claimed
to be one important cue (Arshba 1979: 7). The spectrograms in Figure 9 show a near-minimal
pair for stress, where the stressed syllable has higher pitch and intensity. Stressed vowels also
appear to be slightly longer. Future work studying stress more systematically is needed to test
how representative these patterns are of the language.
3
In addition to the sources cited in this section, treatments of Abkhaz stress include Arshba 1979; Dybo
1989, 2000, 2007; Trigo 1992, and Yanagisawa 2000.
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. 31 Mar 2021 at 10:13:29, subject to the Cambridge Core terms of use.
16
Journal of the International Phonetic Association: Illustrations of the IPA
Figure 9 (Colour online) Initial stress on [»ala] ‘the eye’ (left), and final stress on [a»l˘a] ‘the dog’ (right). Pitch (100–175 Hz)
is shown in blue, and intensity (50–75 dB) in yellow.
Segments in Abkhaz are underlyingly specified as either accented (marked with an underscore) or unaccented (Kathman 1992: 211).4 The stress is then determined in most cases by
Dybo’s Rule (Dybo 1977). In Spruit’s (1986) formulation, Dybo’s Rule says: Assign word
stress to the leftmost accented segment not immediately followed by another accented segment. If a string contains no such segments (either because all segments are accented, or
because none are), assign final stress.
For example, in [»aXW˘a] ‘the ash(es)’ above, the root /XW˘a/ contains no lexical accents.
The prefix /a-/ is accented, so it is the leftmost accented segment not followed by an unaccented segment. By Dybo’s Rule, the prefix is therefore stressed. By contrast, the vowel and
consonant in the root /XW˘a/ ‘peak’ bear lexical accents. Since the word /a-XW˘a/ ‘the peak’
contains only accented segments, the word has final stress: [a»XW:a].
Lexical accent and stress are relevant to the status of underlying /0/ in Abkhaz. Consider
the minimal pair given for the /0/–# contrast in (10) above, repeated here for convenience:
(13) /ə/– Ø
BROAD
NARROW
SPELLING
TRANSLATION
/a-z/
[az]
aз
brown
/a-ˈzə/
[aˈzə]
aзы
for it
Note that the second word, unlike the word for ‘brown’, has stress on the schwa rather than the
[a]. On an analysis that takes lexical accent into account, this is not a coincidence. For Spruit
4
The accented segments have also been called ‘dominant’, and the unaccented ones ‘recessive’ (Hewitt
2010: 14, following Spruit 1986). As far as we can tell, these terms are used interchangeably for
Abkhaz, and do not denote different lexical specifications. The terminology thus contrasts with that
used elsewhere in the literature on phonetics and phonology, e.g. for Tokyo Japanese, argued to have
both dominant accented and dominant unaccented suffixes (Alderete 2001). In this article we only mark
lexical accents (with an underscore) where relevant to the point at hand.
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. 31 Mar 2021 at 10:13:29, subject to the Cambridge Core terms of use.
Samuel Andersson, Bert Vaux & Zihni Pysipa: Cwyzhy Abkhaz
17
(1986), both words end underlyingly in /z/. The difference between the two lies in lexical
accent: [a»z0] ‘for it’ has two accents, /a-z/, and the accented /z/ triggers schwa epenthesis.
[az] ‘brown’ has its only accent on the prefix, /a-z/, so there is no epenthesis. In our Cwyzhy
data, all /0/– # minimal pairs differ in stress in this way, which might make it possible to
analyse the language with only one underlying vowel. Whether or not such an analysis is
viable depends on the status of unstressed schwa. Yanagisawa (2005) argues that unstressed
schwas are also predictable from general phonotactic constraints, while Vaux & Samuels
(2018) argue that they are not fully predictable in Cwyzhy.
Transcription of recorded passage
We provide phonemic, phonetic, and orthographic transcriptions of ‘The North Wind and the
Sun’ in the Cwyzhy dialect, translated from the Turkish version (Zimmer & Orgun 1992) by
Mr. Pysipa. ‘The North Wind and the Sun’ has also been translated into the literary Abzhywa
dialect by Zaira Khiba (published by Hewitt 1978). In the phonemic transcription, as elsewhere in this article, we treat /0/ as underlying even in cases where it could be analysed as
epenthetic.
