ALLOMORPHIC VARIATION OF DEFINITE ARTICLES IN JERSEY:
A SONORITY BASED ACCOUNT
By
MIRANDA KELLY MCCARVEL
B.A. in Liberal Studies, University of Montana, Missoula, Montana, 2004
Thesis
presented in partial fulfillment of the requirements
for the degree of
Master of Arts
in Linguistics
The University of Montana
Missoula, MT
May 2010
Approved by:
Perry Brown, Associate Provost for Graduate Education
Graduate School
Dr. Mizuki Miyashita
Linguistics
Dr. Leora Bar-el
Linguistics
Dr. Naomi Shin
Modern and Classical Languages and Literatures
McCarvel, Miranda, M.A., Spring 2010
Linguistics
Allomorphic Variation of Jersey Definite Articles: a Sonority Based Account
Chairperson: Dr. Mizuki Miyashita
Allomorphic variation is a common linguistic phenomenon in Jersey (Jersey Norman
French). Definite articles in Jersey each have at least two allomorphs. The occurrence of
each allomorph has been attributed to the composition of word initial syllable following
the article (Liddicoat 1994). Instead of using a ruled-based approach, this thesis
examines the variation found among Jersey definite articles and uses sonority-based
principles to analyze the allomorphic variation. Using Jersey phonotactics, this thesis
first puts forth a Jersey specific sonority hierarchy and then utilizes that hierarchy and
principles of syllabification to syllabify phrases containing definite articles. Then using
sonority based principles, such as the Sonority Sequencing Principle and Syllable Contact
Law, this thesis analyzes the syllabified phrases. The analysis identifies the sonority
based conditions that trigger the allomorphic variation found in the data. This thesis
contributes to the field of linguistics in several ways. It supports the use of both the
Universal Sonority Hierarchy and language specific sonority hierarchies. This thesis also
supports the practice of using available data sources for analysis. The analysis of a
described but analyzed phenomenon contributes valuable information to the general
knowledge of Jersey and sonority. Finally, this thesis also serves as an important
resource for the study of Norman dialects in Europe such as Guernsey, Sark and Norman,
as Jersey is a member of this linguistic group. This thesis contributes to both the field of
Jersey linguistics and to the field of theoretical linguistics, while accounting for the
allomorphic variation of Jersey definite articles.
ii
Acknowledgements
I would like to thank my thesis chair, adviser, and friend, Dr. Mizuki Miyashita. My
exposure to phonology, and indirectly sonority, is because of her. She spent many hours
reading and editing this thesis and provided invaluable guidance throughout the research
and writing of this thesis. I also extend my appreciation and gratitude to Dr. Leora Barel. Not only did she agree to sit on my committee, she has provided me with excellent
instruction and guidance as my professor and advisor. I never would have experienced
the joy of linguistics if it had not been for Dr. Bar-el. I thank, also, Dr. Naomi Shin for
providing superb insight as a member of my committee and for her encouragement. I am
also deeply indebted to Drs. Tully Thibeau and Irene Appelbaum for their outstanding
instruction and assistance during my linguistic education. I would also like to extend my
gratitude to several of my peers. First to Ryan Denzer-King, who not only read my thesis
and provided insightful and thought provoking feedback, but who also has provided
many hours of interesting linguistic discussion throughout out our time as graduate
students. I also thank Scott Sterling for reading and commenting on my thesis. I would
like to express my gratitude to Gustavo Guajardo. He read my thesis and was the
prescriptive grammarian I needed. This thesis, and my education, would never have been
possible if it had not been for the support of my best friend, my husband, Sean McCarvel.
He has believed in my abilities even when I have faltered. He has provided the
foundation for my education. Lastly, I would like to thank my family for their love and
support.
iii
Table of Contents
Abstract
ii
Acknowledgements
iii
Section 1: Introduction
1
Section 2: Language Background
2.1 Geography
2.2 Status - Documentation and Vitality
2.3 Linguistic History
2.4 Dialect or Language
2.5 Dialects
2.6 Parlers
4
4
5
6
7
8
8
Section 3: Data Sources
3.1 Compilation of Data
10
10
Section 4: Phoneme Inventory
4.1 Consonants and Glides
4.2 Vowels and Diphthongs
12
12
13
Section 5: Syllables
5.1 Jersey Syllable Structure
5.1.1 Open Syllables
5.1.2 Closed Syllables
5.1.3 Unallowable Syllable Types
5.1.4 Consonant Cluster Avoidance Strategies
15
16
16
18
19
19
Section 6: Theoretical Background
6.1 Syllable Principles
6.1.1 Minimal Onset Satisfaction Principle
6.1.2 Maximal Onset Realisation Parameter
6.1.3 Law of Initials
6.1.4 Law of Finals
6.1.5 Romance Resyllabification Principle
6.2 Sonority Principles
6.2.1 Sonority Hierarchies
6.2.2 Sonority Sequencing Principle
6.2.3 Core Syllabification Principle
6.2.4 Syllable Contact Law
6.3 Summary
22
22
22
23
24
26
27
29
30
31
32
33
35
iv
Section 7: Jersey Sonority Hierarchy
7.1 Sonority Rankings
7.2 Factors in Determining Sonority Hierarchies
36
36
37
Section 8: Morphophonemics of Jersey Definite Articles
8.1 Masculine Definite Singular Article Allomorphs - [l] and [le]
8.2 Feminine Definite Singular Article Allomorphs - [la] and [l]
8.3 Definite Plural Article Allomorphs - [lei], [lz], and [leiz]
8.4 Data Summary
45
46
49
51
54
Section 9: Sonority Based Analysis of Definite Article Allomorphic Variation
9.1 Masculine Definite Singular Article Allomorphs - [l] and [le]
9.2 Feminine Definite Singular Article Allomorphs - [la] and [l]
9.3 Definite Plural Article Allomorphs - [lei], [lz], and [leiz]
9.5 Analysis Summary
55
55
60
63
68
Section 10: Conclusion
70
References
76
v
SECTION 1: INTRODUCTION
Definite articles in Jersey exhibit allomorphic variation. The masculine and feminine
singular article morphemes /l/ and /la/ each have two allomorphs, while the plural definite
article morpheme, /lei/, has three allomorphs. Grammars of the Jersey language describe
the variation of allomorphs and attribute the phenomenon to the phonological
composition of the word following the article, specifically whether the word begins with
a vowel, consonant, or consonant cluster (see Birt 1985 and Liddicoat 1994). While this
provides a basic, surface explanation, it does explain the underlying motivation for such
alternation of allomorphs. In order provide a theoretical explanation for the alternation of
allomorphs I posit a language specific sonority hierarchy of Jersey. Using this hierarchy
and principles of syllabifications, such as the Minimal Onset Satisfaction Principles
(Rocca 1994) and the Core Syllabification Principles (Clements 1990), I syllabify Jersey
phrases containing definite articles. I then use principles of sonority, for example the
Sonority Sequencing Principle (Clements 1990) and the Syllable Contact Law, to
illustrate how the alternation exhibited by the definite article allomorphs is predictable
when these principles are taken into account.
This thesis contributes to both the field of theoretical linguistics and to the field of Jersey
linguistics. Theoretically this thesis provides support for the use of language specific
hierarchies in the analysis of syllable structure. The absence of speaker intuitions in
syllabification can be mitigated by using a language specific sonority hierarchy in
addition to principles of sonority and syllable structure. Using the sonority hierarchy and
these principles phrases can be syllabified without the aid of speaker judgments. The
1
language specific sonority hierarchy put forth in this thesis is a finer gradation of the
Universal Sonority Hierarchy (Clements 1990). The Universal Sonority Hierarchy
captures general trends across languages, while language specific sonority hierarchies
capture the intricate phonotactic details of a language. The Jersey Sonority Hierarchy
does not contradict that Universal Sonority Hierarchy, but instead reinforces its rankings
of various classes of sounds.
This thesis also contributes to both fields in that it analyzes a previously unanalyzed
phenomenon, that of allomorphic variation of Jersey definite articles, using cross
linguistic principles. The current literature (Liddicoat 1994) describes the occurrence of
definite article variation, but does not provide an analysis or put forth any underlying
theoretical motivation. The field of Jersey linguistics has been limited to mostly
sociolinguistic and historical linguistic research. This thesis is the first in many years to
approach Jersey linguistics from the standpoint of phonological theory. It is the only
thesis I know of that uses principles of sonority to analyze Jersey phrases and posit a
Jersey Sonority Hierarchy.
This thesis is organized as follows: in Section 2 I discuss the language background of
Jersey, including where it is spoken, its status, history, and dialects. The data I use in this
thesis, along with the sources for that data, are reviewed in Section 3. I give an overview
of the phonemic inventory, including consonants, vowels, and diphthongs, in Section 4.
Syllables, syllable structure, and Jersey syllable structure are reviewed in Section 5.
Principles, parameters, and laws dealing with syllables and sonority are discussed in
2
Section 6. In Section 7, I propose a Jersey specific sonority hierarchy. I present the
morphophonemics of Jersey definite articles, examining the allomorphic variation for
each article and positing an underlying morpheme for each in Section 8. In Section 9, I
use the principles from Section 6 and the hierarchy from Section 7 to analyze the
allomorphic variation of Jersey definite articles seen in the data from Section 8. I
conclude the thesis and discuss implications in Section 10.
3
SECTION 2: LANGUAGE BACKGROUND
This section provides an overview of where Jersey is spoken, its status relative to speaker
numbers, and some of the linguistic history of Jersey. I also present the issue of whether
Jersey is a language or a dialect of French and the dialectal variation found among Jersey
and neighboring Norman languages and within Jersey itself.
2.1 Geography
Jersey is an endangered language traditionally spoken on the Island of Jersey. This
island, along with the Islands of Guernsey, Sark, Herm, and Alderney, form the Channel
Islands, which are a dependency of the United Kingdom (Jones 2001). The Channel
Islands are located off the coast of Normandy, France in the Bay of St. Malo, as shown
on the map in Figure 1.
Figure 1 Location of the Channel Islands
(http://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/File:Channel_islands_location.png)
4
The Island of Jersey is divided into parishes, which are geo-political divisions within the
island, similar to counties in the United States (Figure 2 below). Each parish has its own
distinct dialect of Jersey, which are referred to as parlers and are discussed below in
Section 2.6.
Figure 2 Parishes on the Island of Jersey
(http://user.itl.net/image/maps/jsyall.gif)
2.2 Status – Documentation and Vitality
Jersey is a fairly well documented, yet endangered language (Birt 1985; Brasseur 1977,
1978a, 1978b; Carŕ 1972; Collas nd; Jones 2001, 2003, 2005a, 2005b, 2007; Le Feuvre
1976, 1979, 1983; Le Maistre 1966, 1979a, 1979b, 1981, 1993; Liddicoat 1990, 1991,
1994; Spence 1960, 1985, 1987). According to La Societe Jersiais 1 in a 2001 census of
La Societe Jersiais is a society founded in 1873 for the preservation and encouragement of Jersey culture
and language.
1
5
the Island of Jersey, 2,674 people were fluent speakers of Jersey (societe-jersiaise.org).
This represents approximately 3% of the Island‟s population, with the majority of the
population speaking English. An earlier census in 1989 showed 5,720 speakers (Jones
2001). Despite active revitalization efforts, the Jersey speaking population is declining
(Jones 2001).
2.3 Linguistic History
Jersey, known as Jerriais [ʒɛrjei] to speakers, is also referred to in the literature as
Norman French and Jersey Norman French. Jersey should not be confused with Jersey
Legal French or Jersey French, which is the dialect of French used administratively on
the Island of Jersey for written legal documents and contracts. Historically, the original
inhabitants of the Island of Jersey probably spoke Gaulish, which was eventually
replaced by Vulgar Latin in the second century (Jones 2001). In 933 William Longsword
brought Norman French to the island after the annexation of Channel Islands into
Normandy (Liddicoat 1994). The version of Norman French brought by Longsword
evolved into the Jersey language spoken on the Island today. As a result of the rich
linguistic history of the Island, Jersey‟s lexicon contains many words of Norse, Latin, and
Gaulish origin. Though the Island of Jersey is part of the United Kingdom and has been
influenced by English, French has had a stronger cultural influence on the Island. Due to
contact with both countries and their languages, there are also a number of English and
French loanwords.
6
2.4 Dialect or Language
While Jersey is related to French, both daughters of the Northern division of the GalloRomance subfamily, there is some debate as to whether Jersey is a dialect of French or a
language unto itself. Ethnologue (Gordon 2005) and some linguists, such as Anthony
Liddicoat and N. C. W. Spence, consider Jersey a dialect of French, yet speakers and
those engaged in revitalization of Jersey, Frank Le Maistre and Paul Birt, for example,
consider Jersey to be a language distinct from French (Jones 2001). Jones (2001)
considers Jersey a language and not a dialect of French. The criterion of mutual
intelligibility is often used to distinguish a language from a dialect (Chambers and
Trudgill 2002). Social and political factors must also be taken into consideration in
distinguishing languages from dialects. For example, Norwegian and Swedish are
mutually intelligible, but are not considered dialects; instead they are viewed as
languages, just as Italian and Spanish, also mutually intelligible, are considered separate
languages (Chambers and Trudgill 2002). This distinction between dialect and language
is based on sociopolitical reasons and not linguistic reasons, such as mutual intelligibility.
