PAPERS AND STUDIES IN CONTRASTIVE LINGUISTICS, Vol. XXIV P L
ADAM MICKIEWICZ UNIVERSITY PRESS • POZNAN
ISSN
01372459
A PARAMETER OF SYLLABIFICATION*
ROLAND NOSKE
Univertity of Amsterdam
0. Introduction
In the litcrature concerning the syJIable (e.g., Kalm 1976), essentially one
principle of assignment of syllable structure has been advanced: an algorithm
which links elements to syllabic nodcs in the following order:
a. every vowel;
b. a maximal number of consonants preceding every vowel; these eonso
nants must form a permissible wordinitial cluster;
c. a maximal number of consonants following the vowel; these consonants
must form a permissible wordfinal cluster.
Although there exists, according to this principle, a welldeJined order of appli
oation within the syllable, nothing is said about the way the algorithm applies
to a larger order of segments (e.g. a prosodie unit). Apparently, all the vowels
are linked first, then the consonants which are going to form the onsets, and
finally the codas. Thus, within a prosodie group which constitutes a domain
for syllabification, all syllables are assigned simultaneously. This is why we
wish to call this type of syllabification non-directional.
However, Kaye and Lowenstamm (1981:30611) propose another principle:
according to them, syllabification is directional and can take place from lelt
to right in some languages, and from right to left in others. The principle of
* We wish to thank Norval Smith for much valuable discussion, as well as for a num
ber of corrections to our English. This work was supported by tho Stichting Taalwetenschap, which is funded by the Netherlands Organisation for the Advancement of Pure
Soientific Research (ZWO).
48
R. Noske
ayllabifieation which they advance is given (1) and (2) (1981:3079):
(1) The Rightward Strategy
Scanning a word frora left to right, make t h e first syllable as unmarked as
possible. If the resulting syllable on the right conforms t o the formal and
substantive constraints of the Ianguage, then there is a syllable boundary a t
t h a t point. If the resulting right syllable violates a eonstraint, move the
syllable boundary over one segment to the right and try again. Repeat
until the resulting right syllable is Heit. Alter the first syllable boundary
has been found, repeat t h e process for each successive syllable until the
end of the string is reached.
(2) The Leftward Strategy
Scanning a word from right to left, make t h e last syllable as unmarked as
possible. If the resulting syllable on the left conforms to the formal and
substantive constraints of the Ianguage, then there is a syllable boundary
a t theflpoint. If the resulting left syllable violates a eonstraint, move the
syllable boundary over one segment to t h e left and try again. Repeat
until t h e resulting left syllable is licit. After the first syllable boundary
has been found, repeat t h e process for each successive syllable until the
beginning of the string is reached.
We refer the reader to K a y e and Lowenstamm (1981) for an explanation of
their concept of markedness. I t will suffice here to mention the concrete exam
ples mentioned by the authors as motivation for their theory. As a Ianguage
using the Rightward Strategy they mention English of which they give the
following examples:
(3) a. command
b . canteen
c. astute
kamaend
ksentiyn
astuwt
Although in English the segmeuts or clusters of segments m, nt, st are per
missible wordfinally, the syllable boundaries are not located after these seg
ments or clusters of segments b u t at the first possible place (going from left
t o right) (provided it is possible to syllabify the whole word, which is the reason
t h a t in (3b), the syllable boundary is placed after the n). I n (3b, j ) the syllable
initial position of the dental piosives can be proved by the fact t h a t they are
aspirated.
Aa a Ianguage which uses the Leftward Strategy Kay and Lowenstamm men
tion Polish. Speakers of Polish generally agree t h a t the syllabification of words
Jike wyspa 'island', ospa 'smallpox', tykwa 'pumpkin', stacja 'station' is as in (4):
(4) wyspa ospa tykwa stacja
A parameier of eyUabifloation
49
This syllabification can be explained if the Leftward Strategy (2) is adopted.
The Rightward Strategy (1) would give rise to syllabifications like *wy-spa,
*ty-kwa, etc.