As is common in Abkhaz storytelling, this story uses evidential endings on verbs. We
have not translated the evidential meaning here, retaining the traditional English translation
of the story.
Phonemic transcription
apʰʃat’lakʷ’j amaɾaj ħawa ɾajħa amətʃʰʲ əzmada ħʷa əʃajmaɾk’ːawaz jəwap’a əʃʷənə
nəqʷ’aɥək’ daajzaap’ jakʰʷʃaħatʰχəjt’ ɾəwa ɾapʰχʲa anəqʷ’aɥə jəwap’a əjʃʷəzəɾχəz
ɾajħa amətʃʰʲ ʃəjmaw azə. nas apʰʃat’lakʷ’ aalaɡəjt’ asɾa əjəwzəmətʃʰʲħaɾatʷ’ə.
apʰʃat’lakʷ’ asəpʰχʲadza anəqʷ’aɥə jəwap’a ajħaɡʲə jətʃʰalajħʷawa dalaɡazaap’.
ats’əχʷazə apʰʃat’lakʷ’ aqʷ’əts’zaap’ asɾa. wəj aʃʲtʰaχʲ amaɾa jək’aχːaw awaɥə
jəkʰəpʰχaw jalaɡazaap’. anəqʷ’aɥə ɡʲə jəwap’a aajʃʷəjχzaap’. abɾəj ala apʰʃat’lakʷ’
akʰʷʃaħatʰχazaap’ amaɾa amətʃʰʲ ɾajħa jəʃəʁʷːawa ala.
Phonetic transcription
apʰˈʃatʼɫakʷʼɪː ˈamaɾej ‖ ˈħawa ɾejħa ˈʔamɪtʃʰʲ ɪzˈmadæ ħᶣæ ‖ ɪˈʃejmaɾkʼːɔːz̥ ‖ jɪˈwapʼa
ɪʃᶣʏˈnə ‖ ˈnɵqʷʼaɥʏkʼ ˈdaːjzaːpʼ ‖ ˈjakʰʷʃaħatʰχɪːtʼ ‖ ˈɾʊːa ˈɾapʰχʲæ aˈnɵqʷɔɥɪ jɪˈwapʼæ
iːʃᶣʏˈzəɾχəz̥ ‖ ɾejˈħ‿amɪtʃʰʲ ˈʃiːmɔː azə ‖ ˈnas apʰˈʃatʼɫakʷʼ aˈaɫaɡiːtʼ asɾæ
꤮
iːuːˈzəmɪtʃʰʲħaɾtpaʼə ‖ apʰˈʃatʼɫakʷʼ ˈasəpʰχʲædzæ aˈnɵqʷʼɔɥʏ jɪˈwapʼa ejħaˈjɪ jɪˈtʃʰaɫejħᶣɔː
ˈdaɫaɡazaːpʼ ‖ aˈtsʼɵχʷazə apʰˈʃatʼɫakʷʼ aˈqʷʼɵtsʼzaːpʼ asɾæ ‖ ˈwɪː aʃʲtʰæχʲ ‖ ˈamaɾa ‖
jɪkʼaˈχːɔː ‖ awɔˈɥʏ ˈjɪkʰəpʰχɔː ˈjaɫaɡazaːpʼ ‖ aˈnɵqʷʼaɥʏ ɡʲɪ jɪˈwapʼ‿aːj ʃᶣiːʁzaːpʼ‖ aˈbɾiː
æɫæ ‖ apʰˈʃatʼɫakʷʼ ‖ ˈakʰʷʃaħatʰχazaːpʼ ‖ ˈamaɾa ˈamɪtʃʰʲɾejˈħa jɪʃɵʁʷːɔː aɫa
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. 31 Mar 2021 at 10:13:29, subject to the Cambridge Core terms of use.