The case of Jersey and French is similar. There is a certain amount of mutual
intelligibility, though phonological, morphological, semantic and syntactic differences
make Jersey harder to understand for French speakers than other dialects of French
(Liddicoat 1994). Also, much of the Jersey lexicon is not found in standard French. For
example, „young girl‟ in Jersey is hardelle [hardel] while in French it is jeune fille [ʒœn
fiːj]. Another difference between the two is that Jersey, unlike French, does not
distinguish between first person singular and plural in its pronouns. Jones (2001) argues
that for these linguistic reasons, plus sociopolitical reasons, Jersey is not a dialect of
7
French, but a language distinct from French2. After taking into account the linguistic
difference between Jersey and French, this thesis refers to Jersey as a language.
2.5 Dialects
Among the Channel Islands there are other languages that are closely related to Jersey
and are considered dialects of one another. The dialect most closely related to Jersey is
the dialect of Sark (Sercquiais), which descends from Jersey speakers who settled the
Island of Sark. Two other related dialects are Guernsey (Guernesiais), spoken on the
Island of Guernsey, and the now extinct Alderney (Auregnais), once spoken on the Island
of Alderney. Jersey and the dialects of Sark and Guernsey share a certain degree of
mutual intelligibility, with Jersey and Sark being more intelligible, and there is a small
inventory of lexical items that are unique to each dialect (Liddicoat 1994). The use of the
word dialect in this thesis refers to those dialects mentioned in this section.
2.6 Parlers
Within Jersey there are dialectical differences found from parish to parish. Jersey
linguists refer to these variants as parlers and are referred to as such in this thesis.
Parlers are regional dialects of Jersey, similar in idea to regional dialects of English in
the United States. They are mutually intelligible, regional variations found within a
language. The variation found among the parlers is primarily phonological and, to my
knowledge, there are no syntactic or semantic differences. The phonological difference
most pertinent to this thesis is that of vowel changes. In some parishes the phonemes /e/
2
For a more extended discussion on the differences between Jersey and French, see Spence (1960), Jones
(2001), and Liddicoat (1994).
8
and /ɛ/ “have coalesced into a single phoneme” (Liddicoat 1994: 114). The phoneme /ei/
has become /ɛː/ in some parlers. Also in the dialect of Sark and the parler of St. Ouen,
/z/ has become /ð/. Due to the fact that the quality of the vowel has no direct bearing on
my analysis and for consistency in data presentation in this thesis, I have chosen to use
the most common parler. This parler is referred to as West Jersey, which is spoken in
most of the parishes on the western part of the Island.
9
SECTION 3: DATA SOURCES
All data used in this thesis come from Liddicoat (1994). This source was chosen due to
the fact that it is the only widely available source of phonetically transcribed Jersey
sentences. Liddicoat (1994) uses several sources for his data. The primary source of data
comes from interviews conducted by Liddicoat in 1985. Liddicoat interviewed ten Jersey
speakers and also used a questionnaire to obtain his data. These speakers were primarily
over sixty years of age and were all native speakers. Liddicoat‟s secondary sources of
data are written texts, such as newspapers. These include two texts found in Ĺ Bulletîn
d’Quart d’An de l’Assembliee d’Jerriais. All data presented by Liddicoat are
phonetically transcribed into IPA, except the glossary, which is phonemic. The data in
this thesis are taken directly from Liddicoat‟s phonetic transcriptions.
3.1 Compilation of Data
Allomorphic variation occurs in a variety of parts of speech in Jersey. Variation can be
found in pronouns, negation morphemes, conditionals, verbs, nouns, and adjectives. For
this thesis only articles are focused on in order to eliminate the possibility of other
factors, such as semantic constraints, influencing allomorph choice. In order to obtain
data for this thesis, all of the examples (over 500 items) in Liddicoat (1994) were
reviewed. Since Liddicoat (1994) contains data from both Sark and Jersey, I then
separated the Jersey data from the Sark. Next, all the Jersey data (341 individual items
along with four texts) were examined for examples that contained articles. These were
then sorted by gender, number, and definiteness. These data were culled through to give
examples from a variety of phonological environments, such as prevocalic, postvocalic,
10
preconsonantal, postconsonantal, and sentence initial. This ensures a well balanced array
of data available for analysis.
11
SECTION 4: PHONEME INVENTORY
In this section I review the basic phonological inventory of Jersey. Jersey‟s phoneme
inventory is broken down into two sections, consonants and glides in Section 4.1 and
vowels and diphthongs in Section 4.2.
4.1 Consonants and Glides
The phonemic inventory of consonants and glides found in Jersey is presented below in
(1), which is adapted from Jones (2001)3. The few allophonic variations of consonant
that occur in Jersey are not included in the table. The phoneme /r/ has two allophones, [r]
and [ɹ], with the latter rarely occurring (Liddicoat 1994). Also, consonant length in
Jersey is phonemic, with length being represented by the following geminates: /tt/, /dd/,
/ss/, /zz/, /ʃʃ/, /ʒʒ/, /ll/, /nn/, /rr/, and /ðð/ (Spence 1960, Liddicoat 1994).
(1)
Jersey Consonants and Glides
Bilabial
Stop
Fricative
Affricate
Nasal
Lateral
Approximant
Trill
Labiodental
Dental
Alveolar
ð
t d
s z
p b
f v
m
n
l
w
Palatoalveolar
ʃ ʒ
ʧ ʤ
Palatal
Velar
k ɡ
h
ɲ
ʎ
j
r
3
The table in (1) is adapted from Jones (2001) in that /ð/ has been given its own place of articulation
column, „Dental,‟ distinct from /s/ and /z/. The table does not contain the glide /ɥ/. While /ɥ/ does occur
phonemically in Jersey it is not listed by Spence (1960), Liddicoat (1994), or Jones (2001). Liddicoat
(1994:124) does note that in Jersey /ɥ/ “may fall together with /w/ thereby decreasing the number of
semivocalic phonemes to two.”
12
Glottal
4.2 Vowels and Diphthongs
The inventory of vocalic phonemes found in Jersey is shown below in (2). The
phonemes /y, ø, œ, u, o/ are rounded vowels, while /i, e, ɛ, ə, a/ are unrounded.
(2)
Jersey Vowels
Long and nasal vowels are phonemic in Jersey. The phonemes /e/, /ɛ/, /a/, and /o/ have
both long and nasal forms, while the phonemes /i/, /y/, /œ/ and /u/ have long forms but
lack nasal forms (Liddicoat 1994). The phonemes /ø/ and /ə/ lack both nasal and long
forms (Liddicoat 1994).4 The vocalic allophones, excepting [ə], can also be long and/or
nasal. For a discussion on the phonemes found in Jersey, consult Spence (1960, 1985,
1987) and Liddicoat (1994).
Diphthongs are found in most of the parlers of Jersey, though there are some parlers that
lack diphthongs (Liddicoat 1994). There are two diphthong phonemes, /au/ and /iə/, and
several allophones. The following allophones are the diphthongs listed by Liddicoat
4
While Liddicoat (1994) states that /ø/ has no phonemic nasal form, I found several instances of phonemic
/ø̃/.
13
(1994): [ou] for /oː/, [ei] for /eː/, [wɔ] and [uo] for /uː/, and [øy], and [ɐy] for /œː/. For
the purpose of this thesis, I treat diphthongs as one segment. This choice in analysis is
discussed in Section 5.1.3.
14
SECTION 5: SYLLABLES
As sonority is the principle framework used in this thesis for analyzing the allomorphic
variation of Jersey articles, syllables must be discussed in order to understand the analysis
presented in Section 9. Syllables are the structural units that organize strings of
phonological sound, and the organization of syllables is based on the inherent sonority of
the phonological segments (Blevins 1996).
Syllables are composed of three parts, the onset, the nucleus and the coda. This is seen
by examining the Jersey word [dyð] „hard.‟ The onset of [dyð] is composed of the
consonant [d] while the nucleus is the vowel [y]. The coda position is occupied by the
consonant [ð].
The type of segment allowed to occupy each position is language-dependent. In English,
any segment with the feature [+sonorant] can occupy the nucleus of a syllable (Mohanan
1991). Often, syllable nuclei are composed of a segment that has the feature [+syllabic]
(Clements 1990) and are usually vowels. Though sometimes, again depending on the
language, the nucleus can be composed of a glide or a consonant (Selkirk 1984, Clements
1990). For example, in English, which has syllabic consonants, the phoneme /r/ can be a
nucleus, as in /pheɪ.pr̩/ paper. This is not true of Jersey, which does not allow syllabic
consonants (Liddicoat 1994).5
Syllables, at a minimum, must contain a nucleus, with the most basic syllable type being
that of a single vowel (V). The most common syllable type is that of a consonant (C) and
5
The absence of syllabic consonants in Jersey may be due to consonants lacking a [+syllabic] feature.
15
a vowel, CV (Clements 1990, Blevins 1996). These two syllable types are known as
open syllables, referring to the absence of a coda. A closed syllable is one with a coda,
(C)VC, with the onset being optional. Open syllables in Jersey are discussed in Section
5.1.1 and closed syllables in Section 5.1.2.
5.1 Jersey Syllable Structure
There are a number of allowable syllable types in Jersey, which I discuss in the following
sections. Allowable syllable composition in languages is often attributed to sonority
based constraints (Clements 1990, Blevins 1996).
5.1.1 Open Syllables
In Jersey, as with most languages, the open syllable is the preferred syllable type, with
over 60% of verbs ending in open syllables (Liddicoat 1994). The composition of open
syllables in Jersey can be that of a nucleus-only, as in the example in (3), or that of an
onset plus a nucleus, as in the example in (4). These vowel only syllables can be
composed of any vowel, except for /ə/ (Liddicoat 1994). Unlike other languages that
allow syllabic consonants, such as English (Crystal 2003) and Berber (Dell and
Elmedlaoui 1985), the nucleus position in Jersey syllables can only be occupied by
vowels (Liddicoat 1994). Consonants and glides cannot occupy the nucleus of a syllable,
with glides being treated as consonants for the purpose of syllable composition (Liddicoat
1994).
16
(3)
(4)
Open Syllables - V
a.
/ɛ̃/
„fishing hook‟
b.
/a/
„at, in, to‟
c.
/oː/
„bone‟
Open Syllables - CV
a.
/bɛ̃/
„well‟
b.
/sy/
„sour‟
c.
/ma/
„evil‟
In word initial onset position, C can be any consonant except /ð/ and /z/, though /z/ does
occur in this position in loanwords, such as [z̃] „zinc‟ borrowed from German zink. /ð/
and /z/ can be onsets of syllable that are not word initial, such as [krœː.ðɛ] „to excavate‟
(Liddicoat 1994).
Open syllables can also have consonant clusters in onset position, as in example (15)
below. Consonant clusters of more than two consonants, especially those in which the
third consonant is not a glide, are usually avoided in Jersey, and are quite rare (see
Section 5.4). The composition of consonant clusters of more than two consonants is
restricted, with the third consonant being almost always limited to glides (Liddicoat
1994), as in [krwɛː] „cross.‟
17
(5)
Open Syllables - CCV
a.
/brø̃/
„brown‟
b.
/fwɛː/
„faith‟
c.
/dvɑ̃/
„front‟
5.1.2 Closed Syllables
Closed syllables, while not the preferred syllable type (Liddicoat 1994), are found in
Jersey. The simplest, though more marked, closed syllable is composed of a vowel and a
consonant, as seen in (6).
(6)
Closed Syllables - VC
a.
/ɑːl/
„garlic‟
b.
/ɛð/
„floor‟
c.
/œʃ/
„axle-pin‟
Closed syllables can also be any of the following compositions: CVC, CCVC, CCCVC,
CVCC, CCVCC, and CVCCC (Liddicoat 1994). As noted above, the triple consonant
clusters are avoided, so while syllables of the type CCCVC and CVCCC are shown in
glossaries and dictionaries, they are rarely realized this way in speech. Avoidance
strategies employed in speech are discussed in Section 5.4. Examples of the above
syllable types are shown in (7).
18
(7)
Closed Syllables
a.
CVC
/lɛːz/
„width‟
b.
CCVC
/ɡrɛːs/
„grease‟
c.
CCCVC
/ɡlwɛð/
„glory‟
d.
CVCC
/batr/
„to hit‟
e.
CCVCC
/prɑ̃dr/
„to take‟
f.
