[go: up one dir, main page]

0% found this document useful (0 votes)
2 views139 pages

Hoover ThompsonDissertation

Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1/ 139

Women Who Self-Objectify and Objectify Other Women:

The Role of Self-Compassion

by

Alysia Ann Hoover-Thompson

A dissertation submitted to the faculty of Radford University


in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of
Doctor of Psychology in Counseling Psychology
in the Department of Psychology

Dissertation Chair: Dr. Sarah L. Hastings

May 2013

Copyright 2013, Alysia Ann Hoover-Thompson

_______________________________ __________________________
Dr. Sarah L. Hastings Date
Dissertation Chair

_______________________________ __________________________
Dr. Tracy Cohn Date
Committee Member

_______________________________ __________________________
Dr. Thomas Pierce Date
Committee Member
ABSTRACT

Sexual objectification is harmful to women’s psychosocial health, as this form of

oppression leads to increased body surveillance, body shame and decreased self-esteem. The

construct of self-compassion (Neff, 2003a), and more specifically, the new concept of body or

physical self-compassion (Berry, Kowalski, Ferguson, & McHugh, 2010; Magnus, Kowalski, &

McHugh, 2010, respectively), has been found to mitigate this objectification. In addition,

Szymanski and Carr (2011) called for social justice initiatives surrounding empowerment of

female clients, and Szymanksi et al. (2011) suggested the need for further research about women

who objectify other women. Although the professional literature has addressed self-compassion

and these findings suggest the positive influence of increased body self-compassion, the

literature has not extended a body self-compassion induction on women who experience self-

objectification and women who objectify other women. Utilizing an experimental design to

address this gap in the literature, I proposed this research study to examine the effects of a self-

compassion induction on both self-objectification and objectification of other women. Four

hundred and ten undergraduate female research participants were randomly assigned to a self-

compassion induction or control group. After completing the induction or the control writing

prompts, all participants completed the Self-Objectification Scale (Noll & Fredrickson, 1998), a

modified version of this scale to address objectification of other women, the Rosenberg Self-

Esteem Scale (Rosenberg, 1965) and a demographics questionnaire developed by this researcher.

Analysis of variance (ANOVA) was then conducted to determine whether there were differences

in self-objectification and objectification of other women between the self-compassion induction

group and the control group. Results concluded that the effect of group membership was not

ii
statistically significant for scores of self-objectification or objectification of other women.

Multiple regression was then used to find the unique contribution of the self-compassion

induction beyond what was accounted for by self-esteem. Results concluded that the self-

compassion induction did not account for a significant proportion of variability in scores for self-

or other-objectification beyond that already accounted for by self-esteem.

Alysia Ann Hoover-Thompson, Psy.D.


Department of Psychology, 2013
Radford University

iii
TABLE OF CONTENTS

Page

Abstract………………………………………………………………………ii-iii

Table of Contents…………………………………………………………….iv-viii

List of Tables…………………………………………………………………ix

Chapter 1. SUMMARY OF THE ISSUES.....................................................1

Importance of This Study…………………………………………. 1

Objectification Theory……………………………………………. 4

Self-Objectification………………………………………. 5

Objectification of Others…………………………………. 7

Self-Compassion…………………………………………………. 8

Self-Kindness…………………………………………….. 9

Common Humanity………………………………………. 10

Mindfulness………………………………………………. 10

Self-Compassion versus Self-Esteem…………………..… 10

Body Image and Self-Esteem……………………. 12

Self-Compassion in the Literature………………………. 14

Self-Compassion and Body Image……………… 17

Research Questions………………..…………………………….. 19

Procedure………………………………………………………… 20

Self-Compassion Induction……………………………… 21

Control Group…………………………………………… 21

iv
Participants………………………………………………………. 22

Results…………………………………………………………… 23

Post-Hoc Analyses………………………………………. 24

Discussion……………………………………………………….. 25

Post-Hoc Analyses………………………………………. 27

Limitations and Future Research………………………………... 29

2. LITERATURE REVIEW............................................................. 31

Importance of this Study…………………………………………. 31

Objectification Theory…………………………………………… 34

Self-Objectification……………………………………… 36

Objectification of Others………………………………… 38

Definition of Body Image………………………………………… 39

Transmission of Body Image……………………………………. 40

Media……………………………………………............ 40

Familial Transmission…………………………………… 44

Self-Compassion…………………………………………………. 45

Self-Kindness…………………………..………………… 46

Common Humanity……….……………………………… 46

Mindfulness………..……………………………………. 46

Self-Compassion versus Self-Esteem……………………. 47

Body Image and Self-Esteem…………………….. 48

Self-Compassion in the Literature……………………….. 51

v
Self-Compassion and Body Image………………. 54

Conclusion and Research Questions……………………………… 55

3. METHODOLOGY....................................................................... 57

Participants……………………………………………………..… 57

Instruments………………………………………………………. 58

Self-Objectification Questionnaire………………………. 59

Objectification of Others Questionnaire…………………. 60

Rosenberg Self-Esteem Scale… ………………………… 60

Demographics …………………………………………… 61

Procedure………………………………………………………… 61

Self-Compassion Induction……….................................. 61

Control Group…………………………………………… 62

Data Analysis……………………………………………………. 62

Hypotheses………………………………………………………. 63

Conclusion………………………………………………………. 64

4. DATA ANALYSIS……………………………………………… 66

Description of the Sample……………………………………….. 66

Sample Demographics…………………………………………… 67

Survey Results…………………………………………………… 70

Rosenberg Self-Esteem Scale……………………………. 70

Self-Objectification Questionnaire………………………. 70

Other-Objectification Questionnaire…………………….. 70

vi
Statistical Tests of Research Questions…………………………. 73

Research Question One…………………………………. 73

Research Question Two…………………………………. 75

Unique Contribution of Self-Compassion………………. 77

Post-Hoc Analyses………………………………………………. 80

Research Question One…………………………………. 78

Research Question Two………………………………… 84

Self-Esteem…………………………………………….. 86

Unique Contribution of Self-Compassion……………… 86

Conclusion……………………………………………………… 89

5. DISCUSSION………………………………………………….. 90

Research Summary…………………………………………….. 90

Discussion of the Results………………………………………. 91

Research Question One………………………………… 91

Research Question Two………………………………… 92

Unique Contribution of Self-Compassion……………… 94

Post-Hoc Analyses……………………………………… 95

Limitations and Future Research……………………………….. 96

Conclusion……………………………………………………… 97

References……………………………………………………………… 100

Appendix A: The Self-Objectification Questionnaire…..……………… 120

Appendix B: Objectification of Other Women………………………… 122

vii
Appendix C: Rosenberg Self-Esteem Scale……………………………. 123

Appendix D: Demographics Questionnaire……………………………... 124

Appendix E: Informed Consent for Mid-Sized State University…..……. 125

Appendix F: Informed Consent for Large State University……………. 126

Appendix G: Writing Prompt for All Participants………………………. 127

Appendix H: Self-Compassion Induction Writing Prompts…………….. 128

Appendix I: Control Group Writing Prompts…………………………….. 129

viii
LIST OF TABLES

Table 4.1 – Demographic Information of Participants…………………………….. 69

Table 4.2 – Descriptive Statistics of Measures……………………………………. 72

Table 4.3 – Tests of Between-Subjects Effects for Self-Objectification Scores….. 74

Table 4.4 – Tests of Between-Subjects Effects for Other-Objectification Scores... 76

Table 4.5 – Summary of Regression Analysis for Variables Predicting Self-

Objectification………………………………………………………………………78

Table 4.6 – Summary of Regression Analysis for Variables Predicting Other-

Objectification………………………………………………………………………79

Table 4.7 – Demographic Information of Post-Hoc Participants………………….. 81

Table 4.8 – Post-Hoc Tests of Between-Subjects Effects for Self-Objectification

Scores………………………………………………………………………………. 83

Table 4.9 – Post-Hoc Tests of Between-Subjects Effects for Other-Objectification

Scores………………………………………………………………………………. 85

Table 4.10 – Post-Hoc Summary of Regression Analysis for Variables Predicting

Self-Objectification………………………………………………………………… 87

Table 4.11 – Post-Hoc Summary of Regression Analysis for Variables Predicting

Other-Objectification………………………………………………………………. 88

ix
CHAPTER I

SUMMARY OF THE ISSUES

This chapter provides an overview of this research study. It begins with an overview of

the need for this research and then details the theoretical framework supporting this research:

Objectification Theory (Fredrickson & Roberts, 1997). This chapter examines relevant research

focusing on the self-objectification of women and women objectifying other women. It also

examines the literature identifying the construct of self-compassion, how it is measured, and its

relationship to body image and Objectification Theory. This chapter then details the procedures

used for this research, followed by a description of the participant sample and the results of this

study. It concludes with a discussion of the results, as well as limitations and future research

implications.

Importance of this Study

Female standards of beauty have been dictated by society throughout history. The

nineteenth century brought corsets to shrink women’s waists, accentuate their hips, and achieve

an hourglass figure. By the middle of the twentienth century, the curvy hourglass figure

dissolved into curveless bodies, subjected to girdles, self-mutilation, stomach stapling, and self-

starvation. Over time, the standards by which we judge the female body has changed

(Waterhouse,1997).

The 1960s and 1970s in the United States brought about a new culture of thinness in

women. A shift occurred in which voluptuous, curved figures gave way to the angular, lean

body that many women idealize today. The pervasiveness of dieting among women best

exemplified this shift (Garner, Garfinkel, Schwartz, & Thompson, 1980).

1
More recently, Cash and Henry (1995) found that 63% of adult women in the United

States were dissatisfied with their current weight and 49% reported preoccupation with their

body weight. Cash and Henry postulated that while women have made significant strides in

gender equality in the last century, there is “little evidence that the recent economic,

occupational, and political gains of women in the United States have brought improvements in

their body images” (p.26).

If modern women in the United States are making such great strides in gender equality,

why has the nature of the feminine body ideal shifted? Society barrages women with negative

body image messages through a multitude of outlets. The messages are seen on television, in

magazines, heard in the halls of schools and internally repeated each time women walk past

mirrors (Waterhouse, 1997). They are ingrained in culture, to the extent that the messages are

hardly noticed. “Though many women, spurred by the revival of the feminist movement, are

presenting themselves more proudly and confidently in public, in the privacy of their own minds

too few seem to have favorable images of themselves” (Sanford & Donovan, 1985, p.4).

The feminist movement sought to release women from the objectification of their bodies.

“How anyone experiences, fantasizes about, or internally represents her or his embodiment

grows from experience, learning and self-definition in the family and in the culture” (Chodorow,

1989, p. 101). The existence of a gendered and sexually organized world does not allow society

to experience embodiment as simply being in a body. Gender and sex differences create a

dichotomy of embodiment that dictates how women experience femininity and determines who

women are, what women do, and how women are perceived (Chodorow, 1989).

2
Body dissatisfaction can be explained by Objectification Theory (Fredrickson & Roberts,

1997). The theory posits that women’s bodies are looked at, evaluated, and objectified. This

objectification means that women are simply treated as bodies that exist for the pleasure of

people around them, creating significant pressure for women to be preoccupied with their bodies.

Women internalize observers’ perspectives of their bodies leading to habitual self-conscious

body monitoring and self-objectification.

Utilizing this theory, Strelan and Hargreaves (2005) conducted a study on one hundred

thirty-two university students and their friends (64 women and 68 men) who completed three

questionnaires: the Self-Objectification Questionnaire (Noll & Fredrickson, 1998), 2) a modified

version of that questionnaire that measured individuals’ objectification of others, and 3) the Body

Cathexis scale (Slade, Dewey, Newton, & Brodie, 1990). They found that women were more

likely than men to self-objectify, self-objectification was negatively related to body satisfaction

for women, and both men and women objectified other women more than they objectified men.

Self-esteem has often been linked with body satisfaction (Ben-Tovim, Walker, Murray, &

Chin, 1990; Sanford & Donovan, 1985; Usmiani & Daniluk, 1997). However, there have

recently been criticisms about self-esteem as a measure of psychological health (Baumeister,

Smart, & Boden, 1996; Baumeister, Campbell, Krueger, & Vohs, 2003; Crocker, Luhtanen,

Cooper, & Bouvrette, 2003; Twenge, Konrath, Foster, Campbell, & Bushman, 2008). To

address these criticisms, Neff (2003a) proposed a new construct: self-compassion, which is an

alternative conceptualization of a healthy attitude and relationship to oneself. This healthier

attitude has been linked to less self-judgment (Neff, 2003a) and less judgment of others (Brown,

1999). Therefore, one could argue that higher levels of self-compassion could lead to less self-

3
objectification and less objectification of others. The next section addresses this objectification

through a review of the theory, as well as a literature review of self-objectification and

objectification of others.

Objectification Theory

The physical body has traditionally been the defining characteristic used to distinguish

men from women. Anatomical, genetic, and hormonal influences on personality, experience,

and behavior have been routinely explored. This biological basis has long been criticized by

feminists, as they have argued that gender differences have little to do with the physical body

and more to do with the socialization of boys and girls. Bodies exist within social and cultural

contexts and are therefore constructed within sociocultural practices (Fredrickson & Roberts,

1997).

Western culture is saturated with heterosexuality and the assumption that men may

sexualize women (Westkott, 1986). Within this sexual gazing, there exists the possibility for

sexual objectification, or women being treated as just bodies. The mass media has propagated

this sexual objectification through sexualized images of the female body, making these images

unavoidable in the American culture (Fredrickson & Roberts, 1997). Research has indicated that

an unattainable standard of women’s physical beauty is what is depicted in the media. This

narrow view is often linked with a woman’s sexiness and worth (APA, 2007).

Fredrickson and Roberts (1997) sought to uncover why objectification occurs, with the

understanding that women exist in a culture in which their bodies are looked at and evaluated.

Objectification Theory “places female bodies in a sociocultural context with the aim of

illuminating the lived experiences and mental health risks of girls and women who encounter

4
sexual objectification” (p. 174). The researchers argued that sexual objectification is only one

form of gender oppression, but that it may perhaps act as a catalyst for other oppressions women

face.

Objectification Theory posits that girls and women treat themselves as objects to be

looked at and evaluated. This adoption of an observer’s perspective may be due to compliance

with external pressures (Fredrickson & Roberts, 1997). Effective socialization begins with

compliance with external pressures, leading to interpersonal identification, and ending with

individuals claiming ownership of socialized values and attitudes. This ownership may result in

a new notion of self (Costanzo, 1992).

Self-Objectification

Women learn that their “looks” matter and that how they are viewed by observers can

determine how they are treated. Objectification Theory argues that women can adopt a “third-

person” view of themselves as a way to control how they are treated (Fredrickson & Roberts,

1997). Physical beauty can be powerful for women (Citrin, Roberts, & Fredrickson, 2004),

however, a majority of studies indicate negative psychological consequences of sexual

objectification (Moradi & Huang, 2008).