18
Journal of the International Phonetic Association: Illustrations of the IPA
Orthographic version
Аҧшатлакәи амараи «Ҳауа реиҳа амыч иызмада ҳәа» иышеимарккоуз иыуапа
ишәыны ныҟәаҩык дааизаап. Иақәшаҳаҭхыит: «Рыуа раҧхьа аныҟәаҩы иыуапа
иыишәызырхыз» реиҳа амыч шиымоу азы. Нас, аҧшатлакә аалагыит асра,
иыыузымыҷҳаратәы. Аҧшатлакә асыҧхьаӡа аныҟәаҩы иыуапа аиҳагьы
иыҽалаиҳәоу далагазаап. Аҵыхәазы аҧшатлакә аҟәыҵзаап асра. Уыи ашьҭахь
амара иыкаххоу ауаҩы иықәыҧхоу иалагазаап. Аныҟәаҩыгь иыуапа ааишәихзаап.
Абри ала аҧшатлакә ақәшаҳаҭхазаап амара амыч реиҳа иышыҕәҕәоу ала.
English translation
The North Wind and the Sun were disputing which was the stronger, when a traveler came
along wrapped in a warm cloak. They agreed that the one who first succeeded in making
the traveler take his cloak off should be considered stronger than the other. Then the North
Wind blew as hard as he could, but the more he blew the more closely did the traveler fold his
cloak around him; and at last the North Wind gave up the attempt. Then the Sun shined out
warmly, and immediately the traveler took off his cloak. And so the North Wind was obliged
to confess that the Sun was the stronger of the two.
Acknowledgements
The authors would like to thank James Clackson, Patrick Taylor, and especially Slava Chirikba,
George Hewitt, and Zaira Khiba for helpful comments on earlier drafts of this article. We also thank
the reviewers for their comments, and the editorial team for their work.
We dedicate this study to the memory of Mr. Pysipa, who worked tirelessly and generously on
documenting his language with Ken Hale and us for more than twenty-five years until his unexpected
passing at the beginning of 2018.
Supplementary material
To view supplementary material for this article (including audio files to accompany the language
examples), please visit https://doi.org/10.1017/S0025100320000390.
References
Alderete, John. 2001. Dominance effects as transderivational anti-faithfulness. Phonology 18(2), 201–253.
Allen, William Sidney. 1965. On one-vowel systems. Lingua 13, 111–124.
Anderson, Stephen R. 1978. Syllables, segments, and the Northwest Caucasian languages. In Alan Bell &
Joan B. Hooper (eds.), Syllables and segments, 47–58. Amsterdam: North Holland.
Andersson, Samuel. 2017. Lateral allophony in Georgian and Cwyzhy Abkhaz. Ms., Yale University.
Andrade, Amhlia. 1999. On /l/ velarization in European Portuguese. In John J. Ohala, Yoko Hasegawa,
Manjari Ohala, Daniel Granville & Ashlee C. Bailey (eds.), 14th International Congress of Phonetic
Sciences (ICPhS XIV), Berkeley, University of California, vol. 1, 543–546.
Appelbaum, Ayla & Matthew Gordon. 2013. A comparative phonetic study of the Circassian languages.
In Chundra Cathcart, Shinae Kang & Clare S. Sandy (eds.), Proceedings of the 37th Annual Meeting
of the Berkeley Linguistics Society: Special Session on Languages of the Caucasus (BLS 37), 3–17.
Retrieved from https://escholarship.org/uc/item/16x641q4 (24 November 2020).
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. 31 Mar 2021 at 10:13:29, subject to the Cambridge Core terms of use.
Samuel Andersson, Bert Vaux & Zihni Pysipa: Cwyzhy Abkhaz
19
Aristava, Shota, Khukhut Bgazhba, Mirian Tsikolia, Lidia Chkadua & Konstantin Shakryl. 1968.
Grammatika abxazskogo jazyka: fonetika i morfologija [A grammar of the Abkhaz language: Phonetics
and morphology]. Sukhumi: Alashara.
Arshba, Nelli. 1979. Dinamicheskoje udarenie i redukcija glasnyx v abxazskom jazyke [Dynamic stress
and vowel reduction in Abkhaz]. Tbilisi: Mecniereba.
Arsthaa, Shota & L. Chkadua. 2002. Aphsua literaturatw byzshwa agrammatika [A grammar of the
Abkhaz literary language]. Akwa: [n.p.]