Independently of the proposals b y K a y e a n d L o w e n s t a m m , T e r Mors (1982)
proposes a directional syllabification for Klamath, a Penutian language of
Oregon. W h a t is interesting liere is t h a t the direction of syllabification can be
demonstrated in terms of epenthesis site.
I n Klamath, an epenthesis process applies in order to resolve unsyllabifiable
consonant clusters. Consider the underlying form in (5):
(5) /snogwk/
In this form, the sequence gwk is not a permissible wordfinal cluster. The
syllable template of K l a m a t h is given in (6):
(6) syllable template for Klamath
(O c v (C(O)
[—son]
[—sou]
I n order t o make the sequence syllabifiable ,there are two conceivable epenthe
sis sites: between g and w, and between w et k. I n fact the process of epenthesis
insert the schwa between g and w, creating the form snogdivk. After having
undergone an independently motivated vocalisation process, which changes
dw t o o: the form surfaces as (7):
(7) [snogo:k]
The form in (8), which would result if epenthesis were to take place between
k, is ungrammatical.
(8) *[snogwak]
As in m a n y analyses concerning epenthesis, Ter Mors conceives of this process
as the creation of a V on the level of the skeleton. This empty V is then filled
u p with t h e neutral vowel by a rule (or. according to the proposals of Archan
geli (1984), by a complement rule):
(9)
V
The V on the skeletal level is assumed to have been inserted by the process
of syüabification proper. I t is thus t h a t the reiationship between epenthesis
50
R. Noske
a n d syllabic structure is estabUshed (recall t h a t epenthesis functions in order
to resolve otherwise unsyllabifiable sequences). The following syllabification
mechanism is proposed b y Ter Mors (1985:316):
(10) Ruk of ASSOCIATION:
Map in a R h [ = r i g h t t o l e f t , R N fashin
t h e syllable t e m p l a t e onto t h e segmental trier; maximize clusters.
Because of the directionality, the following structure is created for /snogwk/;
(11)
a
a
C C V
C V C C
s n o
g
w
k
C
C
First, | and | are syllabified. Then the syllabification mechanism expects
k
w
a V. Because of the fact t h a t the element which it then encounters is not
a V, a V is inserted:
(12)
o
A\
V C C
w k
If one were to apply the wellknown Maximal Onset Principle (incorporated
in the proposal by K a h n ) , one would derive erroneously: 1
(13)
a
a
C C V
C C V c
s n o
g w
k
which, after application of (9) would produce the ungrammatical form in (8).
We would get the same result if we were to adopt a lefttoright syllabification.
H a ving outlined the essence of the proposal by Ter Mors, we will now show t h a t
the principle of directional syllabification accounts for certain processes in
Yawelmani. We will see t h a t the principle of directional syllabification re
1
I n fact, this is not completely true. Strictly speaking, the maximal onset principle
would fail to syllabify t h e second syllable altogether: in order to make an onset maximal
it should be able to refer to the nucleus. Since there is no V to perform this function, the
proeesa of syllabification would be blocked.
A pa?'ameter of syllabification
51
ceives additional motivation because it explains certain syllabic processes other
thaii epenthesis. Then, applying the principle to a third language, Tigrinya,.
we will see t h a t the direction of syllabification is parametrised.
1. The syllabic processes in
Yawelmani
Yawelmani, also a Penutian language (but not particularly closely related
to Klamath), spoken in South Central California, has attracted a lot of atten
tion from linguists. We mention here only the theses of Kuroda (1967), Kisse
berth (1969) and Archangeli (1984). The main source is Newman (1944). Se
veral processes affecting syllable structure take place in this language. The ana
lysis we present here can be found in a more extended form in ÏSToske (1985).
Yawelmani has three types of syllable: CV, CVC and CV: (0V| V t ). Like Kla
math, Yawelmani has an epenthesis process which serves to "repair" the syl
labic structure. This is why we propose t h a t in a certain phase of the deriva
tion the nucleus 2 can be empty, into which vowels will be epenthesised at
a later stage. These are the wellformedness conditions for the onset and t h e
rhyme in Yawelmani:
(14) possible Yawelmani onset
/
O
syllabic structure
C
skeleton
c
consonantal segmental tier
(15) possible Yawelmani rhymes
b.