Utilizing Fredrickson and Roberts’ (1997) Objectification Theory, Frederick, Peplau, and

Lever (2006) studied correlates of body image in a sample of 52,677 heterosexual men and

women. The researchers examined associations of body satisfaction to age, height, gender, and

BMI and found that fewer men than women reported being too heavy (41% versus 61%) and

rated their body as being unattractive (11% versus 21%), consistent with Objectification Theory.

5
In addition, slender women (BMI = 14.5-22.49) reported more body satisfaction than most of the

other women, also consistent with Objectification Theory.

Mercurio and Landry (2008) investigated the impact of self-objectification on women’s

overall feelings of self-worth and their perceptions of their own well-being in terms of life

satisfaction. Two hundred twenty-seven female undergraduates completed the Objectified Body

Consciousness Scale (McKinley & Hyde, 1996), the Rosenberg Self-Esteem Scale (Rosenberg,

1965), and the Satisfaction with Life Scale (Diener, Emmons, Larsen, & Griffin, 1985). The

researchers found that self-objectification impacted overall life satisfaction through its relation

with body shame and self-esteem.

Greenleaf and McGreer (2006) examined Objectification Theory (Fredrickson & Roberts,

1997) among physically active (N= 115) and sedentary (N= 70) female college students. The

women completed the Self-Objectification Questionnaire (Noll & Fredrickson, 1998), the Body

Surveillance and Body Shame subscales of the Objectified Body Consciousness Scale

(McKinley & Hyde, 1996), the Appearance Anxiety Scale (Dion, Dion, & J. Keelan, 1990), a

flow experiences measure (Tiggemann & Slater, 2001), and the Eating Attitudes Test–26

(Garner, Olmsted, Bohr, & Garfinkel, 1982). They found that self-objectification predicted

disordered eating in both groups of women and those women high in self-objectification reported

higher levels of body surveillance, body shame, appearance anxiety, and self-reported disordered

eating attitudes.

Rolnik, Engeln-Maddox, and Miller (2010) investigated the impact of sorority rush on

self-objectification and body image disturbance. First-year undergraduate women either

participating (n=68)or not participating (n=59) in sorority rush at a Midwestern university in the

6
U.S. completed the Objectified Body Consciousness Scale (McKinley & Hyde, 1996), the Eating

Attitudes Test–26 (Garner, Olmsted, Bohr, & Garfinkel, 1982), a demographics questionnaire

developed by the researchers, and a rush-specific questionnaire developed by the researchers.

The researchers found that women who dropped out of rush had significantly higher body mass

indexes (BMI’s) prior to beginning the rush process, as compared to those who completed the

rush process, and rushees’ BMI’s significantly predicted whether they would drop out of rush.

Consistent with previous findings that sorority women are especially likely to promote a thin

body ideal (Schulken, Pinciaro, Sawyer, Jensen, & Hoban, 1997), this research illustrated that

the further women’s bodies were from the culturally sanctioned thin ideal, the more negative

their experience of sorority rush was. The researchers also found that rush participants

evidenced increased levels of self-objectification and eating disordered behavior.

Objectification of Others

Not only do women self-objectify (Greenleaf & McGreer, 2006;Mercurio& Landry,

2008;Peplau& Lever, 2006; Rolnik, Engeln-Maddox, & Miller, 2010) but they objectify other

women (Bearman, Korobov, & Thorne, 2009; Strelan & Hargreaves, 2005). This objectification

of other women may take the form of comments or suggestions, glances, and other behaviors that

communicate the thin-ideal to which women should conform (Szymanski, Moffitt, & Carr,

2011).

Tylka and Sabik (2010) studied Social Comparison Theory (Festinger, 1954) and self-

esteem as they were integrated into the framework of Objectification Theory (Fredrickson &

Roberts, 1997). Two hundred and seventy-four women from a Midwestern U.S. college

completed the Feedback on Physical Appearance Scale (Tantleff-Dunn, Thompson, & Dunn,

7
1995), the Rosenberg Self Esteem Scale (Rosenberg, 1965), the Body Surveillance subscale and

the Body Shame subscale of the Objectified Body Consciousness Scale (McKinley & Hyde,

1996), the Body Comparison Scale (Fisher & Thompson, 1998), and the Eating Attitudes Test-26

(Garner et al., 1982). The researchers found that women who are focused on their own bodies

are more attentive to the appearance of other women’s bodies and how they compare to their

own. This body comparison may act as a feedback loop to their own body, perpetuating

heightened body surveillance and objectification of other women’s bodies. The researchers also

found that self-esteem uniquely predicted body surveillance, body comparison, and body shame.

This finding illustrated the importance of including self-esteem as a variable within the

objectification framework.

Szymanski et al. (2011) argued that to “properly address women’s own contribution to

sexually objectifying environments, a great deal more research is needed to explore the ways

women objectify other women” (p. 29). Szymanski and Carr (2011) emphasized the need for

psychologists to empower therapy clients to address social problems such as sexual

objectification. They see this empowerment as a social justice initiative “given the harmful role

of external and internalized sexual objectification and other forms of oppression on individuals’

psychosocial health” (p.165). The following section will define the construct of self-

compassion, compare it with self-esteem, and explore the construct in the current literature.

Special attention will be paid to the constructs as they relate to body image.

Self-Compassion

Neff’s (2003a) construct of self-compassion represents a conceptualization of a healthy

attitude and relationship to oneself. Self-compassion allows individuals to see themselves with

8
the nonjudgmental understanding and sense of humanness that is often afforded to others. Neff

(2003a) defined self-compassion as:

being touched by and open to one’s own suffering, not avoiding or disconnecting from it,

generating the desire to alleviate one’s suffering and to heal oneself with kindness. Self-

compassion also involves offering nonjudgmental understanding to one’s pain,

inadequacies and failures, so that one’s experience is seen as part of the larger human

experience (p. 87).

As part of this definition, Neff (2003a) described three components of self-compassion that work

together for mutual enhancement:

(a) self-kindness – extending kindness and understanding to oneself rather than harsh

judgment and self-criticism, (b) common humanity – seeing one’s experiences as part of

the larger human experience rather than seeing them as separating and isolating, and (c)

mindfulness – holding one’s painful thoughts and feelings in balanced awareness rather

than over-identifying with them (p.89).

Self-Kindness

The kindness component of self-compassion recognizes that humans experience

challenges in life and cannot be perfect. When one encounters suffering, feelings of inadequacy,

or failure, self-compassion encourages warmth and understanding toward oneself. When life’s

setbacks are denied, suffering occurs in the form of self-criticism. When they are accepted with

kindness, greater emotional balance may occur (Neff, 2003a; Neff, 2008).

9
Common Humanity

The recognition that all humans suffer allows one to see that suffering and failure are part

of the shared human experience and are something larger than the self. Existing as a human

means experiencing failure. This failure is one of the conditions which binds people to one

another. It is the denial of this common link that leads to a competitive mind-set and perpetuates

self-judgment and comparison to others (Neff, 2003a; Neff, 2008).

Mindfulness

Mindfulness allows individuals to observe their thoughts and emotions as they exist,

without attempting to suppress, change or deny them. In this way, mindfulness allows the

individual to be nonjudgmental toward the self (Neff, 2003a; Neff, 2008). Mindfulness does not

negate negative emotions or feelings, rather it allows for emotional balance that prevents the

individual from over identifying with an emotional state (Bishop et al., 2004).

Self-compassion tends to soften ego-protective barriers, allowing us to be kind to

ourselves, rather than just being kind to those around us. This self-kindness does not require an

inflated self-image and is available when self-esteem is not. It is available “when we fall flat on

our face, embarrass ourselves, or otherwise come in direct contact with the imperfection of life”

(Neff, 2011, p.6). The following section examines this difference between self-compassion and

self-esteem.

Self-Compassion versus Self-Esteem

Rogers (1951) defined self-esteem as the extent to which an individual likes, values, and

accepts himself or herself. Self-esteem is central in everything one does. Having positive self-

esteem enhances one’s confidence, self-image, and overall happiness. Having negative self-

10
esteem creates negative thinking and leads one to believe negative comments made by others

(Perera, n.d.).

Self-esteem has routinely served as the marker of psychological well-being (Erikson,

1968; Rosenberg, 1979) to the extent that the self is seen as separate from shared human

experience. Within recent years, researchers have voiced concerns about self-esteem as a

measure of psychological health (Baumeister et al., 1996; Baumeister et al., 2003; Crocker et al.,

2003; Harter, 1999; Twenge et al., 2008). It was found that self-esteem is largely the outcome of

doing well, rather than the cause of doing well (Baumeister et al., 2003). It was also speculated

that the emphasis placed on self-esteem in our society has led to an increase in narcissism scores

of college students. Researchers found that 65% of recent college students scored higher in

narcissism than previous generations of students (Twenge et al., 2008). Finally, self-esteem may

be linked to inflated self-views, which could be problematic when it is contingent on a particular

outcome (Crocker et al., 2003).

Harter (1999) found that global self-esteem is shaped by evaluations of self-worth in

domains such as appearance, social approval, and academic/work performance. This means that

other potentially important life successes may be neglected in order to maintain high self-esteem.

It also means that one’s self-esteem is contingent upon recent successes and failures. Kernis

(2005) found that this contingent self-esteem makes people more vulnerable to depression and

reduced self-concept clarity. Further exemplifying this finding, the following section addresses

the link between body image and self-esteem and how this may be harmful to the individual.

11
Body image and self-esteem. Usmiani and Daniluk (1997) argued that “body image is formed

to a degree as a function of the culturally defined images of desirable bodily appearances for men

and women” (p. 47), and that from this perspective “a girls’ self-esteem may be influenced by

the degree to which she believes she meets cultural standards” (p. 47). A woman’s sense of who

she is may be defined through conformity within a culture. Women in the United States may

form their identity using a Westernized notion of feminine stereotypes that depict how a woman

is expected to look and act (Grogan, 2008).

In discussing their self-esteem enhancement groups, Sanford and Donovan (1985)

reported that nearly every female participant had a negative body image. The group members

summarized that it is “difficult to dislike your body or a specific part of your body and still like

yourself,” (p. 369). It is hard to discuss self-esteem without also including body image. For

some women, a poor body image leads to low self-esteem and for others, a low self-esteem lends

itself to poor body image.

Sanford and Donovan (1985) argued that parents’ self-acceptance influences a child’s

self-perception. Parents acting as role models can often be just as negative of an influence as

they are a positive influence. For example:

My mother was constantly concerned about her weight when I was a kid, and when I

reached puberty, she decided I looked “porky.” She dragged me to the gym with her,

which I hated, and we both went on the same diets, although I ate tremendous volumes at

school. I spent my allowance on hot lunches, in addition to eating the rabbit food she

sent along with me. She was always on me about how I looked, until I gave up and

withdrew. I stopped eating around her. This concerned her right away and she took me

12
to the doctor who told her I was the correct weight and that all my bulges were just

normal. After that, she left me alone (p. 369).

The researchers speculated that if the mother had a more positive self-concept, she may have

recognized that her daughter did not have a weight problem. If, in reality, the daughter did have

a weight problem, the mother could have demonstrated some reasonable and safe ways to

manage her weight.

Sanford and Donovan (1985) also argued that while some women do not see themselves

in an entirely negative light, they may still lack self-esteem. They may be well aware of their

good qualities, but have a hard time understanding that their good qualities matter as much as

their negative qualities or flaws. “Oh, I know I’ve got a nice face, that I’m pretty bright, a good

teacher, a kind person and all that,’ a woman might say. ‘But,’ she is quick to add, ‘I should lose

ten pounds,’”(p. 16).

Recent research suggests that self-compassion is more predictive of healthy self-related

functioning than is self-esteem (Neff & Vonk, 2006). Measures of self-compassion and global

self-esteem, as well as a number of self-related processes known to be maladaptive: self-esteem

instability and contingency, social comparison, narcissism, reactive anger, public self-

consciousness, and self-rumination were completed by the research participants. The researchers

found, after accounting for variance in outcomes attributable to self-esteem, self-compassion

predicted significant additional variance for self-esteem instability, self-esteem contingency,

social comparison, reactive anger, public self-consciousness, and self-rumination. Moreover, in

almost every case, self-compassion was a much stronger negative predictor of these outcomes

than was self-esteem.

13
Neff, Kirkpatrick, and Rude (2007) examined the relationship of self-compassion to

psychological health. Ninety-one undergraduates completed measures on self-compassion, self-

esteem, negative affectivity, and anxiety. They found that self-compassion, and not self-esteem,

buffered anxiety when participants were faced with an ego-threat in a laboratory setting.

Neff (2003a; 2009; 2011) argued that there is a way for people to feel good about

themselves without the positive or negative self-judgment or evaluation commonly associated

with self-esteem. Her research on the construct of self-compassion found that it offers similar

benefits to self-esteem, but without the pitfalls. She found that self-compassion is equally

relevant when suffering occurs either through external circumstances or one’s personal

inadequacies. Also, Neff (2004) suggested that because self-compassion does not require people

to adopt an unrealistic view of the self, it should be easier to raise than self-esteem. The

following section addresses self-compassion in the current literature.

Self-Compassion in the Literature

Research has helped establish the advantageous nature of self-compassion. Thus far,

most of the research on self-compassion has been conducted using the Self-Compassion Scale

(Neff, 2003a), which measures the degree to which individuals display self-kindness versus self-

judgment, common humanity versus isolation, and mindfulness versus over identification.

Utilizing this scale, higher levels of self-compassion have been associated with greater life-

satisfaction, emotional intelligence, social connectedness, learning goals, wisdom, personal

initiative, curiosity, happiness, optimism, and positive affect, as well as less self-criticism,

depression, anxiety, fear of failure, thought suppression, perfectionism, performance goals, and

14
disordered eating behaviors (see Neff, 2009, for a review). Self-compassion recognizes that we

all have strengths and weaknesses and that we are “imperfect human beings” (Neff, 2011, p. 9).

Neff, Hsieh, and Dejitthirat, (2005) examined the link between self-compassion and

motivation among college students to determine whether self-compassion might be adaptive in

academic contexts. Using a sample size of 222 college students, the researchers found that self-

compassion was positively associated with mastery goals, which include the joy of learning for

its own sake, and negatively associated with performance goals, which involve defending or

enhancing one’s sense of self-worth through academic performances. These findings were

replicated (N= 110) with students who had recently failed a midterm examination and indicated

that self-compassionate students exhibited more adaptive ways of coping with failure.

In a similar study, Akin (2008) examined the relationship between self-compassion and

achievement goal orientation. Research participants included 646 university students in Turkey

who completed scales on self-compassion and achievement goal orientation. Results concluded

that self-compassion was positively related to learning approach and negatively related to

performance approach. Given the findings, the author called for educational settings to foster

self-compassion in students.