Bgazhba, Xuxut Solomonovich. 1964. Bzybskij dialekt abxazskogo jazyka [The Bzyb dialect of the
Abkhaz language]. Tbilisi: Izdatel’stvo Akademij Nauk Gruzinskoj SSR.
Boersma, Paul & David Weenink. 2017. Praat: Doing phonetics by computer. Version 6.0.29.
Brinda, William D. 2020. Data analysis with R: The big picture. Online resource available at
http://quantitations.com/static/books/Data-Analysis-with-R.pdf.
Carter, Paul & John Local. 2007. F2 variation in Newcastle and Leeds English liquid systems. Journal of
the International Phonetic Association 37(2), 183–199.
Catford, J. C. 1972. Labialization in Caucasian languages, with special reference to Abkhaz. In André
Rigault & René Charbonneau (eds.), Proceedings of the Seventh International Congress of Phonetic
Sciences (ICPhS VII), 679–682. Mouton: The Hague.
Catford, J. C. 1977. Fundamental problems in phonetics. Bloomington, IN: Indiana University Press.
Chirikba, Viacheslav. 1994. Abxazo-abazinskaja dialektologija i rekonstrukcija praabxazskoj fonemnoj
sistemy [Abkhaz-Abaza dialectology and the reconstruction of the Proto-Abkhaz phonemic system].
In Asker Gadagatl & Ruslan Khadzhebiekov (eds.), Nartskij epos i kavkazskoe jazykoznanie (The
Nart Epic and Caucasology). Societas Caucasologica Europaea, Vith Colloquium, 23–25 June 1992,
Maikop, 331–342. Maikop: Adygheian Academy of Sciences.
Chirikba, Viacheslav. 1996a. Sadz, an Abkhaz dialect in Turkey. In Howard Aronson (ed.), NSL 8:
Linguistic Studies in the Non-Slavic Languages of the Commonwealth of Independent States and the
Baltic Republics, 67–81. Chicago, IL: Chicago Linguistic Society.
Chirikba, Viacheslav. 1996b. A dictionary of Common Abkhaz. Leiden: [n.p.].
Chirikba, Viacheslav. 1997. Distribution of Abkhaz dialects in Turkey. In Ayşe Sumru Özsoy (ed.),
Proceedings of the Conference on Northwest Caucasian Linguistics, 10–12 October 1994 (Studia
Caucasologica III), 63–88. Oslo: Novum forlag.
Chirikba, Viacheslav. 1999. The West Caucasian material in The North Caucasian Etymological
Dictionary by S. L. Nikolayev & S. A. Starostin. In Helma van den Berg (ed.), Studies in Caucasian linguistics: Selected papers of the Eighth Caucasian Colloquium, 152–170. Leiden: CNWS Publications.
Chirikba, Viacheslav. 2003. Abkhaz. Munich: Lincom Europa.
Chirikba, Viacheslav. 2012. Abxazskij jazyk [The Abkhaz language]. In Jurij D. Anchabadze & Jurij
Argun (eds.), Abxazy: Izdanie vtoroe, ispravlennoe [Abkhaz, 2nd edn., revised], 23–39. Moskva:
Nauka.
Chirikba, Viacheslav. 2014. Sadzskij dialekt abxazskogo jazyka i ego govory [The Sadz dialect of
Abkhaz and its subdialects]. In Shalva D. Inal-Ipa (ed.), Sadzy: Istoriko-etnograficheskie ocherki [Sadz:
Historical-ethnographical essays], 2nd edn., 274–374. Sukhumi: [n.p.].
Chirikba, Viacheslav. To appear. The Sadz dialect of Abkhaz. In Yuri Koryakov & Yury Lander (eds.),
The Caucasian languages: An international handbook, vol. 1. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.
Choi, John. 1991. An acoustic study of Kabardian vowels. Journal of the International Phonetic
Association 21, 4–12.
Colarusso, John. 1992. A grammar of the Kabardian language. Calgary: University of Calgary Press.