R
/N^
N
V
V
v
2
.
> syllabic structure
Cd J
skeleton
vocalic segmental tier
consonantal segmental tier
We assume here the wellknown onset-rhyme bipartition, with the rhyme being
subdivided into nucleus-coda, for reasons that are outside the scope of this article, but
which are given in Noske (1985).
52
R. Noske
I n (15b) the coda is present, b u t obligatorily empty. The reasons for this do
not concern us here, b u t are given in Noske (1985).
We purpose the following syllabification principles for Yawelmani (cf. Noske
1985:347):
16) Syllabification principles for Yawelmani
syllable structures are mapped onto the skeleton
a. from right to left
b . in such a way t h a t the number of empty syllabic nodes is mini
mised.
There are three reasons for the directionality. The first two concern the
rules of Shortening and Elision postulated for Yawelmani b y Kuroda (1967)
a n d Kisserbeth (1969):
(17) Shortening: V >■ [—long] /
(18) Elision: V 0 /
V
i
#
The functioning of Shortening can be seen in (19):
(19) / t a x a a + t / [taxat]
taxaa-, 'bring'; -t, passive aorist
The functioning of Elision can be seen in (20):
(20) / l a g a a + i n + h i n / [laginhin] lagaa- 'spend the night'; -in-, mediopassive,
-hn9 aorist
Applying the syllabic wellformedness conditions given in (10, 11) as well as
the syllabification principles in (16), one obtains the syllabic structure in (21):
(21)
0
/
O
\
R
/ \
N Cd
C V V
c
\/ I
v
c
I
c
We now see t h a t the process of shortening follows auxomatically from the re
trograde character of syllabification: the mechanism will try to syllabify ac
cording to the templates in (15a, b). Because of the fact t h a t it encounters
a C, it will select (15a). Then, (continuing to go from right to left) it encounters
the rightmost V, and links this to the nucleus node. Now a second V is en
A parameter of syllabiflcation
53
eountered. Because of the faet t h a t (15a) allows only one V t o be linked to the
syllabic structure, this V is then skipped and is therefore not realised, since we
assume t h a t in order to be pronounced an element on the CVtier has to be
linked to the syllabic structure.
We obtain a similar result to t h a t in (21) with vowel elision in the case of a
closed syllable. Cf. (22):
(22)
a
f
!
/\
N Cd
1!
G V V C
j
v
v
i
I n (22), the retrograde syllabiflcation links only the rightmost V to the sylla
bic structure, thus the V to its left is not realised, which is the correct result.
For an open syllable, it is the requirement t h a t the two contiguous V s be lin
ked to a single element on the vocalic segmental tier t h a t rules out the form
in (23):
(23)
R
O
/\
N Cd
/ \
c
V
V
We thus see t h a t two processes in Yawelmani which up till now have had to
be explictly stated in terms of rules, follow from the assumption of a directio
nal syllabification mechanism. We now come to a third reason for the di
rectionality. Consider the foUowing two rules, proposed by Kuroda (1967) and
Kisseberth (1969):
(24) Epenthesis:
0 » i /
o{*o
B . Koske
54
(25) Two Sided Open Syllable Deletion (Kuroda 1967:32):
V
[long]
0/VC
CV
The derivations in (26) ülustrate t h e functioninf og these two rules:
(26) paH-, 'fight'; Tm, aorist; -t, passive aorist
a. p a ? t + h n
pa2iti+hin
pa?it+hin
pa^ithin
b. p a ^ t + t
pa?it+it
pa^it+t
pa?tit
Underlying Representation
Epentiesis (24)
Two Sided Open Syllable Deletion (25)
Surface Representation
I n s t e a d of Epenthesis (24) we propose the following rule, which fills an e m p t y
node w i t h t h e neutral vowel (which is i in Yawelmani):
(27) Epenthesis:
N
V
0 +|/_
This rule differs from rule (9) only in t h a t i t refers to a n e m p t y syllabic node
(nucleus), instead of a n e m p t y V. L e t us now look a t t h e structures t h a t will
be generated b y onr syllahification principles in (16) for t h e structure in (26):
(28)
(29)
O
O
R
R
/ \
N
V
a
P
G
O
N
C
O
R
/ \
/ \
Cd
I !