In a study designed to explore the relationships among self-compassion, positive

psychological functioning, and the Big Five personality traits in college students (N=177), Neff,

Rude, and Kirkpatrick (2007) found that self-compassion was associated with greater reflective

and affective wisdom, curiosity and exploration, happiness, optimism, positive affect,

Extraversion, Agreeableness and Conscientiousness. It was found that self-compassion was

15
negatively associated with Neuroticism. They also found that self-compassion was linked to

personal initiative, which supports the notion that self-compassion leads to self-growth.

Neff and McGehee (2010) examined self-compassion among adolescents (N=235) and

young adults (N=287). Utilizing measures of self-compassion, depression, anxiety,

connectedness, maternal support, family functioning, attachment, and personal fable, the

researchers found that self-compassion was strongly associated with well-being for both age

groups. They also found that self-compassion contributed to well-being while controlling other

factors, suggesting that self-compassion’s mental health benefits were not confounded by family

influence or life stage. The authors argued that this finding may further support the notion that

self-compassion can be enhanced with practice (Gilbert and Proctor, 2006).

Leary, Tate, Adams, Allen, and Hancock (2007) utilized five separate studies that

investigated the cognitive and emotional processes by which self-compassionate people deal

with unpleasant life events. In the first study, 117 undergraduate students reported on negative

life events. Results showed that self-compassion predicted emotional and cognitive reactions to

negative events. The second study asked 123 undergraduate students to respond to hypothetical

scenarios which elicited feelings of loss, failure, and humiliation. The researchers found that

self-compassion buffered people against negative emotions when imagining distressing social

events. In the third study, 66 undergraduate students disclosed personal information to another

individual and then received either positive or neutral feedback. Their emotional reactions and

judgments of the other person were then assessed. The results concluded that self-compassion

moderated negative emotions and receiving ambivalent feedback and this was especially true for

participants who scored low on self-esteem. The fourth study videotaped 102 undergraduate

16
students while they performed an awkward and mildly embarrassing task. The videotapes were

then either self-rated or rated by another participant. The results showed that low-self-

compassionate participants undervalued their own videotaped performances relative to observers.

The final study was designed to examine how self-compassion moderates reactions to

remembered life events and to examine if self-compassion could be experimentally induced

among the 115 undergraduate research participants. Results showed that self-compassion allows

individuals to acknowledge their role in negative events without feeling overwhelmed with

negative emotions and that self-compassion could be experimentally induced.

Continuing the use of a self-compassion manipulation, Zabelina and Robinson (2010)

studied 86 undergraduate students to better understand the use of a self-compassion manipulation

as it relates to original creative thinking. The researchers found that self-judgmental individuals

displayed lower levels of creative originality in the control condition, but equal levels of creative

originality in the self-compassion manipulation condition. They also further exemplified the

positive outcome of a self-compassion induction.

Self-compassion and body image. A pilot study conducted by Gilbert and Irons in 2004

found that self-critical thoughts were linked to many different social situations. Prior to a

compassionate mind training technique, participants were asked to keep diaries to explore self-

critical themes. When the participants were asked to describe the situations or events that

elicited self-criticism, they identified being at the gym and body image-related situations, among

others. Thus, while self-compassion has been conceptualized as a construct related to the overall

self, it could also be more relevant to specific self-perceptions of the physical self.

17
Investigating the outcome of inducing a state of self-compassion on restrained eating,

Leary and Adams (2007) asked 84 female undergraduate students to complete the Revised Rigid

Restraint Scale, designed by the researchers. Participants were then randomly assigned to one of

the experimental conditions: preload/self-compassion condition, preload/no self-compassion

condition, or no preload/control condition. The preload consisted of a set amount of food

presented to participants who performed a bogus taste test to measure eating behavior. They

then completed a self-evaluation. The researchers found that highly restrictive eaters felt worse

after eating the preload compared to the control condition, but those who completed the self-

compassion induction showed lower negative affect and reduced self-criticism.

Magnus et al., (2010) found that self-compassion was positively related to intrinsic

motivation and negatively related to external and introjected motivation, ego goal orientation,

social physique anxiety and exercising in ways that are harmful to the self. They found that self-

compassion may foster a healthy attitude towards the self and identified the need for physical

self-compassion as a potential construct for future exploration.

Furthering this idea, Berry, Kowalski, Ferguson, and McHugh (2010) proposed that body

self-compassion could be a sub-domain of global self-compassion. Using Neff’s (2003)

research, they defined body self-compassion as “a kind, understanding and non-judgmental

attitude individuals extend toward their body in response to their perceived physical

imperfections, limitations and failures” (p. 295). Using this new construct, the researchers

interviewed five young adult women using an empirical phenomenological method. Three

essential themes emerged as the women experienced more body self-compassion: appreciating

18
one’s unique body, taking ownership of one’s body, and engaging in less social comparison.

These findings suggest the benefits of increasing body self-compassion.

Research Questions

Sexual objectification is harmful to women’s psychosocial health. It leads to increased

body surveillance, body shame, and decreased self-esteem. For centuries, women’s worth has

been linked with physical attractiveness. Sexual objectification perpetuates social injustice,

depriving women of opportunities to feel comfortable in their bodies and to experience life

without the added strain of needing to monitor or enhance physical appearance. Szymanski and

Carr (2011) called for social justice initiatives surrounding empowerment of female clients and

Szymanksi et al. (2011) suggested the need for further research about women who objectify

other women.

Recent critics of self-esteem (Baumeister et al., 1996; Baumeister et al., 2003; Crocker et

al., 2003; Harter, 1999; Twenge et al., 2008) have opened the doors to a new concept of a

healthy attitude and relationship to oneself in the construct of self-compassion (Neff, 2003a).

Given the recent research on self-compassion (Neff, 2003a; 2009, 2011) and the new concept of

body or physical self-compassion (Berry et al., 2010; Magnus et al., 2010, respectively), one can

hypothesize that women with higher self-compassion will be less likely to self-objectify and

objectify other women. We also know from the research that self-compassion can be

experimentally induced (Leary & Adams, 2007; Leary at al., 2007; Zabelina & Robinson, 2010).

Research has establish the advantages of self-compassion to include greater life-

satisfaction, emotional intelligence, social connectedness, learning goals, wisdom, personal

initiative, curiosity, happiness, optimism, and positive affect, as well as less self-criticism,

19
depression, anxiety, fear of failure, thought suppression, perfectionism, performance goals, and

disordered eating behaviors (Neff, 2009, for a review). Using body self-compassion, Berry et al.

(2010) used an empirical phenomenological approach to interview five women and found themes

that suggested that as women experienced more body self-compassion, they appreciated their

unique body, took ownership of their body and engaged in less social comparison. While these

findings suggest the positive influence of increased body self-compassion, the literature has not

extended a body self-compassion induction on women who experience self-objectification and

women who objectify other women.

Based on this review of the literature, it is evident that there is a gap in the literature with

respect to this extension of a self-compassion induction based on objectification. Therefore my

research questions were:

1. How does a self-compassion induction affect self-objectification?

2. How does a self-compassion induction affect objectification of other women?

Procedure

The purpose of this research study was to examine the influence of a body self-

compassion induction on college-aged women. Similar to Leary et al.’s (2007) Study 5,

participants wrote about a negative event from their past and answered questions about it.

Participants began by thinking about a negative event they experienced in high school or college

that made them feel badly about their bodies – something that involved failure, humiliation or

rejection. Participants were then asked to describe the event by including details regarding what

led to the event, who was present, what happened during the event, and how they felt and

20
behaved at the time. After writing about the event, participants were then randomly assigned to

one of two conditions: a) self-compassion induction or 2) control group.

Self-Compassion Induction

Participants in the self-compassion condition responded to three prompts which

instructed them to consider their negative event in a self-compassionate manner (Neff, 2003a).

The first prompt, designed to focus on the common humanity element of self-compassion, asked

participants to list ways in which other women have experienced a similar event. The second

prompt, designed to focus on self-kindness, asked participants to write how they would express

understanding, kindness and concern to themselves in the same way they might express concern

to a friend who had lived the same experience. The final prompt asked participants to describe

their feelings about the event in an objective and unemotional way, which is designed to induce a

mindful perspective on the event.

Control Group

Participants in the control condition also responded to three prompts to make the control

condition as similar to the self-compassion condition as possible. The first prompt asked

participants to describe the factors they considered in their college choice. The second prompt

asked participants to describe their favorite college class. The final prompt asked participants to

write about their least favorite college class. The prompts were designed to be neutral, so as to

not further induce negative or positive feelings regarding body image.

Following the writing prompts, all participants completed the Self-Objectification

Questionnaire (Noll & Fredrickson, 1998), a modified version of the Self-Objectification

21
Questionnaire used to determine objectification of other women, the Rosenberg Self-Esteem

Scale (Rosenberg, 1965) and a demographics questionnaire.

Participants

Data was collected at the end of the Fall semester and beginning of the Spring semester.

Participants in this study consisted of undergraduate women from one mid-sized state university

and one large state university in the Southeastern United States. Participants from the mid-sized

state university received research credit in their course and participants from the large state

university were eligible to enter a drawing for a gift card. There were initially 584 participants in

this research study, but participants who had missing responses or incomplete answers were

removed during data cleaning. Following data cleaning, 382 women from the mid-sized

university and 28 women from the large university remained in the research sample. The end

result was a sample of 410 undergraduate women who completed this research study. Of those,

220 were randomly assigned to the control group and 190 were randomly assigned to the self-

compassion induction group.

The ages of the research participants ranged from 18 to 50 years (M=19.30; SD= 2.22).

Two-hundred and twenty-five participants (54.9%) identified themselves as freshman, 81

(19.8%) identified themselves as sophomores, 50 (12.2%) identified themselves as juniors, and

54 (13.2%) identified themselves as seniors. Three hundred and thirty-six students (82%) self-

identified as Caucasian, three (0.7%) self-identified as Native American/American Indian, 42

(10.2%) self-identified as African American, three (0.7%) self-identified as Pacific Islander, five

(1.2%) self-identified as Asian, 11 (2.7%) self-identified as multi-ethnic, and 10 (2.4%) self-

identified as “other.” Ten participants (2.4%) responded that they had been diagnosed with an

22
eating disorder, while 400 (97.6%) responded that they had never been diagnosed with an eating

disorder. Each participant’s reported height and weight were used to calculate a BMI. BMI

values ranged from 15.66 to 40.35(M = 23.40, SD = 4.11).

Results

Self-esteem scores ranged from 4 to 30, with a mean score of 20.05 and a standard

deviation of 4.94. Self-Objectification Questionnaire scores ranged from -25 to 25, with a mean

of 3.16 and a standard deviation of 12.79. Other-Objectification Questionnaire scores ranged

from -25 to 25, with a mean of 9.31 and a standard deviation of 13.41. It is interesting to note

that the mean score for objectification of other women was significantly higher than the mean

score for self-objectification, (F(1, 409) = 69.04, p< .001).

Research question one sought to identify how a self-compassion induction affects self-

objectification of women. The effect of the group membership (induction vs. control) was not

statistically significant (p=.572).

Research question two sought to identify how a self-compassion induction affects how

women objectify other women. The effect of the group membership (induction vs. control) was

not statistically significant (p=.478).

The results of Tylka and Sabik’s (2010) study indicated the need to find the unique

contribution of the self-compassion induction beyond what was accounted for by self-esteem.

Self-compassion induction did not account for a significant proportion of variability in scores for

self-objectification beyond that already accounted for by self-esteem (R2 Change < .001, F(1,

407) = .03, p=.86). The self-compassion induction did not account for a significant proportion of

23
variability in scores for other-objectification beyond that already accounted for by self-esteem

(R2 Change = .001, F(1, 407) = .29, p=.59).

Post Hoc Analyses

Previous research has focused on the differences between college students who

participate in research early in the semester versus late in the semester. Early semester

participants have been found to be more socially responsible (Holden &Reddon, 1987), more

intrinsically motivated (Hom, 1987), more compliant (Masling, O'Neill, & Jayne, 1981), more

academically and achievement oriented with higher ACT and GPA scores, and to possess a more

internal academic locus of control (Evans & Donnerstein, 1974). Given these differences in

motivation and personality attributes, a post-hoc analysis combining data from early semester

participants from the mid-sized university and the sample from the large university, because they

were not participating for credit and were therefore more intrinsically motivated to complete the

study, was conducted. The control group had 89 participants and the self-compassion induction

had 83 participants. The total number of participants in this post-hoc analysis was 172.

Research question one sought to identify how a self-compassion induction affects self-

objectification of women. The effect of the group membership (induction vs. control) was not

significant (p=.145).

Research question two sought to identify how a self-compassion induction affects how

women objectify other women. The effect of the group membership (induction vs. control) was

not significant (p= .461).

A post-hoc analysis found significant differences among self-esteem scores between the

self-compassion induction and control groups (p = .031, R2Change = .027). Self-esteem scores

24
for the self-compassion induction group were significantly higher than the self-esteem scores for

the control group.

The results of Tylka and Sabik’s (2010) study indicated the need to find the unique

contribution of the self-compassion induction beyond what was accounted for by self-esteem.

Self-compassion induction did not account for a significant proportion of variability in scores for

self-objectification beyond that already accounted for by self-esteem (R2 Change = .005, F(1,

169) = .94, p =.33). Self-compassion induction did not account for a significant proportion of

variability in scores for other-objectification beyond that already accounted for by self-esteem

(R2 Change = .002, F(1, 169) = .34, p = .56).

Discussion

The recent research on self-compassion (Neff, 2003a; 2009, 2011) and the new concept

of body or physical self-compassion (Berry et al., 2010; Magnus et al., 2010, respectively) has

demonstrated that women with higher levels of self-compassion are kinder to themselves. It was

hypothesized that this kindness toward self can be generalized to kindness toward one’s body

and therefore, women with higher levels of self-compassion would be less likely to self-

objectify. It may be the case that this new concept of body or physical self-compassion (Berry

et al., 2010; Magnus et al., 2010, respectively) cannot be raised using a traditional self-

compassion induction. It may also be the case that the online data collection method, used by

the researcher, did not result in a true self-compassion induction for all of the participants, as

some of them may not have adequately thought about their responses in a self-compassionate

manner.

25
It was hypothesized that women who complete a self-compassion induction would report

lower scores in the modified version of the Self-Objectification Questionnaire, as compared to

the control group. This present study found that the effect of group membership (self-

compassion induction vs. control) was not significant with respect to scores on the modified

version of the Self-Objectification Questionnaire. However, it was found that the mean score

for objectification of other women was higher than the mean score for self-objectification for the

total sample. This meant that women were objectifying other women more than they were self-

objectifying.