Dybo, Vladimir. 1977. Zapadnokavkazskaja akcentnaja sistema i problema ee proisxozhdenija [The
North-West Caucasian accent system and the problem of its origin]. Konferencija “Nostraticheskie
jazyki i nostraticheskoe jazykoznanie”, tezisy dokladov [Book of abstracts from the Nostratic
Languages and Nostratic Linguistics Conference], 41–45. Moskva: Institut Slavjanovedenija i
Balkanistiki.
Dybo, Vladimir. 1989. Tipologija i rekonstrukcija paradigmaticheskix akcentnyx sistem [Typology and
reconstruction of paradigmatic accentual systems]. In Rimma Bulatova (ed.), Istoricheskaja akcentologija i sravnitel’no-istoricheskij metod [Historical accentology and the historical comparative
method], 7–145. Moscow: Nauka.
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. 31 Mar 2021 at 10:13:29, subject to the Cambridge Core terms of use.
20
Journal of the International Phonetic Association: Illustrations of the IPA
Dybo, Vladimir. 2000. Abxazskaja akcentnaja sistema [The Abkhaz accentual system]. In Vladimir A.
Dybo (ed.), Morfonologizovannye paradigmaticheskie akcentnye sistemy. T. I. Tipologija i genezis
[Morphophonologized paradigmatic accentual systems, vol. 1: Typology and genesis], 660–734.
Moscow: Jazyki slavjanskoj kultury.
Dybo, Vladimir. 2007. Zapadnokavkazskaja akcentologicheskaja rekonstrukcija [West Caucasian accentological reconstruction]. Lingvisticheskaja komparativistika v kul’turnom i istoricheskom aspektax [Comparative linguistics in cultural and historical aspects], 88–113. Moscow: Moscow State
University.
Frisch, Stefan & Sylvie Wodzinski. 2016. Velar–vowel coarticulation in a virtual target model of stop
production. Journal of Phonetics 56, 52–65.
Genko, Anatolij. 1957. Foneticheskie vzaimootnoshenija abxazskogo i abazinskogo jazykov [The interrelation between the phonetics of Abkhaz and Abaza languages]. Trudy Abxazskogo instituta jazyka,
literatury i istorii 28, 177–225.
Hewitt, Brian George. 1978. The North Wind and the Sun in literary Abkhaz. Bedi Kartlisa 36, 266–274.
Hewitt, Brian George. 1979. Abkhaz: A descriptive grammar. Amsterdam: North Holland.
Hewitt, Brian George. 1989. Abkhaz. In Brian G. Hewitt (ed.), The indigenous languages of the Caucasus,
vol. 2. Delmar & New York: Caravan.
Hewitt, Brian George. 1999. Language. In Brian G. Hewitt (ed.), The Abkhazians: A handbook, 167–175.
Richmond, England: Curzon.
Hewitt, Brian G. 2006. Abkhaz. In Keith Brown (ed.), The Elsevier encyclopedia of language &
linguistics, 2nd edn., 7–9. Oxford: Elsevier.
Hewitt, Brian George. 2010. Abkhaz: A comprehensive self-tutor. Munich: Lincom Europa.
Jakovlev, Nikolaj. 2006. Grammatika abxazskogo literaturnogo jazyka [A grammar of the Abkhaz literary
language]. Sukhumi: Alashara.
Kathman, David. 1992. Stress and accent in Abkhaz. In Germhn Westphal, Benjamin Ao & HeeRahk Chae (eds.), ESCOL ’91: Proceedings of the Eighth Eastern States Conference on Linguistics,
210–221. Columbus, OH: The Ohio State University.
Kilba, Èmma K. 2000. O dialektnoj baze rechi batumskix (ferijskix) abxazov. K probleme sadzskogo
dialekta [On the dialectal source of the speech of the Batumi (Fier) Abkhaz: On the problem of the Sadz
dialect]. Abxazovedenie: Jazyk, fol’klor, literature [Abkhaz Studies: Language, folklore, literature],
vol. 1, 39–45. Sukhumi: Alashara.
Kilba, Èmma. K. 2012. K probleme rekonstrukcii istoricheskogo sadzskogo dialekta abxazskogo jazyka
[On the problem of reconstruction of the historical Sadz dialect of the Abkhaz language]. In Denis
Chachxalia (ed.), Dzhigetskij sbornik, vol. 1: Voprosy etnokul’turnoj istorii Zapadnoj Abxazii ili
Dzhigetii [Jighet collection, vol 1: Ethnocultural issues in the history of Western Abkhazia or Jighetia],
106–113. Moscow: AKVA–Abaza.