CVCCC
/sɛrkj/
„weed‟
5.1.3 Unallowable Syllable Types
There are two syllable types that are not allowed in Jersey. As mentioned above,
consonants cannot be a syllable nucleus and as a result C is in an unallowable syllable
type. Also, in monomorphemic words, a syllable composed of only two vowels, VV, is
not allowed (Liddicoat 1994). As mentioned in Section 4.2.1, vowels that are adjacent in
Jersey are treated as diphthongs and are thus treated as a single segment in being assigned
to a single nucleus of a syllable. This analysis is supported by the analysis of Schane
(1994), who notes that diphthongs correspond to two non-identical vowels within a single
nucleus.
5.1.4 Consonant Cluster Avoidance Strategies
While there are examples of consonant clusters, such as those in (7), consonant clusters
are not the preferred composition of onsets or codas in Jersey syllables. As noted above
in Section 5.1.1, Jersey avoids complex consonant clusters, i.e. those having more than
two consonants. According to Liddicoat (1994) Jersey uses epenthesis and deletion to
19
break up consonant clusters. There are certain consonant clusters in Jersey onsets that are
realized as CəC, with /ə/ being inserted between consonants (Liddicoat 1994). This is
seen in the examples below in (8).
(8)
/ə/ Epenthesis
a.
/lvi/ → [ləvi] „lever‟
b.
/rmjɛd/ → [rəmjɛd] „remedy‟
c.
/skabɛ/ → [səkabɛ] „stool‟
(Liddicoat 1994: 136-137)
Insertion of /ə/ between consonants does not occur in all consonant clusters. For
example, it does occur when the sequence is [lv], as in (18a) above, but not when the
order is reversed as in [vlik] „whelk.‟ This may stem from the sonority of the respective
sounds, which will be discussed in Section 9.
Word final complex consonant clusters of CCC can only be composed of /rbr, rdr, rtr/.
When these do occur, the final /r/ is deleted (Liddicoat 1994). This also occurs in word
final consonant clusters, those of CC, where the final consonant is a /r/. Examples of
both can be seen in (9).
20
(9)
/r/ Deletion
a.
/arbr/ → [arb] „tree‟
b.
/tœrtr/ → [tœrt] „to twist‟
c.
/batr/ → [bat] „to hit‟
(Liddicoat 1994: 139)
The presence of these avoidance strategies hints at the fact that Jersey goes to certain
lengths to avoid creating consonant clusters that do not adhere to the Sonority
Sequencing Principle (Clements 1990), which is discussed in Section 6.2.2. It is also
possible that these forms are not a result of phoneme insertion and deletion but are
actually instead occurs of allomorphic variation. Regardless of the analysis, sonority
appears to affect the appearance of morphemes in Jersey.
21
SECTION 6: THEORETICAL BACKGROUND
There are a number of principles, parameters, and laws that are used for the
syllabification of data and analysis in this thesis. This section gives the background and
examples of these various principles. Principles concerning syllabification and sonority
are covered in Sections 6.1 and 6.2 respectively.
6.1 Syllable Principles
Syllables are an important unit of organization in language. Most, if not all, speakers can
provide syllabification of words when asked (Blevins 1996, Duanmu 2008). The basic
assumptions behind syllabification have been codified into principles, laws, and
parameters, some of which I present below. I first discuss three principles dealing with
syllabification and onsets, the Minimal Onset Satisfaction Principle (Roca 1994), the
Maximal Onset Realisation Parameter (Roca 1994), and the Law of Initials (Vennemann
1988). I then discuss the Law of Finals (Vennemann 1988), which covers the
composition of codas. The last principle I discuss is the Romance Resyllabification
Principle, which deals with syllabification across word boundaries.
6.1.1 Minimal Onset Satisfaction Principle
One of the most basic syllable constraints is that of the Minimal Onset Satisfaction
Principle (Roca 1994). The Minimal Onset Satisfaction Principle (Roca 1994) accounts
for the fact that the most preferred syllable type cross-linguistically is CV and that series
of VCV are most often syllabified as V.CV (Blevins 1996). Roca (1994) states that
22
syllables with codas are disallowed when there is a segment present that can be
syllabified into the onset.
(10)
Minimal Onset Satisfaction Principle
When suitable segments are present, they will be syllabified into the onset.
(adapted from Roca 1994)
Using the Minimal Onset Satisfaction Principle a polysyllabic word, such as [piðo],
„gander,‟ is syllabified as [pi.ðo], with [ð] being syllabified into the onset of the second
syllable instead of into the coda of the first syllable, *[pið.o]. The Minimal Onset
Satisfaction Principle helps account for word medial consonant clusters in the data that
may appear as sonority violations, but in fact are parts of separate syllables.6
6.1.2 Maximal Onset Realisation Parameter
Working in conjunction with the Minimal Onset Satisfaction Principle is the Maximal
Onset Realisation Parameter (Roca 1994). The Maximal Onset Realisation Parameter
(Roca 1994) requires that onsets are satisfied before codas in syllable formation.
(11)
Maximal Onset Realisation Parameter
Onset construction must be completed prior to coda formation.
(Roca 1994: 145)
6
Sonority and principles of sonority are discussed in Section 7.2.
23
As with the Minimal Onset Satisfaction Principle, the Maximal Onset Realisation
Parameter aids in the syllabification of word medial consonant clusters. In [pikɑ̃bwɛː]
„wood-pecker‟ there appears to be a choice as to which position [b] can occupy. [b] can
be syllabified into either the coda of the penultimate syllable [pi.kɑ̃b.wɛː] or into the
onset of the ultimate syllable [pi.kɑ̃.bwɛː]. The Maximal Onset Realisation Parameter
and the Minimal Onset Satisfaction Principle dictate that the latter of the two possible
syllabifications is preferred.
6.1.3 Law of Initials
As noted by Vennemann (1988) the composition of syllable onsets is often similar to that
of word initial onsets. Vennemann (1988) notes that this is true in Italian and Latin, and
refers to this preference as the Law of Initials (Vennemann 1988), which is given in
(12).7
(12)
Law of Initials
Word medial syllable heads are the more preferred, the less they differ from
possible word initial syllable heads of the language system.
(Vennemann 1988: 32)
The Law of Initials helps to determine the composition of onsets in a given language.
While the Maximal Onset Realisation Parameter states that onsets must be satisfied
before codas, the Law of Initials provides balance so that the composition of onsets does
not violate a language‟s constraints on the composition of consonant clusters. If
7
In Vennemann‟s (1988) Law of Initials, „head‟ refers to „onset.‟
24
syllabifying word medial consonants into onsets creates a consonant cluster in the onset,
the allowable word initial onset consonant clusters should be examined to see if there is a
similar cluster allowed in the word initial onset. If it is not allowed, then it is preferred to
resyllabify the consonants into allowable clusters. For example, the word [parfnɛ] „to
spread out for drying‟ contains the word medial cluster [rfn]. The Maximal Onset
Realisation Parameter requires that the cluster be syllabified into the onset [pa.rfnɛ],
which does not conform to certain sonority principles, such as the Sonority Sequencing
Principle (Clements 1990) discussed in Section 6.2.2. By using the Law of Initials in
examining the allowable word initial onset clusters for Jersey in (13), it is seen that [rfn]
is not an allowable word initial onset consonant cluster. Taking this into account,
[parfnɛ] is syllabified as [par.fnɛ], with [fn] being an allowable word/syllable initial
onset.
(13) Allowable Word/Syllable Initial Onsets in Jersey
bj
fj
br
fr
bw
fw
bz
ɡj
mj
nj
pj
rɡ
sj
ml
nw
pw
rj
sm
kw mw
rk
kz
kj
ɡlw krw
ɡr
ɡrw
ɡw
ʃw
tr
ʧɥ
vj
trw
vr
sn
trɥ
vw
rkj
st
tw
rw
str
rɥ
tɥ
sw
sɥ
sʧ
25
ʃj
ʒɥ
ʒw
ʒt
To determine the allowable word initial syllable onsets shown in (23), I examined two
Jersey glossaries (Spence 1960 and Liddicoat 1994). Liddicoat (1994) notes that there
are a number of initial consonant clusters that, while listed in glossaries and dictionaries
do not actually occur in everyday speech. These clusters, discuss in Section 5.1.4, are
realized as CəC and I have not included them in the table of allowable onsets. I only list
consonant clusters in this table, since, for the most part, single consonants are allowable
onsets.8 These allowable onsets are also used to help determine the Jersey sonority
hierarchy presented in Section 7.
6.1.4 Law of Finals
Similar to the Law of Initials is the Law of Finals (Vennemann 1988). Vennemann
(1988) notes that like the word initial/syllable onset similarity, word final codas and
syllable codas compositions are similar.
(14)
Law of Finals
Word medial syllable codas are the more preferred, the less they differ from
possible word final syllable codas of the language system.
(Vennemann 1988: 33)
The Law of Finals helps determine allowable coda composition and helps with
syllabification. In the syllabification of [parfnɛ] above, I used the Law of Initials to
determine that [fn] is an allowable onset. Using the Law of Finals I determine that [rf] is
8
See Section 6.1.1 for restrictions on single consonants in the onset position.
26
most likely not an allowable coda since it does not appear in word final codas, as shown
in the table in (15).
(15)
Allowable Word Final/Syllable Final Codas in Jersey
bj
ɡj
kj
lt
mp
pj
rb
rm
rd
rn
rɡ
rp
rk
rs
rkj
rʃ
rl
rʒ
st
As with the allowable onsets, to determine allowable codas I examined the glossaries of
Spence (1960) and Liddicoat (1994) and created a list of word final coda consonant
clusters. Liddicoat (1994: 139) states that “It should be noted that consonant clusters with
final /r/ are, however, rare. The preference is for deletion of the final consonant in such
groups.” I have not included the Cr combinations, since it seems that these forms occur
only in glossaries and dictionaries. As with onsets, there are no distribution restrictions
on the single consonants that can occupy coda position.
6.1.5 Romance Resyllabification Principle
In Carinaletti and Repetti‟s (2009: 94) investigation into the resyllabification and
prozodization of clitics in Romance languages, they discuss the fact that “within
resyllabification contexts, a word final consonant can resyllabify as the onset of the
following word, but not if the result would be a complex onset.” As Jersey is a Romance
27
language I assume that this restriction on resyllabification occurs in Jersey as well. I take
this restriction to apply also to the syllabification of word initial consonants into codas of
preceding words. Since Carinaletti and Repetti do not posit a rule or constraint when
discussing resyllabification, I term this phenomenon the Romance Resyllabification
Principle. The Romance Resyllabification Principle is stated as follows:
(16)
Romance Resyllabification Principle
A consonant at a word‟s edge can re-syllabify as the onset of the following word
or as the coda of the preceding word, but not if it results in the creation of a
consonant cluster.
(adapted from Carinaletti and Repetti 2009)
This principle also prevents the syllabification of consonant clusters across word
boundaries, as this would also result in a consonant cluster. For example, with the phrase
[teim leiz ɑ̃ɡjei] ‟we were English‟ (Liddicoat 1994: 341) the application of the Romance
Resyllabification Principle prevents [l] from being syllabified into the coda of [teim],
*[teiml]. It allows for the syllabification of [z] into the onset of [ɑ̃ɡ], [zɑ̃ɡ]. I assume
that it prevents the syllabification of the consonant cluster across word boundaries also.
The Romance Resyllabification principles prevents [lz] in [ei lz eip̃n] „and the thorns‟
(Liddicoat 1994: 361) from syllabifying into the onset of [ei], *[lzei], as this creates a
consonant cluster where there was not one before.
28
While the term resyllabification implies initial syllabification at the polymorphemic word
level, I do not discuss the level at which syllabification first takes place.9
The principles covered so far in this section are used in Section 9 in the syllabification
and analysis of Jersey phrases that contain definite articles. Principles, such as the
Minimal Onset Satisfaction Principle, the Maximal Onset Realisation Parameter, and the
Law of Initials are used to determine onset composition. The Romance Resyllabification
Principle prevents these principles from syllabifying consonant clusters into onsets or
codas across word boundaries. The Laws of Initials and Finals (Vennemann 1988) help
determine the phonological composition of onsets and codas, respectively, during
syllabification.
6.2 Sonority Principles
The concept of sonority is couched in auditory phonetics.10 The term sonority usually
refers to the overall amplitude or „loudness‟ of a sound when compared to sounds of
similar stress, pitch, and duration (Blevins 1996). Segments are believed to have certain
inherent sonority properties, making certain sounds more sonorous than others, with
vowels being presumed to be the most sonorous (Clements 1990). Sonority has been
adopted into phonology in an attempt to understand the motivation behind syllable
formation. This section examines sonority hierarchies briefly in order to provide proper
background for understanding the sonority principles described herein. I then discuss the
9
It is quite possible that syllabification takes place initially at the word level and then again at the phrasal
level or that it takes place only at the phrasal level. This argument is outside the scope of this thesis and is
not addressed further.
10
While the exact physical correlates of sonority are the topic of debate among linguists, that debate is not
addressed by this thesis.