A 2010 study by Tylka and Sabik found that women who are focused on their own bodies

are more attentive to the appearance of other women’s bodies. This body comparison may act as

a feedback loop to their own body, perpetuating heightened body surveillance and objectification

of other women’s bodies. Given the research on the positive outcomes of increasing one’s self-

compassion (Neff, 2003a; 2009, 2011), especially related to body self-kindness (Berry et al.,

2010; Magnus et al., 2010), it was hypothesized that a self-compassion induction would not only

lead to less self-objectification but also less objectification of other women. Similar to the

results from the first research question, it may be that this new concept of body or physical self-

compassion (Berry et al., 2010; Magnus et al., 2010, respectively) cannot be raised using a

traditional self-compassion induction or that the online data collection method did not actually

raise self-compassion. If the self-compassion induction did not significantly impact self-

objectification, it would naturally be the case that it would not significantly impact the

objectification of others.

26
The finding that women were objectifying other women more than they were self-

objectifying was consistent with the findings of Stelan and Hargreaves (2005). This may

indicate that women place more importance on the appearance of other women than they do on

their own appearance. Women are constantly bombarded with body image messages

emphasizing the thin-ideal (Waterhouse, 1997) and deflecting these messages onto others may

serve as a protective factor for women.

The results of Tylka and Sabik’s (2010) study indicated the need to find the unique

contribution of the self-compassion induction beyond what was accounted for by self-esteem. In

this research sample, no significant effect was found when controlling for self-esteem with

respect to self-objectification or objectification of other women. Similar to the findings above

for the two research questions, we may hypothesize that this new concept of body or physical

self-compassion (Berry et al., 2010; Magnus et al., 2010, respectively) cannot be raised using a

traditional self-compassion induction or that the online data collection method did not result in a

true self-compassion induction for the participants. In either case, self-compassion could not

account for anything significant beyond what was already accounted for by self-esteem.

Post-Hoc Analyses

This research study was conducted at the end of the Fall 2011 semester and the beginning

of the Spring 2012 semester. It was hypothesized that there may be differences in motivation

and personality factors between the students who completed the study at the end of the semester

versus the beginning of the semester. The literature points to differences which include: early

semester participants have been found to be more socially responsible (Holden & Reddon, 1987),

more intrinsically motivated (Hom, 1987), more compliant (Masling, O'Neill, & Jayne, 1981),

27
more academically and achievement oriented with higher ACT and GPA scores, and a more

internal academic locus of control (Evans & Donnerstein, 1974).

Because the students completing the study from the large university did not have the

same class or extra credit, large university participants remained in the post-hoc sample. The

control group had 89 participants and the self-compassion induction had 83 participants. The

total number of participants in this post-hoc analysis was 172.

The same analyses for research questions one and two were conducted using this new

sample. Again, no significant differences were found between the self-compassion and control

groups, similar to the results using the full research sample. It is believed that the same reasons,

as previously described, can account for these findings.

Interestingly, a post-hoc analysis found significant differences among self-esteem scores

between the self-compassion induction and control groups. This meant that self-esteem scores

for the self-compassion induction group were significantly different from and higher than the

self-esteem scores of the control group. Results were inconsistent with Neff’s (2004) suggestion

that because self-compassion does not require people to adopt an unrealistic view of the self, it

should be easier to raise than self-esteem. Given the emphasis that society has placed on raising

self-esteem (Twenge et al., 2008), it may have been the case that the college student sample was

primed to increase self-esteem more than they were self-compassion. It may also have been the

case that this research sample had higher levels of self-esteem prior to participation in this

research study, consistent with the personality and motivation factors previously discussed

(Evans & Donnerstein, 1974; Holden & Reddon, 1987; Hom, 1987; Masling, O'Neill, & Jayne,

1981). The initial writing prompt, which asked participants to write about a negative body

28
image experience, may have temporarily lowered self-esteem. Those women who participated

in the self-compassion induction may then have counterbalanced that lowering of self-esteem,

returning them to their normal level of self-esteem. Those women who participated in the

control group may not have returned to their normal levels of self-esteem, showing a significant

difference in levels of self-esteem between the two groups.

Limitations and Future Research

There were several major limitations to this research. First, while women from two

universities were represented in this study, the universities are still in the same geographic region

and the sample was comprised of only undergraduate women. Therefore, this study design did

not allow for an accurate representation of all women within the sampled age groups. Future

research should include research participants in a variety of urban and rural populations.

Another major limitation was that this study did not include a manipulation check

meaning that self-compassion was not measured following the self-compassion induction to see

if the induction actually increased self-compassion. The researcher chose to not include a self-

compassion questionnaire because of concern for the time requirements of participation in this

research. Future research should include either a pre- and post-induction evaluation of self-

compassion or at least measure the difference among self-compassion scores between the self-

compassion induction and the control group, regardless of the amount of time required to

complete the extra questionnaire.

Finally, it was hypothesized that the methodology used for data collection was not

adequate to induce self-compassion among all group participants. Due to the nature of online

data collection, some of the research participants in the self-compassion induction group may not

29
have spent enough time thinking about and then writing about the three components of self-

compassion (Neff, 2003a). This may have prevented a true self-compassion induction from

taking place. Future research should restrict this methodology to in-person data collection to

ensure that participants are spending adequate amounts of time on the self-compassion writing

prompts.

30
CHAPTER II

LITERATURE REVIEW

This chapter examines the current literature with respect to this research study. It begins

with an overview of the need for this research and then details the theoretical framework

supporting this research: Objectification Theory (Fredrickson & Roberts, 1997). Second, this

chapter examines relevant research focusing on the self-objectification of women and women

objectifying other women. Third, there is an examination of the literature identifying the

construct of self-compassion, how it is measured, and its relationship to body image and

Objectification Theory. Finally, the chapter concludes with the research questions posed by this

study.

Importance of this Study

Female standards of beauty have been dictated by society throughout history. The

nineteenth century brought corsets to shrink women’s waists, accentuate their hips, and achieve

an hourglass figure. By the middle of the twentienth century, the curvy hourglass figure

dissolved into curveless bodies, subjected to girdles, self-mutilation, stomach stapling, and self-

starvation. What happened to change the standard of the ideal female body (Waterhouse,

1997)?

The 1960s and 1970s in the United States brought about a new culture of thinness in

women. A shift occurred in which voluptuous, curved figures gave way to the angular, lean

body that many women idealize today. The pervasiveness of dieting among women best

exemplified this shift (Garner, Garfinkel, Schwartz, & Thompson, 1980).

31
More recently, Cash and Henry (1995) found that 63% of adult women in the United

States were dissatisfied with their current weight and 49% reported preoccupation with their

body weight. Cash and Henry postulated that while women have made significant strides in

gender equality in the last century, there is “little evidence that the recent economic,

occupational, and political gains of women in the United States have brought improvements in

their body images” (p.26).

If modern women in the United States are making such great strides in gender equality,

why has the feminine body ideal shifted? Society barrages women with negative body image

messages through a multitude of outlets. The messages are seen on television, in magazines,

heard in the halls of schools and every time women walk past mirrors (Waterhouse, 1997). They

are ingrained in culture, to the extent that the messages are hardly noticed. “Though many

women, spurred by the revival of the feminist movement, are presenting themselves more

proudly and confidently in public, in the privacy of their own minds too few seem to have

favorable images of themselves” (Sanford & Donovan, 1985, p.4).

The feminist movement sought to release women from the objectification of their bodies.

“How anyone experiences, fantasizes about, or internally represents her or his embodiment

grows from experience, learning and self-definition in the family and in the culture” (Chodorow,

1989, p. 101). The existence of a gendered and sexually organized world does not allow society

to experience embodiment as simply being in a body. Gender and sex differences create a

dichotomy of embodiment that dictates how women experience femininity and determines who

women are, what women do, and how women are perceived (Chodorow, 1989).

32
Sanford and Donovan (1985) argued that “none of the ideal standards by which we judge

ourselves was with us at birth” (p.8). Women learn how to judge themselves through their

culture and family unit. Various components of one’s images and beliefs of self are assembled

like a collage, depicting an identity and sense of self.

It is as if "women are locked into a generational cycle of female body insecurity and

devaluation” (Clarke & Giffin, 2007, p. 715). Women are constantly bombarded with body

image messages from their mothers and the basic ideas women have about themselves were

acquired prior to adulthood (Archibald, Graber, & Brooks-Gunn, 1999; Clarke & Griffin, 2007;

Francis & Birch, 2005; Liechty, Freeman, & Zabriskie, 2006). It has been argued that dieting

has become a national epidemic and binds mothers and daughters (Waterhouse, 1997).

This vicious cycle of body dissatisfaction can be explained by Objectification Theory

(Fredrickson & Roberts, 1997). The theory posited that women’s bodies are looked at,

evaluated, and objectified. This objectification means that women are simply treated as bodies

that exist for the pleasure of people around them. There is significant pressure for women to be

preoccupied with their bodies. Women internalize observers’ perspectives of their bodies

leading to habitual self-conscious body monitoring and self-objectification.

Utilizing this theory, Strelan and Hargreaves (2005) conducted a study on one hundred

thirty-two university students and their friends (64 women and 68 men) who completed three

questionnaires: the Self-Objectification Questionnaire (Noll & Fredrickson, 1998), 2) a modified

version of that questionnaire that measured individuals’ objectification of others, and 3) the Body

Cathexis scale (Slade, Dewey, Newton, & Brodie, 1990). They found that women were more

33
likely than men to self-objectify, self-objectification was negatively related to body satisfaction

for women, and both men and women objectified other women more than they objectified men.

Self-esteem has often been linked with body satisfaction (Ben-Tovim, Walker, Murray, &

Chin, 1990; Sanford & Donovan, 1985; Usmiani & Daniluk, 1997). However, there have

recently been a lot of criticisms about self-esteem as a measure of psychological health

(Baumeister, Smart, & Boden, 1996; Baumeister, Campbell, Krueger, & Vohs, 2003; Crocker,

Luhtanen, Cooper, & Bouvrette, 2003; Twenge, Konrath, Foster, Campbell, & Bushman, 2008).

To address these criticisms, Neff (2003a) proposed a new construct, self-compassion, which is an

alternative conceptualization of a healthy attitude and relationship to oneself. This healthier

attitude has been linked to less self-judgment (Neff, 2003a) and less judgment of others (Brown,

1999). Therefore, one could argue that higher levels of self-compassion could lead to less self-

objectification and less objectification of others. The next section addresses this objectification

through a review of the theory, as well as a literature review of self-objectification and

objectification of others.

Objectification Theory

The physical body has traditionally been the defining characteristic used to distinguish

men from women. Anatomical, genetic, and hormonal influences on personality, experience,

and behavior have been routinely explored. This biological basis has long been criticized by

feminists, as they have argued that gender differences have little to do with the physical body

and more to do with the socialization of boys and girls. Bodies exist within social and cultural

contexts and are therefore constructed within sociocultural practices (Fredrickson & Roberts,

1997).

34
Western culture is saturated with heterosexuality and the assumption that men may

sexualize women (Westkott, 1986). Within this sexual gazing, there exists the possibility for

sexual objectification, or women being treated as just bodies. The mass media has propagated

this sexual objectification through sexualized images of the female body, making these images

unavoidable in the American culture (Fredrickson & Roberts, 1997). Research has indicated that

an unattainable standard of women’s physical beauty is what is depicted in the media. This

narrow view is often linked with a woman’s sexiness and worth (APA, 2007).

Fredrickson and Roberts (1997) sought to uncover why objectification occurs, with the

understanding that women exist in a culture in which their bodies are looked at and evaluated.

Objectification Theory “places female bodies in a sociocultural context with the aim of

illuminating the lived experiences and mental health risks of girls and women who encounter

sexual objectification” (p. 174). The researchers argued that sexual objectification is only one

form of gender oppression, but that it may perhaps act as a catalyst for other oppressions women

face.

Objectification Theory posited that girls and women treat themselves as objects to be

looked at and evaluated. This adoption of an observer’s perspective may be due to compliance

with external pressures (Fredrickson & Roberts, 1997). Effective socialization begins with

compliance with external pressures, leading to interpersonal identification, and ending with

individuals claiming ownership of socialized values and attitudes. This ownership may result in

a new notion of self (Costanzo, 1992).

35
Self-Objectification

Women learn that their “looks” matter and that how they are viewed by observers can

determine how they are treated. Objectification Theory argued that women can adopt a “third-

person” view of themselves as a way to control how they are treated (Fredrickson & Roberts,

1997). Physical beauty can be powerful for women (Citrin, Roberts, & Fredrickson, 2004, p.

208); however, a majority of studies indicated negative psychological consequences of sexual

objectification (Moradi & Huang, 2008).

Utilizing Fredrickson & Roberts’ (1997) Objectification Theory, Frederick, Peplau, and

Lever (2006) studied correlates of body image in a sample of 52,677 heterosexual men and

women. The researchers examined associations of body satisfaction to age, height, gender, and

BMI and found that fewer men than women reported being too heavy (41% versus 61%) and

rated their body as being unattractive (11% versus 21%), consistent with Objectification Theory.

In addition, slender women (BMI = 14.5-22.49) reported more body satisfaction than most of the

other women, also consistent with Objectification Theory.

Mercurio and Landry (2008) investigated the impact of self-objectification on women’s

overall feelings of self-worth and their perceptions of their own well-being in terms of life

satisfaction. Two hundred twenty-seven female undergraduates completed the Objectified Body

Consciousness Scale (McKinley & Hyde, 1996), the Rosenberg Self-Esteem Scale (Rosenberg,

1965), and the Satisfaction with Life Scale (Diener, Emmons, Larsen, & Griffin, 1985). The

researchers found that self-objectification impacts overall life satisfaction through its relation

with body shame and self-esteem.

36
Greenleaf and McGreer (2006) examined Objectification Theory (Fredrickson & Roberts,

1997) among physically active (N= 115) and sedentary (N= 70) female college students. The

women completed the Self-Objectification Questionnaire (Noll & Fredrickson, 1998), the Body

Surveillance and Body Shame subscales of the Objectified Body Consciousness Scale

(McKinley & Hyde, 1996), the Appearance Anxiety Scale (Dion, Dion, & J. Keelan, 1990), a

flow experiences measure (Tiggemann & Slater, 2001), and the Eating Attitudes Test–26

(Garner, Olmsted, Bohr, & Garfinkel, 1982). They found that self-objectification predicted

disordered eating in both groups of women and those women high in self-objectification reported

higher levels of body surveillance, body shame, appearance anxiety, and self-reported disordered

eating attitudes.