Kuipers, Aert. 1960. Phoneme and morpheme in Kabardian. The Hague: Mouton.
Ladefoged, Peter & Ian Maddieson. 1996. The sounds of the world’s languages. Oxford & Cambridge,
MA: Blackwell.
Maddieson, Ian. 1983. The analysis of complex phonetic elements in Bura and the syllable. Studies in
African Linguistics 14, 285–310.
Maddieson, Ian. 1987. The Margi vowel system and labiocoronals. Studies in African Linguistics 18(3),
327–355.
Peterson, Gordon E. & Ilse Lehiste. 1960. Duration of syllable nuclei in English. The Journal of the
Acoustical Society of America 32(6), 693–703.
R Core Team. 2019. R: A language and environment for statistical computing. Vienna: R Foundation for
Statistical Computing. https://www.R-project.org/.
Recasens, Daniel, Jordi Fontdevila & Maria Dolors Pallarès. 1995. Velarization degree and coarticulatory
resistance for /l/ in Catalan and German. Journal of Phonetics 23, 37–52.
Skarnitzl, Radek. 2009. Challenges in segmenting the Czech lateral liquid. In Anna Esposito & Robert
Vçch (eds.), Cross-modal analysis of speech, gestures, gaze and facial expressions, 162–172. Berlin:
Springer.
Spruit, Arie. 1986. Abkhaz studies. Leiden: [n.p.].
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. 31 Mar 2021 at 10:13:29, subject to the Cambridge Core terms of use.
Samuel Andersson, Bert Vaux & Zihni Pysipa: Cwyzhy Abkhaz
21
Trigo, Loren. 1992. Abkhaz stress shift. In Brian G. Hewitt (ed.), Caucasian perspectives, 191–235.
Unterschleissheim & Munich: Lincom Europa.
Uslar, Pjotr Karlovich. 1887. Etnografija Kavkaza. Jazykoznanie. Abxazskij Jazyk [Caucasian ethnography. Linguistics: The Abkhaz language]. Tiflis: Tipografija kanceljarii Glavnonachal’stvujushchego
grazhdanskoj chastiju na Kavkaze [Printing House of the Office of the Chief Civil Unit in the
Caucasus].
Vaux, Bert & Zihni Pysipa. 1997. The C 0
Ü i´0 dialect of Abkhaz. In Susumu Kuno, Bert Vaux & Steve
Peter (eds.), Harvard Working Papers in Linguistics 6, 193–208.
Vaux, Bert & Zihni Pysipa. 2020. The Cwyzhy dialect of Abkhaz. Ms., Cambridge University.
Vaux, Bert & Bridget Samuels. 2018. Abstract underlying representations and prosodic structure. In
Diane Brentari & Jackson L. Lee (eds.), Shaping phonology, 146–181. Chicago, IL: The University of
Chicago Press.
Wood, Sidney. 1994. A spectrographic analysis of vowel allophones in Kabardian. Working Papers 42,
241–250. Lund: Lund University Department of Linguistics.
Yanagisawa, Tamio. 2000. Abkhaz verb accent: A review of V. A. ‘Dybo’s Law’ of Abkhaz accent. Nagoya
Working Papers in Linguistics 16, 41–65. Nagoya: Department of Linguistics, University of Nagoya.
Yanagisawa, Tamio. 2005. Schwa in Abkhaz. Japanese Slavic and East European Studies 26, 23–36.
Yuan, Jiahong & Mark Liberman. 2009. Investigating /l/ variation in English through forced alignment.
Proceedings of Interspeech 2009, 2215–2218. Retrieved from https://www.isca-speech.org/archive/
archive_papers/interspeech_2009/papers/i09_2215.pdf (retrieved 11 October 2020).
Zimmer, Karl & Orhan Orgun. 1992. Turkish. Journal of the International Phonetic Association 22(1–2),
43–45.
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. 31 Mar 2021 at 10:13:29, subject to the Cambridge Core terms of use.