C
CT
N
Cd
I
C
C
V
O
R
R
/ \
/ \
Cd
N
c
N
Cd
C
Cd
I
c
C
n
n
a
I
We see t h a t the e m p t y nodes are precisely in those places where we find an
epenthetic vowel on t h e surface. We now see t h a t we are able to posit a more
general rule for t h e epenthesis process t h a n in (24). Because our rule refers to
syllable structure, it is endowed with explanatory power: it explains t h e fact
t h a t epenthesis takes place only in those cases where it is necessary to "repair"
the syllabic structure. N o t e also t h a t we do n o t need the Two Sided Open Sylla
A parameter
of
syllabification
55
ble Deletion (25) here. I t would indeed be strange t h a t a vowel should be in
serted first, only to be deleted later, as in the derivations in (26).3
If one assumes a syllabification from left to right, one would derive the
structure in (30), which would give rise to the ungrammatical phonetic form.
in (31):
(30)
G
O
R
O
/ \
N Cd
C V c c
R
O
N
R
/ \
/ \
N
Cd
Cd
(31) *[pa?tihni]
V c
a
n
We thus see t h a t the righttoleft directionality can account for three processes
in Yawelmani. I t is useful to mention a fourth reason here, of a more theoreti
cal nature, which argues in favour of a retrograde syllabification. Consider
what would happen if syllabification were to take place from left to right. If
C
the mechanism encountered a postvocalic |, it would not be able to decide to
c
which syllabic node this element were to be linked. For this, it would have to
C V
C
know whether the following element were a |, or a |. I n the same case, the I
c v
c
would have to be assigned to the coda (of the former syllable), in the latter
case to the onset (of the next syllable). This amounts to saying t h a t we would
have to be able to look ahead, which would entail a partial bidirectionality.
This would make the mechanism unnecessarily powerfull: a righttoleft appli
cation does not produce this kind of problem, at least not for CVC languages
in which the onset is obligatorily filled. I n order to see this consider what would
happen if (going from right to left) the mechanism were to encounter a C after
having linked the node of nucleus to a V. Because of the fact t h a t the onset is
obligatorily filled, it would always assign onset status to this C. There is thus
3
A p a r t from cases Itke (26), t h e only motivation adduced for t h e rule of " T w o Sided
Open Syllable Deletion" (27) is constituted b y t h e baheviour of t h e mediopassive morphe
m e — i n — , However, as pointed o u t in Noske (1985 : 359), t h e deletion of i which can
occur in this morphene is t h e result of t h e analogical reanalysis of t h e verb stem foliowed
b y ~~in— as a monomorphemic verb stem (the socalled "fake b a s e " (Newman 1944:75)).
T h e i t h e n alternates w i t h zero, because it is reanalysed as a n epenthetic vowel.
E.. Noske
56
no need to "know" the nature of the element on the CVtier (i.e., a C or a V)
that is to the left of the C in question. By assuming righttoleftdirectionality,
the mechanism can be kept as simple as possible.4
We can now raise the question whether righttolefb directionality is universal,
or, to put the same question less strongly, whether this directionahty is uni
versal for CVC languages (i.e., a language in which the maximal syllable is
CVC). This could be tested by investigating which stie is is selected for the in
sertion of an epenthetic vowel in order to "repair" syllabic structure, like the
one in (24). This boils down to asking the question in (32):
f#
(32) Is the environment
G<n universal in syllable repair in CVC lang
uages?
We will see in section 2 that the answer to this question is negative. To see this,
we will examine the process of Epenthesis in Tigrinya.