Rolnik, Engeln-Maddox, and Miller (2010) investigated the impact of sorority rush on

self-objectification and body image disturbance. First-year undergraduate women either

participating (n=68) or not participating (n=59) in sorority rush at a U.S. Midwestern university

completed the Objectified Body Consciousness Scale (McKinley & Hyde, 1996), the Eating

Attitudes Test–26 (Garner, Olmsted, Bohr, & Garfinkel, 1982), a demographics questionnaire

developed by the researchers, and a rush-specific questionnaire developed by the researchers.

The researchers found that women who dropped out of rush had significantly higher body mass

indexes (BMI’s) prior to beginning the rush process, as compared to those who completed the

rush process, and rushees’ BMI’s significantly predicted whether they would drop out of rush.

Consistent with previous findings that sorority women are especially likely to promote a thin

body ideal (Schulken, Pinciaro, Sawyer, Jensen, & Hoban, 1997), this research illustrated that

the further women’s bodies were from the culturally sanctioned thin ideal, the more negative

37
their experience of sorority rush was. The researchers also found that rush participants

evidenced increased levels of self-objectification and eating disordered behavior.

Objectification of Others

Not only do women self-objectify (Greenleaf & McGreer, 2006;Mercurio& Landry,

2008;Peplau& Lever, 2006; Rolnik, Engeln-Maddox, & Miller, 2010) but they objectify other

women (Bearman, Korobov, & Thorne, 2009; Strelan & Hargreaves, 2005). This objectification

of other women may take the form of comments or suggestions, glances, and other behaviors that

communicate the thin-ideal to which women should conform (Szymanski, Moffitt, & Carr,

2011).

Tylka and Sabik (2010) studied Social Comparison Theory (Festinger, 1954) and self-

esteem as they were integrated into the framework of Objectification Theory (Fredrickson &

Roberts, 1997). Two hundred and seventy-four women from a Midwestern U.S. college

completed the Feedback on Physical Appearance Scale (Tantleff-Dunn, Thompson, & Dunn,

1995), the Rosenberg Self Esteem Scale (Rosenberg, 1965), the Body Surveillance subscale and

the Body Shame subscale of the Objectified Body Consciousness Scale (McKinley & Hyde,

1996), the Body Comparison Scale (Fisher & Thompson, 1998), and the Eating Attitudes Test-26

(Garner et al., 1982). The researchers found that women who are focused on their own bodies

are more attentive to the appearance of other women’s bodies and how they compare to their

own. This body comparison may act as a feedback loop to their own body, perpetuating

heightened body surveillance and objectification of other women’s bodies. The researchers also

found that self-esteem uniquely predicted body surveillance, body comparison, and body shame.

38
This finding illustrated the importance of including self-esteem as a variable within the

objectification framework.

Szymanski et al. (2011) argued that to “properly address women’s own contribution to

sexually objectifying environments, a great deal more research is needed to explore the ways

women objectify other women,” (p. 29). Szymanski and Carr (2011) emphasized the need for

psychologists to empower therapy clients to address social problems such as sexual

objectification. They see this empowerment as a social justice initiative “given the harmful role

of external and internalized sexual objectification and other forms of oppression on individuals’

psychosocial health,” (p.165).

Definition of Body Image

We cannot fully understand the power of objectification without first understanding body

image (Fredrickson & Roberts, 1997). Body image is difficult to clearly define, but there is a

general agreement within the literature that it is multidimensional and complex and involves

biological, psychological, and social factors (Blyth, Simmons, &Zakin, 1985; Petersen,

Schulenberg, Abramobcwitz, Offer, & Jarcho, 1984). Hutchinson (1982) suggested:

Body image is not the same as body, but is rather what the mind does to the body

in translating the experience of embodiment into its mental representation. This

translation from body to body image and from there to body- cathexis is a

complex and emotionally charged process (p. 59).

In this context, body- cathexis was defined as body esteem or body satisfaction.

39
Transmission of Body Image

Body image ideals are transmitted through a variety of sources. The media tells women

what a normal body should look like (Bandura, 1977; Becker, 1994; Harper & Tiggemann, 2008;

Jhally, 2001; Sanford & Donovan, 1985; Strahan et al., 2008). In addition to the media, familial

influence begins at an early age and shapes the way women view their bodies through

socialization, modeling, and encouragement.

Media

An abundant source of social learning is the symbolic modeling provided by television,

films, and other visual media. Bandura (1977) stated:

During the course of their daily lives, people have direct contact with only a small

sector of the environment. Consequently, their perceptions of social reality are

heavily influenced by vicarious experiences- what they see, hear, and read in the

mass media (p.40).

The Internet, another vehicle of mass visual media, is widely used among college students. The

Pew Internet and American Life Project found that young adults are much more likely to spend

time online than the general population. Personal computers, home broadband access and the

use of cell phones make internet access easy for college students (Smith, Rainie, & Zickuhr,

2011).

Sanford and Donovan (1985) found that advertising, television, and fashion magazines

promote cultural ideals becoming the teacher of normalcy for a woman’s body when other

sources of information are lacking. In Killing Us Softly 3 (Jhally, 2001), a film on media

influence on women, the lecturer, Jean Kilbourne, quoted, “What does advertising tell us today

40
about women? It tells us, just as it did 10 and 20 and 30 years ago, that what’s most important

about women is how we look.”

Western societies value a slim body for women, and nonconformity to this value leads to

negative social consequences. “Being overweight is linked to laziness, lack of willpower, and

being out of control” (Grogan, 2008, p.9). It is understood in western societies that women

should change their body shape and size to conform to current trends. These changes may be the

result of dieting, exercise, and body modification such as surgery (Grogan, 2008).

Becker (2004), an anthropologist and psychiatrist, collected narrative data via semi-

structured interviews with Fijian adolescent girls regarding the impact of the introduction of

television in their culture. Traditional Fijian cultural norms strongly support robust bodies and

appetites. This was, in part, due to the reflection of a large body being capable of hard work and

care and nurturing from a dense social network. After the introduction of television in 1995, the

Fijian youth have been affected on multiple levels. Becker stated:

The ensuing changes in self and body image are multifaceted. On the most

superficial and concrete level, television appeared to redefine local aesthetic

ideals for bodily appearance and presentation. Television scenarios appeared to

stimulate desire to acquire elements of the lifestyles portrayed, including the body

shape perceived to be best suited for obtaining a job. Subjects explicitly reported

modeling behavior and appearance on television characters. (p. 540)

With the introduction of television, Becker found an increased prevalence of disordered eating in

ethnic Fijian school girls. Becker concluded:

It is a logical and frightening conclusion that vulnerable girls and women across

41
diverse populations who feel marginalized from the locally dominant culture’s

sources of prestige and status may anchor their identities in widely recognized

cultural symbols of prestige popularized by media-imported ideas, values, and

images. (p. 555)

Strahan et al. (2008) conducted a two part study in which sociocultural norms were

challenged in search of a basis for self-worth. Study one exposed undergraduate women to

media images (commercials) that strongly conveyed cultural norms for ideal appearance (thin,

attractive women). Participants completed a shortened version of the Contingencies of Self-

Worth Scale (Crocker & Wolfe, 2001), a shortened version of the State Self-Esteem Scale

(Heatherton &Polivy, 1991), and seven items from the Self-Consciousness Scale (Fenigstein,

Scheier, & Buss, 1975). In addition, participants were also asked their height and were weighed

to determine their BMI. Researchers found that exposure to images reflecting these cultural

norms led participants to base their self-worth more strongly on their appearance, which led to

more dissatisfaction with their bodies.

The second part of the study conducted by Strahan et al. (2008) involved an intervention

reducing the impact of sociocultural norms conducted at local public schools. Researchers

utilized activities designed to challenge norms for ideal appearance that convey the notion that

“people need to look like the unattainable ideals portrayed in the media to be accepted and

successful in life” (p. 293). The adolescents who participated completed a two-item measure of

perceived legitimacy of norms (Strahan et al., 2008), the internalization component the

Sociocultural Attitudes Toward Appearance Questionnaire (SATAQ) (Heinberg, Thompson, &

Stormer, 1995), a shortened version of the Contingencies of Self-Worth Scale (Crocker & Wolfe,

42
2001), a shortened version of the State Self-Esteem Scale (Heatherton & Polivy, 1991), and three

items from the Self-Consciousness Scale (Fenigstein et al., 1975). Researchers found that

adolescent girls who received the challenge to the norm intervention based their self-worth less

strongly on appearance, which led to more body satisfaction and less concerns with others’

perceptions.

Harper and Tiggemann (2008) examined whether viewing media images would increase

self-objectification, negative affect, and body dissatisfaction in young Australian women. Ninety

undergraduate women were randomly assigned to view advertisements that depicted a thin

woman, a thin woman with an attractive man, or one that did not feature any people (product

control advertisement). Participants then completed a Media Consumption Questionnaire

developed by the researchers, a Consumer Response Questionnaire, adapted from Mills, Polivy,

Herman, & Tiggemann (2002), a modified version of the Twenty Statements Test (Fredrickson

et al., 1998), the Physical Appearance State and Trait Anxiety Scale (Reed, Thompson,

Brannick, & Sacco, 1991), the Visual Analogue Scales (Heinberg & Thompson, 1995), and the

Self-Objectification Questionnaire (Noll & Fredrickson, 1998). Researchers concluded that

women who viewed advertisements featuring a thin-idealized woman reported greater self-

objectification, weight-related appearance anxiety, negative mood, and body dissatisfaction than

participants who viewed product control advertisements. These advertisements were not

explicitly directed to the attention of the participants’ bodies, but rather modeled the thin-ideal.

These results demonstrated that women tended to model the Western notion of beauty, which is a

thin woman.

43
Familial Transmission

Several models of eating and weight related values and behaviors, between parent and

child, were assessed by Wertheim, Martin, Prior, Sanson, and Smart (2002). Six hundred and

nineteen parent-daughter pairs completed a demographics questionnaire, the Eating Disorders

Inventory (Garner, 1991; Garner, Olmsted, & Polivy, 1983), Body Figure Rating Scales for

Children (Collins, 1991) and dieting self-reports. The mean age of the girls was 12.81 (SD=

.23). The researchers found that the daughters’ drive for thinness and body dissatisfaction were

related to encouragement to diet from both parents, though mothers were found to be more

influential than fathers. They also found that menstrual girls had higher BMIs, greater body

dissatisfaction and higher levels of disordered eating.

Neumark-Sztainer, Bauer, Friend, Hannan, Story, and Berge (2010) examined the

associations between family variables including: parents weight talk, parent dieting, and family

weight-teasing and adolescent girls’ weight status, body satisfaction, and disordered eating

behaviors. Forty-five percent of the girls reported that their mothers encouraged them to diet and

fifty-eight percent reported that their family members teased them because of their weight.

Researchers found that weight-teasing was strongly associated with higher BMI, body

dissatisfaction, and unhealthy weight control behaviors. Parent weight talk, especially by

mothers, was associated with disordered eating among the adolescent girls and mother dieting

was associated with girls’ unhealthy weight control behaviors.

Research has suggested that for girls, mothers are primary agents of socialization about

the body and may act as a vehicle for carrying messages about body image from one generation

to the next (Archibald, Graber, & Brooks-Gunn, 2000; Clarke & Griffin, 2007; Francis & Birch,

44
2005; Liechty, Freeman, & Zabriskie, 2006; Neumark-Sztainer et al., 2010; Wertheim, 2002).

However, the role of the father and other family members has also been associated with

adolescent girls’ body dissatisfaction and weight control behaviors (Neumark-Sztainer et al.,

2010; Wertheim, 2002). Objectification Theory stated that women’s bodies are continuously

looked at and evaluated by others and consequently objectification is internalized and leads to

habitual body monitoring. Women are critical of their bodies and this self-criticism directs their

weight control behaviors (Fredrickson & Roberts, 1997).

This section defined body image and addressed the transmission of the construct to

include media, culture and family. The following section will define the construct of self-

compassion, compare it with self-esteem, and explore the construct in the current literature.

Special attention will be paid to the constructs as they relate to body image.

Self-Compassion

Neff’s (2003a) construct of self-compassion is a conceptualization of a healthy attitude and

relationship to oneself. Self-compassion allows individuals to see themselves with the

nonjudgmental understanding, and sense of humanness, that is often afforded to others. Neff

(2003a) defined self-compassion as:

being touched by and open to one’s own suffering, not avoiding or disconnecting from it,

generating the desire to alleviate one’s suffering and to heal oneself with kindness. Self-

compassion also involves offering nonjudgmental understanding to one’s pain,

inadequacies and failures, so that one’s experience is seen as part of the larger human

experience (p. 87)

45
As part of this definition, Neff (2003a) described three components of self-compassion that work

together for mutual enhancement:

(a) self-kindness – extending kindness and understanding to oneself rather than harsh

judgment and self-criticism, (b) common humanity – seeing one’s experiences as part of

the larger human experience rather than seeing them as separating and isolating, and (c)

mindfulness – holding one’s painful thoughts and feelings in balanced awareness rather

than over-identifying with them (p.89).

Self-Kindness

The kindness component of self-compassion recognizes that humans experience

challenges in life and cannot be perfect. When one encounters suffering, feelings of inadequacy,

or failure, self-compassion encourages warmth and understanding toward oneself. When life’s

realities are denied, suffering occurs in the form of self-criticism. When they are accepted with

kindness, greater emotional balance may occur (Neff, 2003a; Neff, 2008).

Common Humanity

The recognition that all humans suffer allows one to see that suffering and failure are part

of the shared human experience and are something larger than the self. These shared experiences

leave little room for a separate self-perspective, or failure in isolation.

It is this isolation that perpetuates a competitive mind-set that leads to self-judgment and

comparison to others (Neff, 2003a; Neff, 2008).

Mindfulness

Mindfulness allows individuals to observe their thoughts and emotions as they exist,

without attempting to suppress, change or deny them. In this way, mindfulness allows the

46
individual to be nonjudgmental toward the self (Neff, 2003a; Neff, 2008). Mindfulness does not

negate negative emotions or feelings, rather it allows for emotional balance that prevents the

individual from overly identifying with an emotional state (Bishop et al., 2004).

Self-compassion tends to soften ego-protective barriers, allowing us to be kind to

ourselves, rather than just being kind to those around us. This self-kindness does not require an

inflated self-image and is available when self-esteem is not. It is available “when we fall flat on

our face, embarrass ourselves, or otherwise come in direct contact with the imperfection of life”

(Neff, 2011, p.6). The following section examines this difference between self-compassion and

self-esteem.

Self-Compassion versus Self-Esteem

Rogers (1951) defined self-esteem as the extent to which an individual likes, values, and

accepts himself or herself. Self-esteem is central in everything that one does. Having positive

self-esteem enhances one’s confidence, self-image, and overall happiness. Having negative self-

esteem creates negative thinking and leads one to believe negative comments made by others

(Perera, n.d.).

Self-esteem has routinely been the marker of psychological well-being (Erikson, 1968;

Rosenberg, 1979) to the extent that the self is seen as separate from shared human experience.