29
Sonority Sequencing Principle (Clements 1990) and the Core Syllabification Principle
(Clements 1990), which guide syllabification through the use of sonority rankings. The
Syllable Contact Law (Clements 1990), which assists in the syllabification of word
medial consonants, is then discussed.
6.2.1 Sonority Hierarchies
Though this is discussed in more detail in Section 7 below, it is imperative that I discuss
sonority hierarchies prior to certain sonority principles given in this section. A scalar
ranking of sonority is a key concept in understanding sonority. A sonority hierarchy is
the ranking of sounds by their level of sonority. Sounds are assigned indices and ranked
along a scale by their sonority relative to the sonority of other sounds. This ranking is
determined by taking a number of factors into account, such as features and phonotactics.
The most basic sonority hierarchy is feature based, using the binary values of [±syllabic],
[±vocoid], [±approximant], and [±sonorant] (Clements 1990). The resulting hierarchy
shown in (17) is generally accepted as the universal sonority hierarchy on which many
language-specific hierarchies are based.
(17)
Universal Sonority Hierarchy
Vowels>Glides>Liquids>Nasals>Obstruents
(Clements 1990: 292)
30
These divisions are then assigned an index number based on their rank in the hierarchy.
Sonority hierarchies based on other factors, such as phonotactics, including the Jersey
Sonority Hierarchy, are discussed in Section 7.
6.2.2 Sonority Sequencing Principle
It has been observed that sonority seems to rise and fall within a syllable, with the
nucleus being the peak in a sequence of sonorous sounds. This phenomenon is captured
by the Sonority Sequencing Principle, as put forth by Clements (1990).
(18)
Sonority Sequencing Principle
Between any member of a syllable and the syllable peak, only sounds of higher
sonority rank are permitted.
(Clements 1990: 285)
The Sonority Sequencing Principle explains why certain consonant clusters in syllables
are more preferred in onset and coda positions. For example cross-linguistically onsets
composed of an obstruent and a liquid (OL) and codas of the inverse composition (LO)
are quite common (Clements 1990). Since obstruents are lower in sonority than liquids,
the segment higher in sonority is closer to the syllable nucleus in both the onset and the
coda. This principle may account for /ə/ insertion between certain onset consonant
clusters, which were discussed in Section 5.4. It was noted then that [lv] is not an
allowable consonant cluster word initially while [vl] is. This is explained through the
Sonority Sequencing Principle (Clements 1990). According to the Universal Sonority
31
Hierarchy, [l] is higher in sonority than [v], [l] being a liquid and [v] being an obstruent.
In a sequence of liquid obstruent vowel (LOV), there is a member of the syllable that is
lower in sonority between the initial consonant and the nucleus, which violates the
Sonority Sequencing Principle. When /ə/ is inserted there is no intervening member of
higher sonority between L and the nucleus, instead it is now the onset of the second
syllable (LV.OV)
6.2.3 Core Syllabification Principle
The Core Syllabification Principle was first posited by Clements and Keyser (1981,
1983) and later revised by Clements (1990). The Core Syllabification Principle
(Clements 1990) provides a basic framework for syllabifying words and phrases. This
approach assumes that there is a peak of sonority associated with the nucleus of each
syllable. Any phoneme which is [+syllabic] is assigned as the syllable node and adjacent
segments are syllabified based on their sonority ranking in comparison to the nucleus.
This assures that segments that are lower in sonority than the nucleus are syllabified into
the onset or coda of a syllable. The Core Syllabification Principle (Clements 1990) is
defined below in (19).
(19)
Core Syllabification Principle
a. Associate each [+syllabic] segment to a syllable node
b. Given P (an unsyllabified segment) preceding Q (a syllabified segment) if P is
lower in sonority rank than Q adjoin it to the syllable containing Q (iterative)
(Clements 1990: 317)
32
The Core Syllabification Principle provides a basic framework for the assignment of
segments to nodes, such as onset, nucleus, and coda, within the syllable. For example, if
I apply the Core Syllabification Principle to the word [mwɔʃɛ] „heap pile‟ I first identify
segments that are [+syllabic], which in Jersey are the vowels [ɔ] and [ɛ]. These are then
assigned to their own syllable nodes (also known as the nucleus). Focusing on the first
vowel, [ɔ], the unsyllabified segment preceding [ɔ] is [w], which, as a glide, is lower in
sonority than [ɔ] (refer to the Universal Sonority Hierarchy in (17) for rankings).
According to the Core Syllabification Principle, if the segment is lower in sonority than
the following syllabified segment, it is adjoined to that syllable. Now the first syllable is
composed of [wɔ]. This is an iterative process, so now the segment preceding [w] is
examined. [m] precedes [w] and is lower in sonority than [w]. It is then adjoined to [w]
and the first syllable is formed, [mwɔ.]. As there are no more segments preceding [m],
the second syllable can be formed. [ɛ] is preceded by [ʃ]. [ʃ] as on obstruent is lower in
sonority than [ɛ], so it is adjoined to form the second syllable, [ʃɛ]. The word then is
syllabified as [mwɔ.ʃɛ].
6.2.4 Syllable Contact Law
The Syllable Contact Law (Clements 1990), in addition to the Sonority Sequencing
Principle, is used in this thesis to help explain the avoidance of sonority plateaus and the
syllabification of certain segments into codas or onsets.
33
The Syllable Contact Law, as put forth by Vennemann (1988), deals with consonantal
strength, which works inversely of sonority. The greater the consonantal strength the less
sonorous a phoneme is.
(20)
Vennemann’s Syllable Contact Law11
A syllable contact A$B is the more preferred, the less the Consonantal Strength of
the offset A and the greater the Consonantal Strength of the onset B; more
precisely – the greater the characteristic difference CS(B)-CS(A) between the
Consonantal Strength of B and that of A
(Vennemann 1988: 40)
Clements (1990) adapted Vennemann‟s law and proposed the version in (31), which is
used in this thesis.
(21)
Syllable Contact Law
In any sequence Ca $ Cb there is a preference for Ca to exceed Cb in sonority.
(Clements 1990: 287)
The Syllable Contact Law explains the fact that at syllable margins there is a preference
for the final segment of the first syllable to exceed in sonority the first segment of the
second syllable. The Jersey word [primzol] „primrose‟ is syllabified as [prim.zol]. The
word medial consonant cluster [mz] could be syllabified into the onset of the second
syllable, according to the Maximal Onset Realisation Parameter and Minimal Onset
11
$ = syllable boundary
34
Satisfaction Principle. Since [m], as a nasal, is higher in sonority than [z], an obstruent,
the Sonority Sequencing Principle requires that the [m] be syllabified into the coda of the
first syllable and [z] into the onset of the second syllable. This occurs so that [z], a
segment lower in sonority than [m], does not occur between [m] and the syllable nucleus.
This results in the syllabification of [primzol] as [prim.zol], which is preferred.
6.3 Summary
The principles, parameters, and laws discussed in this section are pertinent to the analysis
of Jersey definite articles presented in Section 9. The Minimal Onset Satisfaction
Principle, the Maximal Onset Realisation Parameter, the Law of Initials, and the Law of
Finals help to determine composition of the onsets and codas of syllables. The Romance
Resyllabification Principle plays an important role in prohibiting the formation of
consonant clusters through resyllabification across word boundaries. This is important
when examining the syllabification of articles, as seen in Section 9. The Sonority
Sequencing Principle, the Core Syllabification Principle, and the Syllable Contact Law
ensure that syllables abide by principles of sonority when syllabification occurs.
35
SECTION 7: JERSEY SONORITY HIERARCHY
The concept of sonority hierarchies was discussed briefly in Section 6.2.1 and is an
important aspect of this thesis. Below I discuss the ranking of sonority along with the
need to create a language specific sonority hierarchy for Jersey. I then demonstrate how
a Jersey specific sonority hierarchy explains certain phonological sequences that the
Universal Sonority Hierarchy (shown in (17) above) cannot. The Jersey Sonority
Hierarchy is used to analyze the variation of definite articles in that it ranks the segments
according to their inherent sonority, which then allows for the syllabification of phrases.
7.1 Sonority Rankings
Clements (1990) originally determined the indices for sonority ranking by adding the
number of pluses of the binary features mentioned in Section 6.2.1. In other hierarchies,
such as those found in Selkirk (1984) and Parker (2002), the assignment of indices is
based solely on the inherent sonority of a segment relative to that of other segments. For
example, the ranking of the Universal Sonority Hierarchy can be as shown in (22).
(22)
Sonority Rankings with Arbitrary Number Assignment
Vowels - 5
Glides – 4
Liquids – 3
Nasals – 2
Obstruents – 1
36
The value of the numbers is not as important as the ranking of each relative to the other.
The Jersey specific sonority hierarchy posited in this thesis uses an arbitrary assignment
of numbers. The group located at the lower end of the sonority hierarchy is assigned the
number 1 and the numbering increases monotonically up the hierarchy.
7.2 Factors in Determining Sonority Hierarchies
As noted with Clements‟s (1990) Universal Sonority Hierarchy, the use of binary features
is one way to determine the ranking of segments. Another way in which sonority
hierarchies are determined is by examining the distribution of the sounds within a given
language, or the phonotactics of the language. Phonotactics refer to the ways in which
phonological sounds can be put together to compose a well formed word (Goldsmith
1996). Phonotactic constraints found within a given language are used to posit specific
values for sounds in order to create a language specific hierarchy. Phonotactic
constraints have been used to determine sonority hierarchy for Blackfoot by Elfner
(2005), for Pali by Hankamer and Aissen (1974), for Tohono O‟odham by Miyashita
(2003), for Gallo-Romance by Jacobs (1992), for Imdlawn Tashlihiyt Berber by Dell and
Elmedlaoui (1985), and for Hungarian by Siptár and Tӧrkenczy (2000), to list a few.
In order to account for the phonotactics found in Jersey, Jersey specific sonority
hierarchy had to be determined. This was due to the fact that while the Universal
Sonority Hierarchy accounted for a portion of the Jersey data, it did not account for all of
the data. In particular there are certain sound patterns found in Jersey that are in direct
contradiction to the Universal Sonority Hierarchy. There are two particular consonant
37
cluster patterns in Jersey syllable onsets that cannot be accounted for using the Universal
Sonority Hierarchy, these being the sequence of Stop+/v/, i.e. /dv/, /ɡv/, (23) and
Stop+/z/, i.e. /bz/, /kz/, /dz/ (24).12
(23)
(24)
Stop+/v/
a.
[dvɑ̃tɛ]
„apron‟
b.
[ɡvil]
„ankle‟
Stop +/z/
a.
[bzɑ̃]
„heavy‟
b.
[kzi]
„look for‟
c.
[dzɛrtaʒ]
„the clearing of land for farming‟
When using the Universal Sonority Hierarchy for words such as [ɡvil] „ankle‟ (33b) and
[dzɛrtaʒ] „the clearing of land for farming‟ (34c), sonority plateaus occur. Sonority
plateaus refer to the condition when two adjacent sounds are of the same sonority
strength. Sonority plateaus are dispreferred within a syllable as accounted for by the
Sonority Sequencing Principle and at syllable margins by the Syllable Contact Law.13 In
(25) the sonority plateau occurs in the onset of the first syllable [dzɛr].
12
/dv/ and /ɡv/ are both listed by Liddicoat (1994) as being affected by /ə/. These forms though are found
throughout the data, see example (8c). This is discussed in Section 10.3, Implications.
13
Blevins (1996) notes that languages which allow syllable initial and syllable final geminates, such as
Trukese and Estonian, respectively, tolerate sonority plateaus. The fact that Jersey does not allow syllable
initial or syllable final geminates suggests that sonority plateaus are not tolerated in Jersey.
38
(25)
[dzɛrtaʒ] ‘the clearing of land for farming
Since the sequences mentioned above in (23) and (24) are fairly common in Jersey, the
sonority hierarchy used in analyzing Jersey should reflect this. In Jersey, then, /v/ and /z/
must be extracted from the obstruents and assigned their own respective indices that must
be ranked higher than obstruents. Cross linguistically there is a tendency for fricatives to
be ranked higher in sonority than stops (Jespersen 1904, Selkirk 1984, Dell and
Elmedlaoui 1985, 1988, Blevins 1996, Parker 2002). This delineation will help to
eliminate the sonority plateau shown in the example in (25).
Another type of sequence that occurs in Jersey is found in syllable codas, and is /st/. The
Universal Sonority Hierarchy cannot account for the consonant cluster /st/ as they are
both obstruents and ranked together on the Universal Sonority Hierarchy. In (26) the
Universal Sonority Hierarchy has been used to assign the sonority indices for the word
[ɛst] „east.‟ This results in a sonority plateau in the coda, [st], as both are obstruents and
thus equal in sonority. As noted above, sonority plateaus are dispreferred.