2. The syllabic processes in Tigrinya
Tigrinya, a South Semitic language of northern Ethiopia, possesses an epen
thesis process which can operate wordinternally as well as wordfinally. Pam
(1973) mentions two different rules, which he later combines. The first rule is
givenin (33) (1973 : 116):
(33) 0 - > i / C C _ ( C ) #
The functioning of this rule can be seen in (34) (1973:114):
(34) a. /kalb+n/ [kA.bin] "dog"+sunixed conjunction
b. /kalb/
[kAlbi:] "dog"
In (34b) the i has been lengthened by a lengthening rule given in (35)
(1973 : 115):
(35) i - > i : / _ #
* We are indebted to Professor Werner Winter for indicating to us that an earlier
version of our text was unelear on this point. If a CVC language allows empty onsets,
but does not allow syllabifications of the type CVC$VC (where $ = syllable boundary),
a syllabification excluded by its high degree of markedness in terms of Kaye & Lowen
starnm's proposals (whatever the direction of syllabification), we can make our prediction
more general and posit that lefttoright syllabification is the marked setting for CVC
languages in general. As the reader will have noted, the suggestions made by Kaye and
Lowenstamm also imply a partial bidirectionality. However, we think t h a t a languago
tends to choose the direction of syllabification which implies the fewest complications,
other things being equal, and that therefore a CVC language would tend to select the
righttoleft syllabification.
A parameter of
syllabification
57"
The second epenthesis rule is given in (36) (1973:111):
(36) 0 > i / # C
C
An example of the functioning of this rule can be seen in (37):
(37) /sbar/ [sibAx] 'break'
P a m combines these two rules as in (38) (1973:117):
(38) 0 > i / [ s y l l ] [ — s y l l ] _ [ s y l l ]
P a m thus has to resort to the expression [syll], i.e. he has to treat word
b o u n d a r i e s o n a p a r w i t h c o n s o n a n t s . H e t h u s h a s to specify these elements ne
gatively with regard to their syllabicity. This is not very satisfying for iri faet
word botmdaries and consonants have very little in common. This is the reason
why phonologists have stopped referring to word boundaries as
[syll],
One can now ask the question whether in Tigrinya, as in Yawelmani,, the epen
thesis process can be analysed as the result of the process of syllabification.
Por this, it is necessary to know what the maximal syllable in Tigrinya is, and
whether epenethesis takes place only if the process of syllabification is confron
ted with an otherwise unsyllablfiable sequence.
The answers to both questions are straightforward: the syllable structure of
Tigrinya is CV (V) (C), the maximal syllable being thus CVVC, and Epenthesis
operates only in those structures where otherwise a more complex consonantal
syllabic structure t h a n O W C would ensue.
We can now see t h a t the rules in (33) and (36) can bedispensed with if we as
sume a left-to-right syllabification, in the same way as the righttoleft syllabi
fication in Yawelmani we proposed above: if a C is encountered by the sylla
bification mechanism a t a place where it only expects a V, a V is projected,
a n d later filled b y the neutral vowel (in the case of Tigrinya an iï).ïï we adopt
such an analysis, we can express the process in a unitary way, without having
to resort to references to [syll]. The only difference from the Yawelmani case
is t h a t the directionality of syllabification is revcrscd, and t h a t the quality of
the epenthetic vowel is süghtly different (/ instead of i). I t can thus be stated
t h a t the parameter of the directionality of syllabification is set differently for
Tigrinya and Yawelmani.
Not only does the epenthesis process provide motivation for the directionality
of the syllabification process, but this is also the case with a process of vowel
deletion, operating in an opposite fashion to Yawelmani. Thus is formulated
by P a m (1973:76) as in (39).
(39) Vowel Elision (Pam 1973:76):
V
V > 0
[+long]
58
K. Noske
The stractural description of this rule is the mirror image of t h a t of rule (18),
formulated by Kisseberth for vowel elision in Yawelmani, except in its requi
rement t h a t the vowel be long, which follows from the fact t h a t Tigrinya al~
lows two V s in a elosed syllable (the two V s represent a long vowel or a diph
thong). An example of the application of rule (39) is given in (40) (Pam
1973:77).