Within recent years, there have also been a lot of criticisms about self-esteem as a measure of

psychological health (Baumeister et al., 1996; Baumeister et al., 2003; Crocker et al., 2003;

Harter, 1999; Twenge et al., 2008). It was found that self-esteem is largely the outcome of doing

well, rather than the cause of doing well (Baumeister et al., 2003). It was also found that the

emphasis placed on self-esteem in our society has led to an increase in narcissism scores of

47
college students. Researchers found that 65% of recent college students scored higher in

narcissism than previous generations of students (Twenge et al., 2008). Finally, self-esteem may

be linked to inflated self-views, which could be problematic when it is contingent on a particular

outcome (Crocker et al., 2003).

Harter (1999) found that global self-esteem is shaped by evaluations of self-worth in

domains such as appearance, social approval, and academic/work performance. This means that

other potentially important life successes may be neglected in order to maintain high self-esteem.

It also means that one’s self-esteem is contingent upon recent successes and failures. Kernis

(2005) found that this contingent self-esteem makes people more vulnerable to depression and

reduced self-concept clarity. Further exemplifying this finding, the following section addresses

the link between body image and self-esteem and how this may be harmful to the individual.

Body image and self-esteem. Usmiani and Daniluk (1997) argued that “body image is formed

to a degree as a function of the culturally defined images of desirable bodily appearances for men

and women” (p. 47) and that from this perspective “a girls’ self-esteem may be influenced by

the degree to which she believes she meets cultural standards” (p. 47). A woman’s sense of who

she is may be defined through conformity within a culture. Women in the United States may

form their identity using a Westernized notion of feminine stereotypes that depict how a woman

is expected to look and act (Grogan, 2008).

Discussing their self-esteem enhancement groups, Sanford and Donovan (1985) found

that nearly every woman in their self-esteem enhancement groups had a negative body image.

The group concluded that it is “difficult to dislike your body or a specific part of your body and

still like yourself,” (p. 369). It is hard to discuss self-esteem without also including body image.

48
For some women, a poor body image leads to low self-esteem and for others, a low self-esteem

lends itself to poor body image.

Sanford and Donovan (1985) argued that parents’ self-acceptance influences a child’s

self-perception. Parents acting as role models can often be just as negative of an influence as

they are a positive influence.

My mother was constantly concerned about her weight when I was a kid, and when I

reached puberty, she decided I looked “porky.” She dragged me to the gym with her,

which I hated, and we both went on the same diets, although I ate tremendous volumes at

school. I spent my allowance on hot lunches, in addition to eating the rabbit food she

sent along with me. She was always on me about how I looked, until I gave up and

withdrew. I stopped eating around her. This concerned her right away and she took me

to the doctor who told her I was the correct weight and that all my bulges were just

normal. After that, she left me alone (p. 369).

The researchers speculated that if the mother had a more positive self-concept, she may have

recognized that her daughter did not have a weight problem. If, in reality, the daughter did have

a weight problem, the mother could have demonstrated some reasonable and safe ways to

manage her weight.

Sanford and Donovan (1985) also argued that while some women do not see themselves

in an entirely negative light, they may still lack self-esteem. They may be well aware of their

good qualities, but have a hard time understanding that their good qualities matter as much as

their negative qualities or flaws. “‘Oh, I know I’ve got a nice face, that I’m pretty bright, a good

49
teacher, a kind person and all that,’ a woman might say. ‘But,’ she is quick to add, ‘I should lose

ten pounds,’”(p. 16).

Recent research suggests that self-compassion is more predictive of healthy self-related

functioning than is self-esteem (Neff & Vonk, 2006). Measures of self-compassion and global

self-esteem, as well as a number of self-related processes known to be maladaptive: self-esteem

instability and contingency, social comparison, narcissism, reactive anger, public self-

consciousness, and self-rumination were completed by the research participants. The researchers

found, after accounting for variance in outcomes attributable to self-esteem, self-compassion

predicted significant additional variance for self-esteem instability, self-esteem contingency,

social comparison, reactive anger, public self-consciousness, and self-rumination. Moreover, in

almost every case, self-compassion was a much stronger negative predictor of these outcomes

than was self-esteem.

Neff, Kirkpatrick, and Rude (2007) examined the relationship of self-compassion to

psychological health. Ninety-one undergraduates completed measures on self-compassion, self-

esteem, negative affectivity, anxiety. They found that self-compassion, and not self-esteem,

buffered anxiety when participants were faced with an ego-threat in a laboratory setting.

Neff (2003a; 2009; 2011) argued that there is a way for us to feel good about ourselves

without the positive or negative self-judgment or evaluation commonly associated with self-

esteem. Her research on the construct of self-compassion found that it offers similar benefits to

self-esteem, but without the pitfalls. She found that self-compassion is equally relevant when

suffering occurs either through external circumstances or one’s personal inadequacies. Also,

Neff (2004) suggested that because self-compassion does not require people to adopt an

50
unrealistic view of the self, it should be easier to raise than self-esteem. The following section

addresses self-compassion in the current literature.

Self-Compassion in the Literature

Research has helped establish the advantageous nature of self-compassion. Thus far,

most of the research on self-compassion has been conducted using the Self-Compassion Scale

(Neff, 2003a), which measures the degree to which individuals display self-kindness versus self-

judgment, common humanity versus isolation, and mindfulness versus over identification.

Utilizing this scale, higher levels of self-compassion have been associated with greater life-

satisfaction, emotional intelligence, social connectedness, learning goals, wisdom, personal

initiative, curiosity, happiness, optimism, and positive affect, as well as less self-criticism,

depression, anxiety, fear of failure, thought suppression, perfectionism, performance goals, and

disordered eating behaviors (Neff, 2009, for a review). Self-compassion recognizes that we all

have strengths and weaknesses and that we are “imperfect human beings” (Neff, 2011, p. 9).

Neff, Hsieh, and Dejitthirat, (2005) examined the link between self-compassion and

motivation among college students to determine whether self-compassion might be adaptive in

academic contexts. Using a sample size of 222 college students, the researchers found that self-

compassion was positively associated with mastery goals, which include the joy of learning for

its own sake, and negatively associated with performance goals, which involve defending or

enhancing one’s sense of self-worth through academic performances. These findings were

replicated (N= 110) with students who had recently failed a midterm examination and indicated

that self-compassionate students exhibited more adaptive ways of coping with failure.

51
In a similar study, Akin (2008) examined the relationship between self-compassion and

achievement goal orientation. Research participants included 646 university students in Turkey

who completed scales on self-compassion and achievement goal orientation. Results concluded

that self-compassion was positively related to learning approach and negatively related to

performance approach. Given the findings, the author called for educational settings to foster

self-compassion in students.

In a study designed to explore the relationships among self-compassion, positive

psychological functioning, and the Big Five personality traits in college students (N=177), Neff,

Rude, and Kirkpatrick (2007) found that self-compassion was associated with greater reflective

and affective wisdom, curiosity and exploration, happiness, optimism, positive affect,

Extraversion, Agreeableness and Conscientiousness. It was found that self-compassion was

negatively associated with Neuroticism. They also found that self-compassion was linked to

personal initiative, which supports the notion that self-compassion leads to self-growth.

Neff and McGehee (2010) examined self-compassion among adolescents (N=235) and

young adults (N=287). Utilizing measures of self-compassion, depression, anxiety,

connectedness, maternal support, family functioning, attachment, and personal fable, the

researchers found that self-compassion was strongly associated with well-being for both age

groups. They also found that self-compassion contributed to well-being while controlling other

factors, suggesting that self-compassion’s mental health benefits were not confounded by family

influence or life stage. The authors argued that this finding may further support the notion that

self-passion can be enhanced with practice (Gilbert and Proctor, 2006).

52
Leary, Tate, Adams, Allen, and Hancock (2007) utilized five separate studies that

investigated the cognitive and emotional processes by which self-compassionate people deal

with unpleasant life events. In the first study, 117 undergraduate students reported on negative

life events. Results showed that self-compassion predicted emotional and cognitive reactions to

negative events. The second study asked 123 undergraduate students to respond to hypothetical

scenarios which elicited feelings of loss, failure, and humiliation. The researchers found that

self-compassion buffered people against negative emotions self-feelings when imagining

distressing social events. In the third study, 66 undergraduate students disclosed personal

information to another individual and then received either positive or neutral feedback. Their

emotional reactions and judgments of the other person were then assessed. The results

concluded that self-compassion moderated negative emotions and receiving ambivalent feedback

and this was especially true for participants who scored low on self-esteem. The fourth study

videotaped 102 undergraduate students while they performed an awkward and mildly

embarrassing task. The videotapes were then either self-rated or rated by another participant.

The results showed that low-self-compassionate participants undervalued their own videotaped

performances relative to observers. The final study was designed to examine how self-

compassion moderates reactions to remembered life events and to examine if self-compassion

could be experimentally induced among the 115 undergraduate research participants. Results

showed that self-compassion allows individuals to acknowledge their role in negative events

without feeling overwhelmed with negative emotions and that self-compassion could be

experimentally induced.

53
Continuing the use of a self-compassion manipulation, Zabelina and Robinson (2010)

studied 86 undergraduate students to better understand the use of a self-compassion manipulation

as it relates to original creative thinking. The researchers found that self-judgmental individuals

displayed lower levels of creative originality in the control condition, but equal levels of creative

originality in the self-compassion manipulation condition. They also further exemplified the

positive outcome of a self-compassion induction.

Self-compassion and body image. A pilot study conducted by Gilbert and Irons in 2004

found that self-critical thoughts were linked to many different social situations. Prior to a

compassionate mind training technique, participants were asked to keep diaries to explore self-

critical themes. When the participants were asked to describe the situations or events that

elicited self-criticism, they identified being at the gym and body image-related situations, among

others. Thus, while self-compassion has been conceptualized as a construct related to the overall

self, it could also be more relevant to specific self-perceptions of the physical self.

Investigating the outcome of inducing a state of self-compassion on restrained eating,

Leary and Adams (2007) asked 84 female undergraduate students to complete the Revised Rigid

Restraint Scale, designed by the researchers. They were then randomly assigned to one of the

experimental condition: preload/self-compassion condition, preload/no self-compassion

condition, or no preload/control condition. The preload consisted of a set amount of food and

then all participants performed a bogus taste test to measure eating behavior and completed a

self-evaluation. The researchers found that highly restrictive eaters felt worse after eating the

preload compared to the control condition, but those who completed the self-compassion

induction showed lower negative affect and reduced self-criticism.

54
Magnus et al., (2010) found that self-compassion was positively related to intrinsic

motivation and negatively related to external and introjected motivation, ego goal orientation,

social physique anxiety and exercising in ways that are harmful to the self. They found that self-

compassion may foster a healthy attitude towards the self and identified the need for physical

self-compassion as a potential construct for future exploration.

Furthering this idea, Berry, Kowalski, Ferguson, and McHugh (2010) proposed that body

self-compassion could be a sub-domain of global self-compassion. Using Neff’s (2003)

research, they defined body self-compassion as “a kind, understanding and non-judgmental

attitude individuals extend toward their body in response to their perceived physical

imperfections, limitations and failures” (p. 295). Using this new construct, the researchers

interviewed five young adult women using an empirical phenomenological method. Three

essential themes emerged as the women experienced more body self-compassion: appreciating

one’s unique body, taking ownership of one’s body and engaging in less social comparison.

These findings suggest the benefits of increasing body self-compassion.

Conclusion and Research Questions

Sexual objectification is harmful to women’s psychosocial health and is a social justice

issue. This form of oppression leads to increased body surveillance, body shame and decreased

self-esteem. Szymanski and Carr (2011) called for social justice initiatives surrounding

empowerment of female clients and Szymanksi et al. (2011) suggested the need for further

research about women who objectify other women.

Recent critics of self-esteem (Baumeister et al., 1996; Baumeister et al., 2003; Crocker et

al., 2003; Harter, 1999; Twenge et al., 2008) have opened the doors to a new conceptualization

55
of a healthy attitude and relationship to oneself in the construct of self-compassion (Neff, 2003a).

Given the recent research on self-compassion (Neff, 2003a; 2009, 2011) and the new construct of

body or physical self-compassion (Berry et al., 2010; Magnus et al., 2010, respectively), one can

hypothesize that women with higher self-compassion will be less likely to self-objectify and

objectify other women. We also know from the research that self-compassion can be

experimentally induced (Leary & Adams, 2007; Leary at al., 2007; Zabelina & Robinson, 2010).

Research has establish the advantages of self-compassion to include greater life-

satisfaction, emotional intelligence, social connectedness, learning goals, wisdom, personal

initiative, curiosity, happiness, optimism, and positive affect, as well as less self-criticism,

depression, anxiety, fear of failure, thought suppression, perfectionism, performance goals, and

disordered eating behaviors (Neff, 2009, for a review). Using body self-compassion, Berry et al.

(2010) used an empirical phenomenological approach to interview five women and found themes

that suggested that as women experienced more body self-compassion, they appreciated their

unique body, took ownership of their body and engaged in less social comparison. While these

findings suggest the positive influence of increased body self-compassion, the literature has not

extended a body self-compassion induction on women who experience self-objectification and

women who objectify other women.

Based on this review of the literature, it was evident that there was a gap in the literature

with respect to this extension of a self-compassion induction based on objectification. Therefore,

this was the focus of my project. My research questions were:

1. How does a self-compassion induction affect self-objectification?

2. How does a self-compassion induction affect objectification of other women?

56
CHAPTER III

METHODOLOGY

Sexual objectification has been found to be harmful to women’s psychosocial health, as

this form of oppression leads to increased body surveillance, body shame and decreased self-

esteem. The concept of self-compassion (Neff, 2003a), and more specifically, the new concept

of body or physical self-compassion (Berry et al., 2010; Magnus et al., 2010, respectively), has

been found to mitigate this objectification. Szymanski and Carr (2011) called for social justice

initiatives surrounding empowerment of female clients and Szymanksi et al. (2011) suggested

the need for further research about women who objectify other women.

This chapter examines the methodology that was used to address this gap in the research

literature. Utilizing an experimental design, this study examined the effects of a self-compassion

induction on both self-objectification and objectification of other women. Research participants

completed the Self-Objectification Scale (Noll & Fredrickson, 1998), a modified version of this

scale to address objectification of other women, the Rosenberg Self-Esteem Scale (Rosenberg,

1965) and a demographics questionnaire developed by the researcher.