39
(26)
[ɛst] ‘east’
In order to account for these and other phonological patterns, I examine Gallo-Romance
sonority hierarch posited by Jacobs (1992). Jacobs (1992) uses phonotactics to determine
a Gallo-Romance specific sonority hierarchy. Since Jersey is a member of the GalloRomance family, and as such, shares certain phonemes in common with Gallo-Romance,
the Gallo-Romance sonority hierarchy forms some basis of the Jersey specific sonority
hierarchy. In the hierarchy in (27), Jacobs (1992) ranks the consonants found in GalloRomance from most to least sonorous.
(27)
Gallo-Romance Sonority Hierarchy
r
l
n, m
s, z
f
p, t, k, b, d, ɡ
(Jacobs 1992: 71)
40
Unlike the Universal Sonority Hierarchy, the Gallo-Romance sonority hierarchy
distinguishes between alveolar fricatives, /s, z/ and other obstruents. In Jersey, /s/ and /z/
pattern quite differently than other obstruents, as discussed above. /z/ does not have the
same distribution as /s/ in Jersey, but this is due to the fact that /z/ is found only
intervocalically and evolved in this position from intervocalic /r/ historically (Spence
1960, Liddicoat 1994)14. In Jersey /z/ is found word initially, but only in loanwords
(Liddicoat 1994).
Liquids in the Universal Sonority Hierarchy are one group, which the Gallo-Romance
sonority hierarchy splits into two separate sections, with rhotics being more sonorous
than laterals. The division of liquids into rhotics and laterals helps explain the fact that /r/
and /l/ have different distribution in Jersey. For example, /rl/ is an allowable word final
coda, as in [kɔrl] „curl‟ but /lr/ is not.
Jacobs (1992) also distinguishes between /f/ and other obstruents. This distinction is
useful in accounting for certain distributional differences between /f/ and /v/ and other
obstruents in Jersey. Although the phoneme /v/ does not occur in Gallo-Romance, I have
included it since it occurs in Jersey and has a similar distribution to that of /f/, the only
difference between the phonemes being voicing. The separation of labiodental fricatives
from other obstruents is highly beneficial to an analysis of Jersey sonority. /f/ and /v/
behave differently than other obstruents. Obstruent+/f/ or /v/ occurs in word initial onsets
in Jersey, for example in [dvɑ̃] „front.‟ The other obstruents are left together to form one
14
The change of one sound segment over time to another brings up the questions as to whether sonority of
that segment also changes. This is discuss in Section 10.3, Implications.
41
category, termed Other Obstruents, as I have not found any motivation to warrant
separating them by voicing or articulation.
Since Jersey is a member of the Gallo-Romance family and shares a similar phoneme
inventory, I use the Gallo-Romance hierarchy to create gradients within the Universal
Sonority Hierarchy to construct a Jersey specific sonority hierarchy, given in (38). The
changes which distinguish the Jersey Sonority Hierarchy from the Universal Sonority
Hierarchy are bolded.
(28)
Jersey Sonority Hierarchy
Vowels - i, y, u, e, ə, o, ɛ, œ, a, ɑ
Glides - w, j, ɥ
Rhotics - r
Laterals - l, ʎ
Nasals - m, n, ɲ
Alveolar Fricatives - s, z
Labiodental Fricatives - f, v
Other Obstruents - p, b, t, d, k, ɡ, ð, ʧ, ʤ, ʃ, ʒ, h
By using the Jersey Sonority Hierarchy to examine the examples given in (35) and (36)
above, the sonority plateaus created by the Universal Sonority Hierarchy disappear.
(29a) below analyzes [dzɛrtaʒ] ((25) repeated here as (29b)) using the Jersey specific
hierarchy.
42
(29)
Analysis of [dzɛrtaʒ] ‘the clearing of land for farming’
(a)
Jersey Sonority Hierarchy
(b)
Universal Sonority Hierarchy
Now that /z/ has been extracted from the obstruents (along with /s/) and ranked in its own
higher ranking group, the sonority plateau has been removed from the onset of the first
syllable. This analysis now abides by the Sonority Sequencing Principle.
In (30) I compare an analysis of [ɛst] using the Jersey Sonority Hierarchy and Universal
Sonority Hierarchy, with (27) being repeated here as (30b).
(30)
Analysis of [ɛst] ‘east’
(a)
Jersey Sonority Hierarchy
(b)
Universal Sonority Hierarchy
43
As with (29) above, the analysis in (30a) does not contain the sonority plateau that occurs
in (30b) when the Universal Sonority Hierarchy is used.
The benefit of using a language specific sonority hierarchy is that language specific
phonological patterns are accounted for. The Jersey Sonority Hierarchy proposed here
aids in the analysis of the allomorphic variation found among Jersey definite articles in
the following section.
44
SECTION 8: MORPHOPHONEMICS OF JERSEY DEFINITE ARTICLES
This section presents the data used in the analysis in Section 9. Like other Romance
languages, Jersey has grammatical gender, where nouns have a grammatically determined
gender that is reflected through agreement in articles and adjectives. The choice of
article depends on both the gender and number of the noun. There is further variation
seen within the article chosen, which, according to Birt (1985) and Liddicoat (1994), is
dependent upon the phonological environment.
Since Jersey definite articles are affected by the number and gender of the nouns, I have
presented the known allomorphs of Jersey definite articles in table (31). The underlying
morphemes for these allomorphs are discussed in their respective sections.
(31)
Jersey Definite Article Allomorphs
Masculine
Definite
Feminine
Definite
Singular
[l], [le]
Plural
[lei], [lz], [leiz]
[la], [l]
[lei], [lz], [leiz]
As is seen in the table in (31), the definite plural articles are identical for both genders,
[lei, lz, leiz] and are therefore treated as one in this thesis. They are simply identified as
the definite plural article without mention of gender. In this section I first discuss the
masculine definite article, followed by the feminine definite article, and then the definite
plural article.
45
8.1 Masculine Definite Singular Article Allomorphs – [l] and [le]
The masculine definite singular article allomorphs are [l] and [le]. According to the
description given by Liddicoat (1994), the use of the two allomorphs, [l] and [le], is
determined by the presence or absence of word initial consonant clusters in the word
following the article. The majority of the data given pattern as described, examples of
which are shown below in (33) and (34). Given the restricted use of the allomorph [le],
occurring only before consonant clusters, I posit that the underlying morpheme for the
masculine definite singular form is /l/. This choice is reinforced by the analysis presented
in Section 9.1.
While Liddicoat (1994) does not specify the conditions under which [l] surfaces, the
elsewhere condition is assumed for this allomorph. This is due to the fact that [l] surfaces
in a rather unrestricted environment, which is when the word following the article begins
with a vowel or a consonant. The data found support this assumption. In (32), [l] occurs
before vowels, as in (32d) and before consonants, as in (32a-c). In the data presented in
this section, the allomorph under discussion is bolded and the environment is underlined.
(32)
[l] before C or V
a.
[vɛː l ɡard̃]
„see the garden‟
(169)
b.
[k̃t l foːsɛ]
„by the hedgerow‟
(263)
c.
[ei l mijœ]
„is the best‟
(182)
d.
[apr̃ l ɑ̃ɡjɛː]
„learnt English‟
(242)
(Liddicoat 1994)
46
In (32b) the noun [foːsɛ] „hedgerow‟ begins with a single consonant, [f], and the
allomorph [l] occurs. In (32d) the noun [ɑ̃ɡjɛː] „English‟ begins with a vowel, [ɑ̃] and the
allomorph [l] surfaces.
As discussed, according to Liddicoat (1994), the other allomorph, [le] is supposed to
occur only before consonant clusters. This distribution is seen in the examples below
(33).15
(33)
[le] before CC
a.
[ɑ̃lvɛːm le vrɛ]
„took the seaweed‟
(286)
b.
[k̃t le proɡrɛː]
„against progress‟
(285)
c.
#[le ʃjɛ̃]
„the he‟ (the one)
(362)
d.
#[le ʒva]
„the horse‟
(234)
(Liddicoat 1994)
In (33a) the noun [vrɛ] „seaweed‟ begins with the consonant cluster [vr] which, according
to Liddicoat (1994) conditions the occurrence of the allomorph [le] instead of [l]. While
the majority of the data abide by the rule posited by Liddicoat (1994), there are cases
where the allomorph [le] is found before single consonants, instead of [l] and the
allomorph [l] is found before consonant clusters instead of [le]. In each example in (34)
The following symbols are used in this thesis: # - sentence boundaries, . – syllable boundaries, || - word
boundaries.
15
47
below, the word following each article begins with a single consonant and, according to
Liddicoat (1994) the allomorph [l] should occur, yet [le] is what actually occurs.
(34)
[le] before C
a.
#[le ʒur]
„the day‟
(339)
b.
[ʤɛð le tɑ̃]
„hardly the time‟
(222)
(Liddicoat 1994)
For example, in (34a) the noun [ʒur] „day‟ begins with a single consonant, [ʒ]. Liddicoat
(1994) implies that this noun should be preceded by the allomorph [l] not the attested [le].
There are also occurrences of the opposite, in that [l] occurs in a position where,
according to Liddicoat (1994), [le] should occur. With consonant clusters, Liddicoat
(1994) predicts that the allomorph [le] should occur, yet there are data where [l] occurs
instead of [le]. This is seen in (35), where [l], which is expected only before vowels and
single consonants, surfaces instead of [le]. The description given by Liddicoat (1994)
makes no mention of data that do not abide by the patterns he discusses.
(35)
[l] before CC
a.
[fɛ l brɑ̃kɑʒ]
„done the „branchage‟‟
(267)
b.
[kə l travɑː]
„the work‟
(348)
(Liddicoat 1994)
48
In (35a) the adjective [brɑ̃kɑʒ] „branchage‟ begins with the consonant cluster [br]. It is
predicted by Liddicoat (1994) that the allomorph [le] should occur, instead the allomorph
[l] occurs.
The use of the allomorph [le] before single consonants, as seen in (34) and the use of the
allomorph [l] before consonant clusters, as seen in (35), are unaccounted for in the
predictions made by Liddicoat (1994). These pieces of data indicate that some force
must be affecting allomorph choice beyond the occurrence of vowels or consonants as the
first segment of the word following the article. I propose that this variation can be
accounted for and predicted if sonority is taken into account. This analysis is explored in
Section 9.
8.2 Feminine Definite Singular Article Allomorphs – [la] and [l]
As with the masculine definite singular article, the feminine definite singular article
exhibits allomorphic variation. The feminine definite article allomorphs are [la] and [l],
with the underlying morpheme being /la/. The choice of /la/ as the feminine definite
singular article underlying morpheme ensures morphemic contrast with the masculine
definite singular article /l/. Both the feminine definite singular allomorphs [l] and [la] are
equally occurring, one before vowels and the other before consonants. This prohibits one
from being an „elsewhere‟ distribution, which is often used when determining underlying
morphemes. In addition to the argument of morphemic contrast, the analysis presented in
Section 9.3 also supports the selection of /la/ as the underlying morpheme.
49
According to Liddicoat (1994), the distribution of [l] and [la] is determined by the
phonological composition of the word following the article. Liddicoat (1994) states that
the allomorph [l] is found prevocalically. I assume, as I did above with the masculine
definite article, that [la] must occur before consonants. This is confirmed by the data I
found, examples of which are shown in (36).
(36)
[la] before C(C)
a.
#[la ɡɑːʃ]
„the cake‟
(245)
b.
[da la re]
„on the road‟
(208)
c.
[dvin la rep̃s]
„guesses the answer‟ (212)
d.
[eikɔl a la trinte]
„school in Trinity‟
(192)
(Liddicoat 1994)
As seen in (36), [la] occurs before single consonants and consonant clusters. In (36a), the
noun [ɡɑːʃ] „cake‟ begins with the consonant [ɡ], and in (36d) the word following the
article begins with a consonant cluster, [tr], with the allomorph [la] occurring in each
example.
As stated, the allomorph [l] occurs when the word following the article begins with a
vowel. Examples of this distribution are shown in (37). In (37c) the noun [armwɛð]
„wardrobe‟ begins with a vowel, [a], and the allomorph [l] occurs.
50
(37)
[l] before V
a.
[avɛk l ɑ̃ɡjɛtɛr]
b.
[nɛ ʧitiːm ʒɑ̃mei l iːl] „never left the island‟
(222)
c.
[ɑ̃ dsuː d l armwɛð]
„under the wardrobe‟
(268)
d.
[tɛz a l eikoul]
„was at school‟
(265)
„with England‟
(342)
(Liddicoat 1994)
As with the masculine definite article, I propose that the variation that occurs among the
feminine definite singular article allomorphs can be explained by the notion of sonority.
I expect all variation found among definite articles to be the result of sonority and I
discuss this analysis in Section 9.
8.3 Definite Plural Article Allomorphs – [lei], [lz], and [leiz]
As with the masculine and feminine definite singular articles discussed above, Liddicoat
(1994) states that the occurrence of the different definite plural allomorphs is dictated by
the phonological composition of the word following the article. Of the three occurring
forms, Liddicoat (1994) only discusses the environment for the allomorph, [lz], which he
states occurs prevocalically. Liddicoat (1994) does not actually list [leiz] as an allomorph
for the definite plural article, though this form was found in the data. This thesis
accounts for all the allomorphic variation found in Jersey definite articles, so [leiz] is
included.