(40) Base
a|—prefixation
Infixation
Vowel Elision
Output
r i*s
a+
a:
0
a+ra:?s
'heads'
"This form surfaces as [ara : Hs], (The place where the epenthesis takes place
seems t o contradict rule (33), as well as our reanalysis of it. However, P a m
(1973:1178) points out t h a t the epentheiss site is exceptional and is re
stricted t o the class of words to which the form in (50) belongs, thus there seems
to be a morphological conditioning here).
We will now make a final point. As was mentioned in section 1, given a di
rectionality in the syllabification mechanism, one would expect it to be rightto
left rather than lefttoright because of the fact t h a t in the latter case the
mechanism would have to look ahead. The righttoleffc directionality would
thus constitute the unmarked case. This seems indeed to be confirmed b y the
facts. There are many CVC languages where
G<^ is the environment for
syllable repair. A situation such as the one in Tigrinya, however, where the
environment for syllable repair could be formulated as
^yC
a
seems to be
very rare, and must be assumed to be the marked case. This conjecture is
confirmed if we look at the situation in languages closely related to Tigrinya,
e,g., Tigre. I n Tigre, the equivalent of (34b) is as in (4):
(41) [k.Alib]
This form is also found in other related languages. Hence it must be conclud
ed t h a t in Tigre, syllabification takes place from right to left, the unmarked
direction, and t h a t Tigrinya is exceptional in its lefttoright syllabification.
«3. Gonclusion
We have shown t h a t the principle of directional syllabification proposed
by Ter Mors (1982) in order to explain the process of epenthesis in Klamath,
as well as b y K a y and Lowenstamm (1982) in order to account for the dif
ference in the distribution of syllable boundaries between English a n d Polish,
A parameter of
syllabification
59
receives additional motivation from the syllahic processes in Yawélmani
such as epenthesis and vowel elision. I t was shown t h a t in Tigrinya too,
syllabification must be assumed to take place directionally, but t h a t in this
language it applies from left to right, in contrast to K l a m a t h and Yawélmani.
The implicit conclusion in Kaye and Lowenstamm, i.e. t h a t the direction
of syllabification is a parameter, is thus shown to be correct. I t is plausible
t h a t the left-to-right syllabification is the marked setting of the directionahty
parameter lor 0 V 0 languages with a obligatorily filled onset.
REFERENCES
Archangeli, ü . 1984. Underspecificalion
in Yawélmani phonology and morphology. U n p u
blished P h . D . dissertation, t h e M.I.T.
Belletti, A. et al. (eds). 1981. Theory of markedness in generative grammar. P i s a : Scuola
Normale Superiore di Pisa.
K a l m , D . 1976. Syllable-based generalizations in Mnglish phonology, Unpublished P h . D .
dissertation, the M.I.T.
K a y e , J . a n d L o w e n s t a m m , J. 1981. "Syllable s t r u c t u r e a n d m a r k e d n e s s t h e o r y " , I n
Belletti, A. et al. (eds). 1981. 287315.
Kisseberth, G. W . 1969. Theoretical impUcations of Yawélmani phonology. Unpublished
P h . D . dissertation, University of Illinois, U r b a n a .
K u r o d a , S.Y. 1967. Yawélmani phonology. Cambridge, M a s s . : The M.I.T. Press.
N e w m a n , S. 1944. Yohuts language of California. N e w York: Viking F u n d Publications
in Anthropology.
Noske, R. 1985. "Syllabification a n d syllable changing processes in Y a w é l m a n i " . I n V a n
der H u l s t , H . a n d Smith, N . S . ' H . (eds). 1985. 33561.
Pain, M. D. 1973. Tigrinya phonology. U n p u b l i s h e d P h . D . dissertation, City University
of New York.
Ter Mors, 0. 1985. Empty-Vnodes
and their role in Klamath vowel altemations. I n V a n der
H u l s t H . a n d S m i t h N . S. H . (eds). 1985. 313 33.
V a n der Hulst, H . a n d Smith, N . S. H . (eds). 1985. Advances in nonlinear
phonology.
Dordrecht: F o r i s Publications.