Participants

Women in undergraduate psychology classes at a mid-sized state university and two

undergraduate, online health classes at a large state university were recruited to participate in this

study. Institutional Review Board (IRB) approval was obtained for this study and all participants

provided their informed consent prior to participation. All psychology undergraduate students at

the mid-sized university have access to a research participant scheduling system (SONA),

through which they can register to participate in university research studies. The SONA website

57
will direct participants to complete a series of surveys using Qualtrics survey software. Female

students in the two online, undergraduate health classes at the large university were also

recruited to participate in this research through email. The professor of the class forwarded the

email containing a link to the Qualtrics survey software website to all students in her class. As

an incentive for participating, research participants from the large university were entered into a

drawing to receive an Amazon.com gift card.

Data was collected at the end of the Fall semester and beginning of the Spring semester.

Participants in this study consisted of undergraduate women from one mid-sized state university

and one large state university in the Southeastern United States. There were initially 584

participants in this research study; however, participants who had missing responses or

incomplete answers were removed during data cleaning. Following data cleaning, 382 women

from the mid-sized university and 28 women from the large university remained in the research

sample. The end result was a sample of 410 undergraduate women who completed this research

study. Of those, 220 were randomly assigned to the control group and 190 were randomly

assigned to the self-compassion induction group. See Chapter 4 for a more detailed description

of the research participants.

Instruments

Four instruments were used in this study. The Self-Objectification Questionnaire (Noll &

Fredrickson, 1998) is a ten-item rank order survey used to assess importance of different body

attributes (Appendix A). A modified version of the Self-Objectification Questionnaire was used

to assess objectification of other women (Appendix B). The Rosenberg Self-Esteem Scale

58
(Rosenberg, 1965) is a ten-item Likert scale used to assess general self-esteem (Appendix C).

Finally, a demographics questionnaire was completed by each participant (Appendix D).

Self-Objectification Questionnaire

The Self-Objectification Questionnaire (Noll & Fredrickson, 1998) consisted of ten rank-

ordered items. Each item identifies a different body attribute and participants ranked them from

1 (least important) to 10 (most important) with respect to their physical self-perception. Five of

the items were competence-based (strength, physical coordination, energy level, health and

physical fitness) and five were appearance-based (weight, sex appeal, physical attractiveness,

firm/sculpted muscles and body measurements). Scores may range from -25 to 25, with higher

scores indicating higher levels of self-objectification.

Given the scoring system for the Self-Objectification Questionnaire, the ipsative, ordinal

nature of rank-ordered data, traditional internal consistency estimates cannot be given

(Vanleeuwen & Mandabach, 2002). Reliability is thus determined by correlating the sum of the

appearance ranks and the sum of the competence ranks (Hill & Fischer, 2008). If participants

rank the appearance-based attributes as more important, then the competence-based attributes

must be ranked as less important which creates a negative correlation between the two sets of

attributes. In a study by Colagero and Jost (2011), a strong negative correlation was

demonstrated between appearance and competence rankings, indicating good reliability (r = -

.88). A similar study by Hill and Fischer (2008) also found good reliability (r = -.81). Using a

sample of undergraduate women, Noll (1996) demonstrated construct validity of the Self-

Objectification Questionnaire showing that it positively correlated (r = .52, p<.01) with scores on

the Appearance Anxiety Questionnaire (Dion, Dion, & Keelan, 1990).

59
Objectification of Others Questionnaire

Objectification of other women was measured using a modified version of the Self-

Objectification Questionnaire (Noll & Fredrickson, 1998), consistent with a study conducted by

Strelan and Hargreaves (2005). The instructions for the questionnaire read “This section is

concerned with how women think about other women’s bodies. Listed below are ten different

body attributes. When you think about, or look at other women, which of these body attributes

are most important? Please rank the attributes in order from 1 (least important) to 10 (most

important) in other women.” The participants were then presented with the same ten attributes

listed on the Self-Objectification Questionnaire (Noll & Fredrickson, 1998) and the modified

version of the scale was scored in the same way, with possible scores ranging from -25 to 25.

Rosenberg Self-Esteem Scale

The Rosenberg Self-Esteem Scale is a ten-item self-report scale that measures global self-

esteem. Ten questionnaires were asked on the scale, half of which are phrased as positive

statements and the other half phrased as negative statements of self. Answers were based on a

four point Likert scale ranging from Strongly Agree to Strongly Disagree (Rosenberg, 1965). An

overall score of self-esteem was calculated by averaging participants’ scores across items, with a

high score indicating a high level of overall self-esteem.

A review of the Rosenberg Self-Esteem Scale showed good internal reliability, with

Cronbach’s alpha levels ranging from .72 to .87 and test-retest reliability ranging from .85 to .88.

The Rosenberg Self-Esteem Scale has also been found to relate to other measures of self-esteem,

such as the Coopersmith’s Self-Esteem Inventory (Wylie, 1989). The Rosenberg scale has been

used for a wide variety of populations and is a widely utilized self-esteem scale in research

60
studies (Brown, 2008; Forbes, Jobe, & Richardson, 2006; Hahn-Smith & Smith, 2001; Hatcher,

2007; Mercurio & Landry, 2008).

Demographics

The demographic questionnaire was designed by this researcher and was used to obtain

biographical information about the research participants. It included questions pertaining to age,

ethnicity, class year, weight, height and history of an eating disorder. Each participant’s reported

height and weight was used to calculate a Body Mass Index (BMI). Engel (n.d.) stated the

formula used for this calculation:

BMI = ( _________Weight in Pounds_________ ) x 703


(Height in inches) x (Height in inches)

Procedure

Similar to Leary et al.’s (2007) Study 5, participants wrote about a negative event from

their past and answer questions about it. After viewing an informed consent document

(Appendix E& F) and clicking a box indicating consent, participants were asked to think about a

negative event they experienced in high school or college that made them feel badly about their

bodies – something that involved failure, humiliation or rejection. Participants were then asked

to describe the event by including details regarding what led to the event, who was present, what

happened during the event, and how they felt and behaved at the time (Appendix G). After

writing about the event, participants were randomly assigned to one of two conditions: a) self-

compassion induction or 2) control group.

Self-Compassion Induction

Participants in the self-compassion condition (Appendix H) responded to three prompts

which instructed them to consider their negative event in a self-compassion manner (Neff,

61
2003a). The first prompt, designed to focus on the common humanity element of self-

compassion, asked participants to list ways in which other women have experienced a similar

event. The second prompt, designed to focus on self-kindness, asked participants to write how

they would express understanding, kindness and concern to themselves in the same way they

might express concern to a friend who had lived the same experience. The final prompt asked

participants to describe their feelings about the event in an objective and unemotional way,

which was designed to induce a mindful perspective on the event.

Control Group

Participants in the control condition also responded to three prompts to make the control

condition as similar to the self-compassion condition as possible (Appendix I). The first prompt

asked participants to describe the factors they considered in their college choice. The second

prompt asked participants to describe their favorite college class. The final prompt asked

participants to write about their least favorite college class. The prompts were designed to be

neutral, so as to not further induce negative or positive feelings regarding body image.

Following the writing prompts, all participants completed the Self-Objectification

Questionnaire (Noll & Fredrickson, 1998), a modified version of the Self-Objectification

Questionnaire used to determine objectification of other women, the Rosenberg Self-Esteem

Scale (Rosenberg, 1965) and the demographics questionnaire.

Data Analysis

Data was analyzed using SPSS for Windows 18.0. Descriptive statistics were reported

for age, year in college, ethnicity, eating disorder history and BMI. Analysis of variance

(ANOVA) was conducted to determine if there was a difference of scores for self-objectification

62
and objectification of other women between the two groups. Given the results found by Tylka

and Sabik (2010), the Rosenberg Self-Esteem Scale was used to find the unique contribution of

the self-compassion induction beyond what is accounted for by self-esteem.

Hypotheses

Utilizing Objectification Theory (Fredrickson & Roberts, 1997), the current literature on

self-compassion ((Neff, 2003a; 2009, 2011), the new concept of body or physical self-

compassion (Berry et al., 2010; Magnus et al., 2010, respectively), and body image (Demarest &

Allen, 2000; Fallon & Rozin, 1985; Frederick, Peplau, & Lever, 2006; Sanford & Donovan,

1985; Usmiani & Daniluk, 1997), two hypotheses existed for this research.

The recent research on self-compassion (Neff, 2003a; 2009, 2011) and the new concept

of body or physical self-compassion (Berry et al., 2010; Magnus et al., 2010, respectively) has

demonstrated that women with higher levels of self-compassion are kinder to themselves. It was

hypothesized that this kindness toward self can be generalized to kindness toward one’s body

and therefore, women with higher levels self-compassion will be less likely to self-objectify.

Women objectify other women (Bearman, Korobov, & Thorne, 2009; Strelan &

Hargreaves, 2005). This objectification of other women may take the form of comments or

suggestions, glances, and other behaviors that communicate the thin-ideal to which women

should conform (Szymanski, Moffitt, & Carr, 2011). In a 2010 study by Tylka and Sabik, the

researchers found that women who are focused on their own bodies are more attentive to the

appearance of other women’s bodies. This body comparison may act as a feedback loop to their

own body, perpetuating heightened body surveillance and objectification of other women’s

bodies. Given the research on the positive outcomes of increasing one’s self-compassion (Neff,

63
2003a; 2009, 2011), especially related to body self-kindness (Berry et al., 2010; Magnus et al.,

2010), one can hypothesize that a self-compassion induction will not only lead to less self-

objectification but also less objectification of other women.

Thus, it was hypothesized that:

H1: Women who complete a self-compassion induction will show lower scores on the Self-

Objectification Questionnaire, as compared to the control group.

H2: Women who complete a self-compassion induction will show lower scores on the modified

version of the Self-Objectification Questionnaire, measuring objectification of other women, as

compared to the control group.

Conclusion

Previous research has illuminated the positive influence of increased body self-

compassion (Berry et al., 2010), but the literature has not incorporated a body self-compassion

induction to assess its effects on women who experience self-objectification and women who

objectify other women. There has been a call for social justice initiatives surrounding

empowerment of female clients (Szymanski & Carr, 2011) and there is a need for further

research about the factors that lead to objectification of other women (Szymanksi et al., 2011).

This chapter examined the methodology that was used to complete this research study. Utilizing

experimental and control groups, self-objectification and objectification of other women were

examined in this study. Research participants from Radford University and Virginia Tech

completed the Self-Objectification Questionnaire (Noll & Fredrickson, 1998), a modified version

of this questionnaire to address objectification of other women, the Rosenberg Self-Esteem Scale

(Rosenberg, 1965) and a demographics questionnaire developed by the researcher. The data

64
were then analyzed to assess the effects of a self-compassion induction on self-objectification

and objectification of other women.

65
CHAPTER IV

DATA ANALYSIS

The purpose of this research study was to examine the influence of a body self-

compassion induction on college-aged women. Research participants completed a writing

prompt about a negative body image experience and were then randomly assigned to either a

self-compassion induction or control group. The self-compassion induction group answered

three writing prompts meant to mimic the three components of self-compassion (Neff, 2003a),

while the control group answered three writing prompts related to college choice. Following the

writing prompts, all research participants completed the Self-Objectification Questionnaire (Noll

& Fredrickson, 1998), a modified version of this questionnaire to address objectification of other

women, the Rosenberg Self-Esteem Scale (Rosenberg, 1965) and a demographics questionnaire

developed by the researcher. The data were then analyzed to assess the effects of a self-

compassion induction on self-objectification and objectification of other women.

Description of the Sample

Participants in this study consisted of undergraduate women from a mid-sized state

university and a large state university. All psychology undergraduate students at the mid-sized

state university had access to a research participant scheduling system (SONA), through which

they may register to participate in university research studies. The SONA website directed

participants to complete the research using Qualtrics survey software. A total of 499 students

from this university participated in the research.

Female students in two online, undergraduate health classes at a large state university

were recruited, through email, to participate in this research. The researcher sent an email to the

66
professor of the classes and she distributed it to students. The email contained a link to the

Qualtrics survey software website. A total of 85 students from the large state university

participated in the research.

There were initially 584 participants in this research study. Participants who had

missing responses or incomplete answers were removed during data cleaning. Following data

cleaning, 382 women from the mid-sized university and 28 women from the large state

university remained in the research sample. The end result was a sample of 410 undergraduate

women who completed this research study. Of those, 220 were randomly assigned to the control

group and 190 were randomly assigned to the self-compassion induction group. The following

section details this sample.

Sample Demographics

Age, year in college, ethnicity, eating disorder history and BMI were reported using a

demographic survey. Ranges, means and standard deviations are reported for the age and BMI

variables. Frequencies and percentages are reported for the variables of year in college,

ethnicity and eating disorder history. Scores on the three measures: Self-Objectification

Questionnaire (Noll & Fredrickson, 1998), a modified version of this questionnaire to address

objectification of other women and the Rosenberg Self-Esteem Scale (Rosenberg, 1965) are also

reported as frequencies and percentages.

Age, at the time of the study, was reported using the demographic survey. The ages of

the research participants ranged from 18 to 50 years (M=19.30; SD= 2.22). Forty-three percent

of the research participants identified themselves as 18 years of age.

67
Research participants also reported their year in college, at the time of participation.

Two-hundred and twenty-five participants (54.9%) identified themselves as freshman, 81

(19.8%) identified themselves as sophomores, 50 (12.2%) identified themselves as juniors, and

54 (13.2%) identified themselves as seniors.

Ethnicity was also reported using the demographic survey. Three hundred and thirty-six

students (82%) self-identified as Caucasian, three (0.7%) self-identified as Native

American/American Indian, 42 (10.2%) self-identified as African American, three (0.7%) self-

identified as Pacific Islander, five (1.2%) self-identified as Asian, 11 (2.7%) self-identified as

multi-ethnic, and 10 (2.4%) self-identified as “other.”

Research participants were also asked if they had ever been diagnosed with an eating

disorder by a medical or mental health professional. Ten participants (2.4%) responded that

they had been diagnosed with an eating disorder, while 400 (97.6%) responded that they had

never been diagnosed with an eating disorder.

Each participant’s reported height and weight were used to calculate a BMI.

BMI ranged from 15.66 to 40.35, with a mean of 23.40 and a standard deviation of 4.11. Weight

status descriptors from the Center for Disease Control and Prevention (2011) were used. BMI

below 18.5 is considered underweight; BMI between 18.5 and 24.9 is considered normal weight;

BMI between 25.0 and 29.9 is considered overweight; and BMI above 30.0 is considered obese.

See Table 4.1 for details of sample demographics

68
Table 4.1

Demographic Information of Participants

Variable N Percentage
Age
18 175 42.7
19 108 26.3
20 52 12.7
21 44 10.7
22 17 4.1
23 and older 14 3.5

Ethnicity
Caucasian 336 82.0
African American 42 10.2
Asian 5 1.2
Native American/ 3 0.7
American Indian
Pacific Islander 3 0.7
Multi-Ethnic 11 2.7
Other 10 2.4

Year in School
Freshman 225 54.9
Sophomore 81 19.8
Junior 50 12.2
Senior 54 13.2

Body Mass Index (BMI)


Underweight 20 4.9
Normal Weight 275 67.1
Overweight 85 20.7
Obese 30 7.3

Eating Disorder History


Eating Disorder Diagnosis 10 2.4
No Diagnosis 400 97.6

69
Survey Results

Rosenberg self-esteem scale. The Rosenberg Self-Esteem Scale is a ten-item self-report

scale that measures global self-esteem. Ten questions are asked on the scale, half of which are

phrased as positive statements and the other half phrased as negative statements of self.