I propose that the underlying morpheme for the definite plural is /lei/. /lei/ is chosen as
the underlying morpheme due to the fact that it occurs where, sonority-wise, other
51
allomorphs could but do not occur. The analysis in Section 9.3 provides additional
support for this argument. Also, /lei/ is morphologically distinct from the masculine and
feminine singular underlying morphemes, /l/ and /la/, and it is the most widely
distributed. Of the 34 plural definite allomorph examples I collected, 26 are examples of
[lei]
Since the other allomorphs given by Liddicoat (1994) are prevocalic [lei] must occur
before words which begin with consonants. The data support this, since all examples
found with [lei] occur prior to words that begin with consonants, examples of which are
shown in (38).
(38)
[lei] before C(C)
a.
[tu lei ʒur]
„everyday‟
(339)
b.
[dɑ̃ lei kjo]
„in the fields‟
(262)
c.
[swɔtr lei moto]
„after the cars‟
(272)
d.
[pur lei sjɛn]
„for the ones‟
(354)
(Liddicoat 1994)
For example, in (38c) the allomorph [lei] is found before the noun [moto] „cars,‟ which
begins with a consonant [m]. [lei] also occurs before consonant clusters, as is seen in
(38b) where the noun begins with the cluster [kj]. It appears that the allomorph [lei]
occurs before consonants and consonant clusters.
52
The other allomorph for the definite plural article discussed by Liddicoat (1994) is [lz],
which, according to Liddicoat, occurs prevocalically. Examples of this distribution are
given in (39). In (11d), where the word, [almɑ̃] „Germans‟ begins with the vowel [a], the
allomorph [lz] surfaces.
(39)
[lz] before V
a.
[pa lz ɑlmɑ̃ː]
„by the Germans‟
(382)
b.
[ei lz eip̃n]
„and the thorns‟
(361)
c.
[parmi lz ɑ̃ɡjei]
„among the English‟
(281)
d.
[paːskɛ lz almɑ̃]
„because the Germans‟
(343)
(Liddicoat 1994)
Now I turn to the allomorph that is not discussed by Liddicoat (1994), [leiz]. In
reviewing the data, I found three distinct occurrences of [leiz], which are shown below in
(40). The distribution of [leiz] seems to match that of [lz] (seen in (39)), with both
occurring prevocalically, yet upon closer examination, appears to have a different
distribution.
(40)
[leiz] before V and after C
a.
#[oprei k leiz ɑlmɑ̃]
„after the Germans‟
(205)
b.
[teim leiz ɑ̃ɡjei]
„we were English‟
(341)
c.
[k i n leiz ave]
„since they had seen‟ (354)
(Liddicoat 1994)
53
In (40b) the word following the article begins with a vowel, [ɑ̃] – [ɑ̃ɡjei], „English.‟ This
is similar to the environment seen above, in example (39c) [parmi lz ɑ̃ɡjei] „among the
English,‟ for the allomorph [lz]. Yet closer examination reveals that [leiz] surfaces only
if the word preceding the article ends in a consonant. In (40b), the word preceding the
allomorph [leiz] ends in a consonant, [m] - [teim], unlike (39c), where the word
preceding the article, [parmi] ends in a vowel, [i].
While the phonological environment for the allomorphs [leiz], [lz], and [lei] can help
predict which allomorph will occur, it does not explain nor account for the variation
among the allomorphs. As with the other definite articles discussed in this section,
sonority is a plausible underlying motivating factor influencing the distribution of the
plural definite article allomorphs.
8.4 Data Summary
I have shown in this section the allomorphic variation found among the definite articles in
Jersey. While Liddicoat (1994) states that the variations shown are predictable by
examining the phonological composition of the initial segment of the word following the
article, I have found data that do not conform to his predictions. I use sonority in the next
section to analyze, account for, and predict the variation found among Jersey definite
articles.
54
SECTION 9: SONORITY BASED ANALYSIS OF
DEFINITE ARTICLE ALLMORPHIC VARIATION
All of the definite articles in Jersey exhibit allomorphic variation, as shown in Section 5.
According to Liddicoat (1994) this variation is attributed to whether the word following
the article begins with a consonant or a vowel. The existence of data that are not in
accordance with the description given by Liddicoat (1994) indicates that it is not the
presence or absence of consonants word initially in the word following the article that is
responsible for this variation. Instead, I put forth in the following section an analysis
which uses sonority to account for this variation.
In the following sections I apply the Jersey Sonority Hierarchy from Section 7.2, along
with sonority and syllable based principles from Section 6, to analyze allomorphic
variation of Jersey definite articles. I also discuss additional reasons for choosing the
underlying morphemes discussed in Section 5.
9.1 Masculine Definite Singular Article Allomorphs – [l] and [le]
In Section 8 I posit that the underlying morpheme for the masculine definite singular is
/l/, with the two allomorphs being [l] and [le]. According to Liddicoat (1994) [le] is
found only when the word following the article begins with a consonant cluster. As I
discuss below, this is not always the case. Instead of a simple phonological rule, it is
sonority that dictates the usage of [l] and [le]. This explains the presence of data that do
not align with the rule presented by Liddicoat (1994).
55
(41)
Syllabification of [vɛː l ɡard̃] ‘see the garden’ (Liddicoat 1994: 169)
a.
[vɛːl.ɡar.d̃]
b.
*[vɛː.le.ɡar.d̃]
In the example in (41a), the allomorph [l] occurs since it can be syllabified into the
preceding coda of the first syllable [vɛːl]. It cannot be syllabified into the following onset
of [ɡar] for two main reasons. First, syllabifying [l] into the onset would violate the
Sonority Sequencing Principle since the phoneme [l] is higher in sonority than [ɡ] and the
Sonority Sequencing Principle dictates that onsets rise in sonority towards the nucleus of
the syllable without any intervening segments lower in sonority (Clements 1990).
Second, syllabification into the onset of [ɡar] would violate the Romance
Resyllabification Principle by creating a consonant cluster in the onset of the adjacent
word by syllabifying [l] across a word boundary. As seen in (41b), the occurrence of the
allomorph [le] would not result in any sonority violations, but since [l] is syllabified into
the coda of the preceding syllable, [vɛː], there is no need to use the allomorph [le]. This
is due to the fact that there is nothing to condition the usage of [le]. The allomorph [l]
surfaces since it abides by the principles of sonority.
56
As noted in the above section, the morpheme /l/ has two allomorphs. According to
Liddicoat [le] appears only in front of words that begin with consonant clusters. These
conditions are sonority based. In the following example (42a), the use of the allomorph
[le] is necessary in order to avoid ungrammatical syllabification that violates sonority
based principles, as seen in example (42b).
(42)
Syllabification of [ɑ̃lvɛːm le vrɛ] ‘took the seaweed’ (Liddicoat 1994: 286)
(a)
[ɑ̃l.vɛːm.le.vrɛ]
(b)
*[ɑ̃l.vɛːm.l.vrɛ]
In (42a), [le] forms its own syllable. If the allomorph [l] were to occur, as in (42b), then
there is no possible way to syllabify [l] into either the coda of the preceding syllable,
[vɛːm], or the onset of the following syllable, [vrɛ]. Both syllabifications are prohibited
by the Romance Resyllabification Principle, which prevents the creation of consonant
clusters, [ml] and [lvr], respectively, across word boundaries. Both possibilities are also
violations of the Sonority Sequencing Principle. In the first syllabification of [l] into
[vɛːm], [vɛːml], sonority would rise to [l] after falling to [m]. In the case of [lvrɛ],
sonority would drop from [l] to [v] and then rise again to [r], violating the Sonority
Sequencing Principle. Using the Law of Initials, [lvr] is also not allowed word initially
as an onset and therefore is not found word medially as an onset. By using the allomorph
57
[le], violations of the Sonority Sequencing Principle and the Romance Resyllabification
Principle are avoided, though the Syllable Contact Law is violated since [m] is lower in
sonority than [l].
The allomorph [l] occurs for the masculine singular definite article, as seen in (41) unless
it violates sonority. In environments where [l] violates sonority, the allomorph [le]
surfaces, as in (42).
An analysis using sonority to determine the allomorphic variation of articles is supported
by the occurrence in the data of forms that do not abide by the rules posited by Liddicoat
(1994). In (43) below, the allomorph [le] occurs before a word beginning with a single
consonant. According to the rule posited by Liddicoat (1994) [l] should occur since the
onset is not a consonant cluster. Due to sonority principles, such as the Sonority
Sequencing and Romance Resyllabification Principles, [le] occurs instead.
(43)
Syllabification of #[le ʒur], ‘the day’ (Liddicoat 1994: 339)
(a)
[le.ʒur]
(b)
58
*[lʒur] or *[l. ʒur]
If the predicted but non-occurring form occurs, as in (43b), there are two possible ways to
syllabify the phrase, both of which involve sonority violations. One way to syllabify
[l ʒur] is to syllabify [l] into the onset of the following word. This would cause violations
of the Sonority Sequencing Principle and the Romance Resyllabification Principle. The
Sonority Sequencing Principle would be violated by the rise then fall in sonority prior to
the rise of sonority to the nucleus since [l] is higher in sonority than [ʒ]. Also
syllabification would create the consonant cluster [lʒ] in the onset as a result of
syllabifying across word boundaries, which violates the Romance Resyllabification
Principle. The other way in which to syllabify this sequence would be to allow [l] to
become its own syllable. Since, according to Liddicoat (1994), syllabic consonants are
not allowed in Jersey, this is not possible.
While the use of the predicted form [l] creates ungrammatical syllabification in the above
example (43), this is not always the case. In (44b) the use of the form that is predicted by
Liddicoat (1994), but is actually non-occurring, does not create ungrammatical
syllabification. In (44a) the unpredicted but actually occurring form is syllabified.
(44)
Syllabification of [fɛ l brɑ̃kɑʒ] ‘done the ‘branchage’’ (Liddicoat 1994: 267)
(a)
[fɛl.brɑ̃.kɑʒ]
(b)
59
*[fɛ.le.brɑ̃.kɑʒ]
In (44a) [l] is syllabified into the coda of the first syllable, [fɛ]. This is acceptable since
there are no consonant clusters created, which would violate the Romance
Resyllabification Principle. In (44b) [le] is the allomorph that is predicted to occur here
as the word following the article begins with a consonant cluster, [br]. Since there is a
vowel to form the syllable nucleus, [l] is syllabified into the onset abiding by Minimal
Onset Satisfaction. Despite this acceptable syllabification, this form does not occur in the
data, instead the form in (44a) occurs. This lends support to the analysis that the
underlying morpheme is /l/, not /le/. In this case the conditions (sonority dictated) do not
exist so the allomorph [le] does not surface and instead the allomorph [l] occurs.
As seen in the examples in (41) through (44), the use of sonority based principles
explains which masculine definite singular allomorph occurs in Jersey. The allomorph
whose usage does not violate sonority is the allomorph that surfaces. When the use of [l]
creates a sonority violation, the allomorph [le] surfaces instead. Sonority explains and
predicts the variation seen in the masculine definite singular articles.
9.2 Feminine Definite Singular Article Allomorphs – [la] and [l]
As with the masculine definite singular article, the feminine definite singular article also
exhibits allomorphic variation that is motivated by sonority. According to Liddicoat
(1994), of the two allomorphs, [la] and [l], [l] occurs only before words that begin with
vowels, while [la] occurs before consonants. In examining the data below, I posit that the
allomorph [la] surfaces unless it violates sonority. When a sonority violation occurs
60
using the allomorph [la], the allomorph [l], whose usage is conditioned, occurs instead.
In (45) below, [la] occurs instead of [l], as there is nothing sonority-based to condition
the usage of [l].
(45)
Syllabification of [dvin la rep̃s] ‘guesses the answer’ (Liddicoat 1994: 212)
(a)
[dvin.la.re.p̃s]
(b)
*[dvin.lre.p̃s]
In (45) [la] must occur given the surrounding environment. In examining (45a), the coda
of the preceding word, [dvin], and the onset of the following word, [rep̃s], are composed
of consonants, [n] and [r] respectively. The Romance Resyllabification Principle
prevents a consonant, in this case [l], from being syllabified to form a consonant cluster,
which would be the case if the allomorph [l] occurred, as in (45b). The use of the
allomorph [la] instead of [l] allows for the formation of a syllable with [l] as the onset of
its own syllable. In (45b), [l] must be syllabified into either the coda of the first syllable,
forming the cluster [nl], or into the onset of the following syllable, forming the cluster
[lr]. Both of these violates the Romance Resyllabification Principle and [nl] violates the
Sonority Sequencing Principle in that [n] is lower in sonority than [l], which results in an
intervening segment of lower sonority between [l] and the syllable nucleus. Also, [lr] is
61
not found word initially in Jersey and [nl] is not found word finally, and therefore neither
are an allowable syllable onset or coda, respectively (as predicted by the Law of Initials
and Law of Finals).
The other allomorph of the feminine definite singular article is [l]. [l] occurs, within the
data I have reviewed, before words that begin with a vowel. The use of [l] is conditioned
by sonority based principles, specifically the Syllable Contact Law. (46a) is an example
of the occurring form, [l], while (46b) is an example of the non-occurring form, [la].
(46)
Syllabification of [tɛz a l eikoul] ‘was at school’ (Liddicoat 1994: 265)
(a)
[ tɛ.za.lei.koul]
(b)
*[tɛ.za.la.ei.koul]
In (46a), in order to satisfy the Minimal Onset Satisfaction Principle, [l] is syllabified into
the onset of the third syllable. If the non-occurring form surfaces, as in (46b) there is a
sonority plateau at the edges of the third and fourth syllables, [.la.ei.]. This is a violation
of the Syllable Contact Law. According to the Syllable Contact Law it is preferred at
syllable margins that the segment at the edge of the preceding syllable be higher in
sonority than the segment it is adjacent to in the following syllable. By using the
allomorph [l], this sonority plateau and the violation of the Syllable Contact are avoided.
62
Using principles of sonority, I can identify the sonority based conditions that trigger the
variation of the feminine definite singular article allomorphs.
9.3 Definite Plural Article Allomorphs – [lei], [lz], and [leiz]
As mentioned above, the definite plural article allomorphs are [lei], [lz], and [leiz], with
the underlying morpheme being /lei/. According to Liddicoat (1994) the choice of which
definite plural article allomorph surfaces is determined, as with other articles, by the
presence or absence of word initial consonants of the word following the article. As
discussed in Section 5.3, the definite plural allomorphs are in complementary
distribution. [lz] and [leiz] occur before words whose initial syllable lacks an onset, yet
[lz] occurs only after words whose final syllable is an open syllable. The allomorph [lei]
occurs before words with codas. This distribution is motivated by sonority.
As noted above the underlying morpheme is /lei/. The fact that [lei] occurs where other
allomorphs can also occur, as in (47c), supports the selection of /lei/ as the underlying
morpheme. In the examples below, only [lei] occurs, as seen in (47a), while [lz] and
[leiz] do not, as seen in (47b) and (47c).
63
(47)
Syllabification of [dɑ̃ lei kjo], ‘in the fields’ (Liddicoat 1994: 262)
(a)
[dɑ̃.lei.kjo]
(b)
*[dɑ̃lz.kjo]
(c)
*[dɑ̃.leiz.kjo]
In the form that occurs in the data, (47a), [lei] is syllabified into its own syllable. [l] is
syllabified into the onset position of the second syllable in order to satisfy the Minimal
Onset Satisfaction Principle and the Maximal Onset Realisation Parameter. In this
example the syllable boundaries are identical to the word boundaries. If the allomorph
[leiz] occurs, as in example (47c), it is theoretically acceptable because it does not violate
sonority, but it is not the form which occurs in the data. The allomorph [lz] cannot occur
since syllabifying [lz] into the coda of the preceding syllable would violate the Romance
Resyllabification Principle by syllabifying a consonant cluster across word edges. The
usage of the allomorphs [leiz] and [lz] is conditioned by sonority and since the
conditions, which are seen in (48) and (49), are not present, the allomorph [lei] surfaces.
While Liddicoat (1994) states that [lz] surfaces when the word following the article
begins with a vowel, this analysis posits that usage is conditioned by sonority based
principles, such as the Sonority Sequencing Principle and the Syllable Contact Law.
These principles dictate the use of allomorph [lz] in example (48a) below.
64
(48)
Syllabification of [parmi lz ɑ̃ɡjei], ‘among the English’ (Liddicoat 1994: 281)
(a)
[par.mil.zɑ̃.ɡjei]
(c)
*[par.mi.lei.zɑ̃.ɡjei]
(b)
*[par.mi.lei.ɑ̃.ɡjei]
If the other allomorph [lei] occurs, (48b), a sonority plateau is created by the vowels at
the syllable boundary of syllables three and four [.lei.ɑ̃.]. As noted above in Section 9.2,
the creation of sonority plateaus is dispreferred as it violates the Syllable Contact Law.
The Syllable Contact Law creates conditions in which the allomorph [lei] cannot surface.
In (48c), the non-occurring allomorph [leiz] surfaces. This does not result in any sonority
violations since [l] can be syllabified into the onset of the third syllable and [z] can be
syllabified into the onset of the penultimate syllable without creating any sonority
violations. Yet this form is not the one found in the data. The conditions that result in the
occurrence of the allomorph [leiz] are not found in this data. These conditions are seen
65
in the examples shown in (49). As these conditions are not present, the allomorph [lz] is
used, as seen in (48a), without any sonority violations.
Unlike the other two allomorphs [lei] and [lz], the allomorph [leiz] is not mentioned in
Liddicoat‟s description of definite plural articles. This form occurs in the data, yet is not
discussed by Liddicoat. The allomorph [leiz] has the most restricted usage of the three
definite plural allomorphs, which is seen in the fact that there are only three occurrences
of [leiz] in the data. In order for the allomorph [leiz] to appear the occurrence of either of
the other two allomorphs [lei] and [lz] must cause violations of sonority. This is seen in
(49), where the use of either [lei], as in (49b) or [lz], as in (49a), causes violations of
sonority. In (49c) the attested form is used with no sonority violations occurring.
66
(49)
Syllabification of [oprei k leiz ɑlmɑ̃] ‘after the Germans’ (Liddicoat 1994:
205)
(a)
*[o.preik.l.zɑl.mɑ̃]
(c)
[o.preik.lei.zɑl.mɑ̃]
(b)
*[o.preik.lei. ɑl.mɑ̃]
In (49a) the allomorph predicted by Liddicoat (1994) to occur is [lz], as the word
following the article begins with a vowel. The use of this allomorph results in a
violation of the Sonority Sequencing Principle and the Romance Resyllabification
Principle. If [l] is syllabified into the onset of the preceding syllable, there is a fall then
rise in the sonority profile violating the Sonority Sequencing Principle. The occurrence of
[lz] also violates the Romance Resyllabification Principle. This is due to the fact that
syllabifying either [l] or [lz] into the coda of [preik] creates consonant clusters, [kl] or
[klz], through syllabification across word boundaries. Syllabifying just [z] into the
67
following onset would be acceptable, but that leaves [l] as its own syllable. [l] cannot be
syllabified as a syllable nucleus as vowels are the only phoneme allowed to form a
syllable nucleus in Jersey.
In (49b) the other non-occurring allomorph [lei] is found. In this case, the syllabification
does not produce any unallowable consonant clusters or improper syllable nuclei, but it
does violate the Syllable Contact Law in that a sonority plateau is created by the two
adjacent vowels of third and fourth syllables. As discuss above, syllable plateaus are not
allowed in Jersey. The sonority based conditions are such that the only allomorph that
can occur is the most restricted allomorph, [leiz]. This is seen in example (49c) where
the sonority based principles put forth herein are abided by. In this example, [lei] forms
its own syllable, of the preferred type CV. The [z] of the allomorph is then syllabified
into the onset of the following syllable, to create the syllable [zɑl].
Sonority creates conditioning environments in Jersey that result in the allomorphic
variation of the plural definite article, and other definite articles. By using sonority based
principles I account for the variation found and predict which allomorph will surface in a
phrase.
9.4 Analysis Summary
By using sonority based principles, I have explained the allomorphic variation of the
definite articles in Jersey. The principles utilized in this thesis account for the variation
described by Liddicoat (1994). Sonority can also account for the distribution seen in
68
Section 9.1 of the singular masculine definite article allomorphs that pattern in direct
opposition to the descriptions given by Liddicoat (1994). The occurrence and
distribution of the undescribed plural allomorph, [leiz], can be explained by using
principles of sonority. The Jersey Sonority Hierarchy allows the ranking and thus
syllabification of Jersey phrases according to sonority principles.
69
SECTION 10: CONCLUSION
The surfacing of the definite article allomorphs in Jersey is conditioned by sonority. This
thesis presented data that exhibit allomorphic variation. The majority of the data is
consistent with the patterns discussed in Liddicoat‟s (1994) grammar. There is a portion
of the data, specifically the masculine definite article allomorphs, that is inconsistent with
the description. There are also data that contain an allomorph not described by the
grammar - the plural definite article allomorph [leiz]. In order to account for the data I
posited that sonority based principles were constraining the variation. To assist in the
analysis I posited a Jersey-based sonority hierarchy. The Jersey Sonority Hierarchy is
based on the Universal Sonority Hierarchy, the Gallo-Romance Sonority Hierarchy, and
Jersey phonotactics. Through syllabification of phrases and the application of sonority
based principles, such as the Sonority Sequencing Principle and the Syllable Contact
Law, I am able to explain and predict the allomorphic variation seen among the definite
articles in Jersey.
This thesis makes several contributions to the field of linguistics. This thesis contributes
to the general fields of linguistics and brings up a variety of issues that warrant further
discussion. First this thesis contributes to Jersey linguistics by providing a theoretical
analysis of morphological processes in Jersey because the primary linguistic work in
Jersey has been in the field of sociolinguistics. This thesis proposed a language specific
sonority hierarchy, which theoretically analyzes the sonority of phonemes and accounts
for possible syllabification.
70
Second, The Jersey Sonority Hierarchy has shown that the Universal Sonority Hierarchy
and language specific hierarchies are not in direct opposition to each other. The Jersey
sonority hierarchy was proposed based on the Universal Sonority Hierarchy, which is
assumed to be applicable cross-linguistically. Other language specific hierarchies
mentioned in this thesis (Dell and Elmedlaoui 1985, Elfner 2005, Hankamer and Aissen
1974, Jacobs 1992, Miyashita 2003, Parker 2000, Siptár and Tӧrkenczy 2000) do not
contradict the Universal Sonority Hierarchy as well. The fact that language specific
sonority hierarchies do not violate the Universal Sonority Hierarchy suggests that the
Universal Sonority Hierarchy is merely a starting point from which to develop a language
specific sonority hierarchy. Language specific sonority hierarchies take categories found
in the Universal Sonority Hierarchy and delineate the categories into more concise
rankings that can account for a language‟s phonotactics.
Third, this thesis brings up the question of the true nature of allomorphy. Early
phonology and morphology posited derivational rules to account for phonological
differences between two forms of the same morpheme. In this thesis, the insertion of /ə/
was introduced as a good example. Liddicoat (1994) describes this as derivational rule of
epenthesis. There are certain consonant clusters that Liddicoat (1994) states surface as
CəC but occur in the data as CC. For example, the combination /dv/ is described as
surfacing as [dəv] yet the examples in (36) and (45) the word [dvin] occurs. The form of
the definite article allomorphs has a similar pattern. The definite article allomorphs in
Jersey seem to exhibit a degree of phonological epenthesis or deletion. The underlying
morpheme posited for the masculine definite article singular is /l/, with the allomorphs
71
being [l] and [le]. The formation of the allomorph [le] from /l/ involves the epenthesis of
the vowel [e]. The exact opposite is seen in the feminine definite singular article, which
in this case is the underlying /la/ and allomorphs [la] and [l]. To form the allomorph [l]
from /la/, the vowel [a] is deleted. Epenthesis and deletion rules seem to account for the
variation seen among these allomorphs and their underlying morphemes. This analysis is
complicated when the plural definite articles are examined. The underlying morpheme
for the plural definite articles is /lei/ and the allomorphs are [lei], [lz], and [leiz]. The
only similarity between /lei/ and [lz] is [l]. The phonological rules needed to obtain the
allomorphs from the underlying morpheme now become more complicated. For example
to obtain [lz], [z] must be inserted at the end of [lei] and then the diphthong, [ei] must be
deleted. The simple derivational rules become complicated, multi-step procedures. The
proposed analysis, in the terms of the Sonority Sequencing Principle, explains why these
insertions and deletions occur, which these rules only provide context and no motivation.
Fourth, this thesis prepares an analysis of the allomorphic variation in the framework of
Optimality Theory (OT) (Prince and Smolensky 1993, McCarthy and Prince 1993). OT
uses ranked constraints to evaluate and select the optimal output candidate. Under the
framework of OT, an optimal output candidate can violate constraints if the constraint is
low in ranking. This property explains the occurrence of an allomorph in violation of a
sonority based principle (i. e. the occurring form violates the Syllable Contact Law, but is
still the form that occurs in the data. The other form, which does not occur, violates
several sonority principles). This does not complicate the analysis, but simply points to
the possible benefit of using OT to further the analysis. Faithfulness constraints found in
72
OT would also be beneficial to this analysis as they could account for the variation seen
among the plural definite article allomorphs. The use of OT to further analyze the data
presented in this thesis may provide valuable insight into the variation.
Fifth, this thesis brings up an attention to the notion of extrasyllabicity with respect to the
distribution of the phonemes /s/ and /r/ in Jersey. As noted by others, Clements (1990),
Siptár and Tӧrkenczy (2000), Parker (2002), and Denzer-King (2009), the phoneme /s/
behaves differently than other fricatives. The /s/+Stop consonant cluster sequence in
syllable onsets violates the Sonority Sequencing Principle, yet occurs in many languages,
including Jersey. In Jersey, /s/ can combine to form consonant clusters in onsets that
violate sonority, such as /st/- /stɑːl/ „stall,‟ /sp/- /spaðɛ/ „to separate‟, /sk/ - /skabɛ/
„stool.‟ The only one that is an issue is the /st/ sequence, since, according to Liddicoat
(1994) the /sp/ and /sk/ sequence are realized as [səp] and [sək]. Extrasyllabicity has
been used to account for the /st/ sequence (Selkirk (1995), Kiparsky (2003), Denzer-King
(2009)). Extrasyllabic segments are believed to be parsed into a category higher than the
syllable. Extrasyllabic /s/ in Jersey is a possibility and warrants more research.
Similarly, the distribution of the phoneme /r/ in Jersey poses problems for both the
Universal Sonority Hierarchy and the Jersey Sonority Hierarchy. /r/+Stop sequences,
such as the onsets /rɡ/ - /rɡardɛ/ „to look,‟ and /rk/ - /rkuvri/, „to recover,‟ violate the
Sonority Sequencing Principle. Selkirk (1995) proposes treating /r/ in sequences such as
this as extrasyllabic, similar to the treatment of /s/ above. This a possible solution to the
problem of /r/+Stop onset sequences.
73
The proposed Jersey specific sonority hierarchy used may have an additional application.
Allomorphic variation is found in many parts of speech in Jersey, such as among
conditionals, pronouns, negation morphemes, and prepositions. Application of the Jersey
Sonority Hierarchy to phrases containing parts of speech that exhibit allomorphic
variation may yield an analysis that can predict these variations. In addition to
allomorphic variation, the Jersey Sonority Hierarchy can also simplify further sonority
based analysis by reducing the number of rules needed or with OT.
Furthermore, the Law of Initials and the Law of Finals are used in this thesis to determine
allowable onsets and codas in Jersey. The composition of onsets and codas abide, for the
most part, by the Sonority Sequencing Principle. Using this principle in conjunction with
the Jersey Sonority Hierarchy can eliminate the need for the two laws mentioned above.
Excepting the cases mentioned in the above paragraph (/s/+Stop, /r/+Stop), the onsets and
codas should abide by the Sonority Sequencing Principle, with rankings based on the
Jersey Sonority Hierarchy. The possible applications of the Jersey Sonority Hierarchy
should be explored.
Finally, one of the basic underlying assumptions of sonority is that the sonority is an
inherent property of a sound that is physical and can be measured. The property to which
sonority is often linked is amplitude (Blevins 1996). If any of the physical properties of a
sound segment change over time, including amplitude, it follows that the sonority index
of the sound should also change. In Jersey, as in all languages, certain sounds have
changed overtime. One of interest to this thesis is the evolution of intervocalic /z/ from
74
/r/. The occurrence of /z/ intervocalically does not cause sonority violations as it can
occupy the onset of the second vowel‟s syllable or the coda of the first vowel‟s syllable.
What it does bring up is the question of how does historical sound change affect
sonority? Historical change may be able to provide insight into the occurrence of
consonant clusters that violate the Sonority Sequencing Principle. A consonant cluster
that once abided by this principle may change over time to create a cluster that now
violates the principle. Sonority based analysis may need to include and account for the
effect of historical sound change.
In conclusion, the implications of this thesis are broad and warrant additional time and
research. The true nature of allomorphy, whether allomorphic differences are merely the
result of phonological processes or are simply separate individual mental representations,
is one issue that falls out from this thesis. Another implication is the analysis of sound
sequences that violate sonority. These sequences may be the result of historical sounds
change and while that may explain why these sequences occur, it does not analyze the
occurrence. This phenomenon could be analyzed using OT or the theory of
extrasyllabicity. This thesis is important to the field of linguistics as it puts for a theorybased analysis of described and undescribed data and successfully accounts of the
variation found among the data. This thesis is also important in that it raises a number of
implications from which further research may be arise.
75
REFERENCES
Birt, Paul. W. 1985. Le Jerriais Pour Tous, a Complete Course on the Jersey Language.
Jersey: Le Don Balleine.
Blevins, Juliette. 1996. The syllable in phonological theory. The Handbook of
Phonological Theory, ed. by J. A. Goldsmith, 206-244. Cambridge: Blackwell.
Brasseur, Patrice. 1977. Le Franҫais dans les îles anglo-normandes. Travaux de
Linguistique et de Litterature 16.97-104.
Brasseur, Patrice. 1978a. Les principales caracteristiques phonetiques des parlers
normands de Jersey, Sercq, Guernesey et Magneville (canton de Bricquebec,
Manche), premíre partie. Annales de Normandie 25/1.49-64.
Brasseur, Patrice. 1978b. Les principales caracteristiques phonetiques des parlers
Normands de Jersey, Sercq, Guernesey et Magneville (canton de Bricquebec,
Manche), premíre partie. Annales de Normandie 25/3.275-306.
Carinaletti, Anna. and Lori Repetti. 2009. Phrase-level and word-level syllables:
resyllabification and prosodization of clitics. Phonological Domains: Universals
and Deviations, ed. by J. Grijzenhout & B. Kabak, 79-104. Berlin: Mouton de
Gruyter.
Carŕ, Alabert L. 1972. English-Jersey Vocabulary. Jersey: Don Balleine.
Chambers, J. K. and Peter Trudgill. 2002. Dialectology. Cambridge: Cambridge
University Press.
Clements. G. N. 1990. The role of the sonority cycle in core syllabification. Papers in
Laboratory Phonology I: Between the Grammar and Physics of Speech, ed. by J.
Kingston and M. E. Beckman, 283-333. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
76
Clements, G. N. and S. J. Keyser. 1981. A three-tiered theory of the syllable. Occasional
Paper No. 19. Center for Cognitive Science, MIT.
Clements, G.N. and S. J. Keyser. 1983. CV Phonology: A Generative Theory of the
Syllable. Cambridge: MIT Press.
Collas, J. P. nd. Les Parlers Jersiais. Unpublished Manuscript.
Crystal, David. 2003. A Dictionary of Linguistics and Phonetics. Cambridge, MA:
Blackwell.
Dell, François and Mohamed Elmedlaoui. 1985. Syllabic consonants and syllabification
in Imdlawn Tashlhiyt Berber. Journal of African Languages and Linguistics
7.105-130.
Dell, François and Mohamed Elmedlaoui. 1988. Syllabic consonants in Berber: some
new evidence. Journal of African Languages and Linguistics 10.1-17.
Denzer-King, Ryan. 2009. The Distribution of /s/ in Blackfoot: an optimality theory
account. Missoula, MT: University of Montana thesis.
Duanmu, San. 2008. Syllable structure: the limits of variation. New York: Oxford
University Press.
Elfner, Emily. 2005. The role of sonority in Blackfoot phonotactics. Calgary Papers in
Linguistics 26.27-91.
Goldsmith, John. 1996. Phonological theory. The Handbook of Phonological Theory, ed.
by J. A. Goldsmith, 206-244. Cambridge: Blackwell.
Gordon, Raymond G., Jr. (ed.) 2005. Ethnologue: Languages of the World, 15th edition.
Dallas: SIL International. Online at: http://www.ethnologue.com/.
77
Hankamer, Jorge and Judith Aissen. 1974. The sonority hierarchy. Papers from CLS 28,
ed. by M. W. LaGaly, R. A. Fox, and A. Bruck, 206-223. Chicago; CLS.
Jacobs, Haike. 1992. The interaction between syllable structure and foot structure in the
evolution from Classical Latin to Old French. Theoretical Analysis in Romance
Linguistics, Selected Papers from the Nineteenth Linguistic Symposium on
Romance Languages, ed. by C. Laeufer and T. A. Morgan, 55-79.
Amsterdam/Philadelphia: John Benjamins Publishing Company.
Jespersen, Otto. 1904. Lehrbuch der phonetik. Leipzig and Berlin: B. G. Teubner.
Jones, Mari C. 2001. Jersey Norman French: A Linguistic Study of an Obsolescent
Dialect. Oxford: Blackwell.
Jones, Mari C. 2003. Jèrriais: Jersey's Native Tongue. Jersey: Le Don Balleine.
Jones, Mari C. 2005a. Some structural and social correlates of single word intrasentential
codeswitching in Jersey Norman French. Journal of French Language Studies
15/1.1-23.
Jones, Mari C. 2005b. Transfer and changing linguistic norms in Jersey Norman French.
Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 8/2.159-175.
Jones, Mari C. 2007. Channel Island French. Language in the British Isles, ed. by D.
Britain, 358-367. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Kiparsky, Paul. 2003. Syllables and moras in Arabic. The Syllable in Optimality Theory
ed. by C. Féry and R. van de Vijver, 147-182. Cambridge: Cambridge University
Press.
La Societe Jersiaise. Online at: http://members.societejersiaise.org/sdllj/articlyes2000.html
78
Le Feuvre, George. F. 1976. Histouaithes et Gens d’Jerri. Jersey: Don Balleine.
Le Feuvre, George. F. 1979. Des viers mots Jerriais. Chroniques du Don Balleine 1.1213.
Le Feuvre, George. F. 1983. J̀rri Jadis. Jersey: Don Balleine.
Le Maistre, Frank. 1966. Dictionnaire Jersiais-Franҫais. Jersey: Don Balleine.
Le Maistre, Frank. 1979a. The Jersey Language. Jersey: Don Balleine.
Le Maistre, Frank. 1979b. The Jersey Language. Jersey: Don Balleine.
Le Maistre, Frank. 1981. Ĺ J̀rriais – chíthe vielle langue que j‟laîssons couôrre!
Chroniques du Don Balleine 4.91-4.
Le Maistre, Frank. 1993. The Jersey Language. Jersey: Don Balleine.
Liddicoat, Anthony. 1990. Some structural features of language obsolescence in the
dialect of Jersey. Language Sciences 12/2-3.197-208.
Liddicoat, Anthony. 1991. The function of pronoun order in imperative verb phrases in
the dialects of Jersey and Sark Linguistische Berichte 131.24-35.
Liddicoat, Anthony. 1994. A Grammar of the Norman French of the Channel Islands; the
Dialects of Jersey and Sark. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.
McCarthy, John and Alan Prince. 1993. Prosodic morphology I, constraint interaction
and satsifacation. MS, Univesity of Massachusettes, Amherst, MA and Rutgers
University, New Brunswick, NJ.
Miyashita, Mizuki. 2003. Consonant clusters in Tohono O‟Odham. Working Papers in
Endangered and Less Familiar Languages, Studies in Uto-Aztecan 5.41-53.
Mohanan, K. P. 1991. On the bases of radical under specification. Natural Language and
Linguistic Theory 9.285-325.
79
Parker, Stephen G. 2002. Quantifying the sonority hierarchy. Amherst, MA: University
of Massachusetts dissertation.
Prince, Alan and Paul Smolensky. 1993. Optimality theory: constraint interacation in
generative grammar. MS, Rutgers Unviersity and University of Colorado,
Boulder.
Roca, Iggy. 1994. Generative phonology. New York: Routledge.
Schane, Sanford A. 1994. Diphthongization in particle phonology. The Handbook of
Phonological Theory, ed. by J. A. Goldsmith, 586-608. Cambridge: Blackwell.
Selkirk, Elisabeth. 1984. On the major class features and syllable theory. Language
Sound Structure, Studies in Phonology Present to Morris Halle by his Teacher
and Students, ed. by M. Aronoff and R. T. Oehrle, 107-136. Cambridge: MIT
Press.
Selkirk, Elisabeth. 1995. The prosodic structure of function words. Papers in Optimality
Theory: University of Massachusetts Occasional Papers, Volume 18, ed. by J. N.
Beckman et al., 439-469. Amherst: GLSA, University of Massachusetts.
Siptár, Péter and Miklós Tӧrkenczy. 2000. The phonology of Hungarian. New York:
Oxford University Press.
Spence, N. C. W. 1960. A Glossary of Jersey-French. Oxford: The Philological Society.
Spence, N. C. W. 1985. Phonologie descriptive des parlers Jersiais: I. Les voyelles.
Revue de Linguistique Romane 49.151-65.
Spence, N. C. W. 1987. Phonologie descriptive des parlers Jersiais: II. Les consonnes.
Revue de Linguistique Romane 51.119-33.
80
Vennemann, Theo. 1988. Preference Laws for Syllable Structure and the Explanation of
Sound Change: with Special Reference to German, Germanic, Italian, and Latin.
Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.
81