Responses are based on a four point Likert scale ranging from Strongly Agree to Strongly

Disagree. The Rosenberg Self-Esteem Scale scores range from 0 (lowest possible self-esteem)

to 30 (highest possible self-esteem) (Rosenberg, 1965). An overall score of self-esteem was

calculated by averaging participant’s scores across items. Self-esteem scores ranged from 4 to

30, with a mean score of 20.05 and a standard deviation of 4.94.

Self-objectification questionnaire. The Self-Objectification Questionnaire (Noll &

Fredrickson, 1998) consists of ten rank ordered items. Each item identifies a different body

attribute and participants rank them from 1 (least important) to 10 (most important) with respect

to their physical self-perception. Five of the items are competence-based (strength, physical

coordination, energy level, health and physical fitness) and five are appearance-based (weight,

sex appeal, physical attractiveness, firm/sculpted muscles and body measurements). Scores

ranged from -25 to 25, with higher scores indicating higher levels of self-objectification. Self-

Objectification Questionnaire scores ranged from -25 to 25, with a mean of 3.16 and a standard

deviation of 12.79.

Objectification of others. Objectification of other women was measured using a

modified version of the Self-Objectification Questionnaire (Noll & Fredrickson, 1998),

consistent with a study conducted by Strelan and Hargreaves (2005). The instructions for the

questionnaire read “This section is concerned with how women think about other women’s

70
bodies. Listed below are ten different body attributes. When you think about, or look at other

women, which of these body attributes are most important? Please rank the attributes in order

from 1 (least important) to 10 (most important) in other women.” The participants were

presented with the same ten attributes listed on the Self-Objectification Questionnaire (Noll &

Fredrickson, 1998) and the modified version of the scale was scored in the same way, with

scores ranging from -25 to 25. Higher scores indicated higher levels of objectification. Other-

Objectification Questionnaire scores ranged from -25 to 25, with a mean of 9.32 and a standard

deviation of 13.41. See Table 4.2 for survey results. It is interesting to note that the mean score

for objectification of other women was significantly higher than the mean score for self-

objectification, (F(1, 409) = 69.04, p< .001).

71
Table 4.2

Descriptive Statistics of Measures


Measure N Mean SD Range Min Max

Self 410 3.16 12.79 50.00 -25.00 25.00


Other 410 9.32 13.41 50.00 -25.00 25.00
Rosenberg 410 20.05 4.94 26.00 4.00 30.00

72
Statistical Tests of Research Questions

Analysis of Variance (ANOVA) was used to determine if the self-compassion induction

and control groups differed significantly in mean levels of both self- and other-objectification.

Given the results found by Tylka and Sabik (2010), multiple regression was used to determine

whether the self- or other-objectification variables contributed significant unique proportions of

variability beyond that accounted for by the scores from the Rosenberg Self-Esteem Scale. All

analyses were run using SPSS for Windows 18.0.

Research Question One

Research question one sought to identify how a self-compassion induction affects self-

objectification of women. ANOVA was used to assess this effect and results concluded that the

effect of the group membership (induction vs. control) was not statistically significant (p=.572).

See Table 4.3 for details.

73
Table 4.3

Tests of Between-Subjects Effects for Self-Objectification Scores (N = 410).

Type III Sum


Source of Squares df Mean Square F Sig

Corrected Model 52.445a 1 52.445 .320 .572

Intercept 4001.089 1 4001.089 24.400 .000

Group 52.445 1 52.445 .320 .572

Error 66902.250 408 163.976

Total 71045.000 410

Corrected Total 66954.695 409

a. R Squared = .001 (Adjusted R Squared = -.002)

74
Research Question Two

Research question two sought to identify how a self-compassion induction affects how

women objectify other women. ANOVA was used to assess this effect and results concluded

that the effect of the group membership (induction vs. control) was not statistically significant

(p=.478). See Table 4.4 for details.

75
Table 4.4

Tests of Between-Subjects Effects for Other-Objectification Scores (N = 410).

Type III Sum


Source of Squares df Mean Square F Sig

Corrected Model 90.856a 1 90.856 .504 .478

Intercept 35138.700 1 35138.700 195.109 .000

Group 90.856 1 90.856 .504 .478

Error 73479.924 408 180.098

Total 109162.000 410

Corrected Total 73570.780 409

a. R Squared = .001 (Adjusted R Squared = -.001)

76
Unique Contribution of Self-Compassion

The results of Tylka and Sabik’s (2010) study indicated the need to find the unique

contribution of the self-compassion induction beyond what was accounted for by self-esteem.

Multiple regression was used to determine whether the self- or other-objectification variables

contributed significant unique proportions of variability beyond that accounted for by the scores

from the Rosenberg Self-Esteem Scale. Scores from the Rosenberg Self-Esteem Scale were

entered into Step 1 and group membership was entered into Step 2. Results concluded that the

Self-compassion induction did not account for a significant proportion of variability in scores for

self-objectification beyond that already accounted for by self-esteem (R2 Change < .001, F(1,

407) = .03, p=.86). See Table 4.5 for details. Results also concluded that the Self-compassion

induction did not account for a significant proportion of variability in scores for other-

objectification beyond that already accounted for by self-esteem (R2 Change = .001, F(1, 407) =

.29, p=.59). See Table 4.6 for details.

77
Table 4.5

Summary of Regression Analysis for Variables Predicting Self-Objectification (N = 410).

Independent df F R2 Change p

Variables

Step 1 Self-Esteem 408 21.168 .049 .000

Step 2 Group 407 .029 .000 .864

Note. * = p< 0.05

78
Table 4.6

Summary of Regression Analysis for Variables Predicting Other-Objectification (N = 410).

Independent df F R2 Change p

Variables

Step 1 Self-Esteem 408 3.915 .010 .049

Step 2 Group 407 .290 .001 .590

Note. * = p< 0.05

79
Post-Hoc Analyses

Previous research has focused on the differences between college students who

participate in research early in the semester versus late in the semester. Early semester

participants have been found to be more socially responsible (Holden & Reddon, 1987), more

intrinsically motivated (Hom, 1987), more compliant (Masling, O'Neill, & Jayne, 1981), more

academically and achievement oriented with higher ACT and GPA scores, and to possess a more

internal academic locus of control (Evans & Donnerstein, 1974). Given these differences in

motivation and personality attributes, a post-hoc analysis combining data from early semester

participants from Radford University and the sample from Virginia Tech, because they were not

participating for credit, was conducted. The control group had 89 participants and the self-

compassion induction had 83 participants. The total number of participants in this post-hoc

analysis was 172. See Table 4.7 for sample demographics of the post-hoc sample.

80
Table 4.7

Demographic Information of Post-Hoc Participants

Variable N Percentage
Age
18 73 42.4
19 46 26.7
20 24 14.0
21 16 9.3
22 8 4.7
23 and older 5 2.9

Ethnicity
Caucasian 144 83.7
African American 12 7.0
Asian 5 2.9
Native American/ 2 1.2
American Indian
Pacific Islander 1 0.6
Multi-Ethnic 4 2.3
Other 4 2.3

Year in School
Freshman 101 58.7
Sophomore 29 16.9
Junior 23 13.4
Senior 19 11.0

Body Mass Index (BMI)


Underweight 9 5.2
Normal Weight 123 71.5
Overweight 28 16.3
Obese 12 7.0

Eating Disorder History


Eating Disorder Diagnosis 4 2.3
No Diagnosis 168 97.7

81
Research Question One

Research question one sought to identify how a self-compassion induction affects self-

objectification of women. ANOVA was used to assess this effect and results concluded that the

effect of the group membership (induction vs. control) was not significant (p=.145). See Table

4.8 for details.

82
Table 4.8

Post-Hoc Tests of Between-Subjects Effects for Self-Objectification Scores (N = 172).

Type III Sum


Source of Squares df Mean Square F Sig

Corrected Model 335.877a 1 335.877 2.149 .145

Intercept 630.877 1 630.877 4.036 .046

Group 335.877 1 335.877 2.149 .145

Error 26571.914 170 156.305

Total 27572.000 172

Corrected Total 26907.791 171

a. R Squared = .012 (Adjusted R Squared = .007)

83
Research Question Two

Research question two sought to identify how a self-compassion induction affects how

women objectify other women. ANOVA was used to assess this effect and results concluded

that the effect of the group membership (induction vs. control) was not significant (p= .461). See

Table 4.9 for details.

84
Table 4.9

Post-Hoc Tests of Between-Subjects Effects for Self-Objectification Scores (N = 172).

Type III Sum


Source of Squares df Mean Square F Sig

Corrected Model 105.906a 1 105.906 .547 .461

Intercept 14409.975 1 14409.975 74.395 .000

Group 105.906 1 105.906 .547 .461

Error 32928.141 170 193.695

Total 47548.000 172

Corrected Total 33034.047 171

a. R Squared = .003 (Adjusted R Squared = -.003)

85
Self-Esteem

A post-hoc analysis found significant differences among self-esteem scores between the

self-compassion induction and control groups (p = .031, R2 Change = .027). Self-esteem scores

for the self-compassion induction group were significantly different from and higher than the

self-esteem scores for the control group.

Unique Contribution of Self-Compassion

The results of Tylka and Sabik’s (2010) study indicated the need to find the unique

contribution of the self-compassion induction beyond what was accounted for by self-esteem.

Scores from the Rosenberg Self-Esteem Scale were entered into step 1 and group membership

was entered into step 2. Results concluded that the self-compassion induction did not account

for a significant proportion of variability in scores for self-objectification beyond that already

accounted for by self-esteem (R2 Change = .005, F(1, 169) = .94, p=.33). See Table 4.10 for

details. The self-compassion induction also did not account for a significant proportion of

variability in scores for other-objectification beyond that already accounted for by self-esteem

(R2 Change = .002, F(1, 169) = .34, p = .56). See Table 4.11 for details.

86
Table 4.10

Post-Hoc Summary of Regression Analysis for Variables Predicting Self-Objectification (N = 172).

Independent df F R2 Change p
Variables

Step 1 Self-Esteem 170 11.029 .061 .001

Step 2 Group 169 .943 .005 .333

Note. * = p< 0.05

87
Table 4.11

Post-Hoc Summary of Regression Analysis for Variables Predicting Other-Objectification (N = 172).

Independent df F R2 Change p

Variables

Step 1 Self-Esteem 170 .957 .006 .329

Step 2 Group 169 .334 .002 .558

Note. * = p< 0.05

88
Conclusion

This chapter presented the sample demographics and documented the survey results for

the Self-Objectification Questionnaire (Noll & Fredrickson, 1998), a modified version of this

questionnaire that addressed objectification of other women and the Rosenberg Self-Esteem

Scale (Rosenberg, 1965). This chapter then reported the tests of a priori research questions of

the effects of a self-compassion induction on both self- and other-objectification. There were no

significant findings for the two research questions. The chapter concluded with post-hoc

analyses, in which there was no significant effect for either research question; however, there

was a significant difference in self-esteem scores between the two groups.

89
CHAPTER V

DISCUSSION

This chapter provides the discussion and conclusion for this research study. The purpose

of this chapter is to relate the findings of this research study to the research findings in the

current literature and to provide a direction for future research. This chapter begins with a

summary and discussion of this study and ends with conclusions that may be drawn from the

research.

Research Summary

The purpose of this research study was to examine the impact of a self-compassion

induction on the self-objectification of women and how women objectify other women. Sexual

objectification has been found to be harmful to women’s psychosocial health, as this form of

oppression leads to increased body surveillance, body shame and decreased self-esteem. The

concept of self-compassion (Neff, 2003a), and more specifically, the new concept of body or

physical self-compassion (Berry et al., 2010; Magnus et al., 2010, respectively), was found to

mitigate this objectification. Szymanski and Carr (2011) called for social justice initiatives

surrounding empowerment of female clients and Szymanksi et al. (2011) suggested the need for

further research about women who objectify other women.

Utilizing an experimental design, undergraduate women from Radford University and

Virginia Tech completed a writing prompt related to a negative body image experience. The

participants were then randomly assigned to either a self-compassion induction or a control

group. The self-compassion induction group responded to three writing prompts meant to

mimic the three components of self-compassion (Neff, 2003a), while the control group answered

90
three writing prompts related to college choice. All participants then completed the Self-

Objectification Scale (Noll & Fredrickson, 1998), a modified version of this scale to address

objectification of other women, the Rosenberg Self-Esteem Scale (Rosenberg, 1965) and a

demographics questionnaire developed by the researcher. Objectification theory (Fredrickson &

Roberts, 1997) provided the theoretical framework for this research.

This research study was directed by two research questions:

1. How does a self-compassion induction affect self-objectification?

2. How does a self-compassion induction affect objectification of other women?

Discussion of the Results

This section will address the findings for the two research questions, as well as the

findings for the unique contribution of self-compassion beyond what can be accounted for by

self-esteem. It will also address the findings from the post-hoc analyses and discuss why such

analyses were conducted.

Research Question One

Research question one sought to identify how a self-compassion induction would affect

self-objectification. It was hypothesized that women who completed a self-compassion

induction would show lower scores on the Self-Objectification Questionnaire, as compared to the

control group. This research study found no significant difference among self-objectification

scores between the self-compassion and control groups.

The recent research on self-compassion (Neff, 2003a; 2009, 2011) and the new concept

of body or physical self-compassion (Berry et al., 2010; Magnus et al., 2010, respectively) has

demonstrated that women with higher levels of self-compassion are kinder to themselves. It was

91
hypothesized that this kindness toward self can be generalized to kindness toward one’s body

and therefore, women with higher levels of self-compassion would be less likely to self-

objectify. It may be the case that this new concept of body or physical self-compassion (Berry

et al., 2010; Magnus et al., 2010, respectively) cannot be raised using a traditional self-

compassion induction. It may also be the case that the online data collection method, used by

the researcher, did not result in a true self-compassion induction for all of the participants, as

some of them may not have adequately thought about their responses in a self-compassionate

manner.

These findings impact how future research should be conducted on this research topic.

Future research should restrict this methodology to in-person data collection to ensure that

participants are spending adequate amounts of time on the self-compassion writing prompts.

Future research should also include a manipulation check to assess if self-compassion was

significantly different between the induction and control groups following the writing prompts.

Research Question Two

Research question two sought to identify how a self-compassion induction would affect

how women objectify other women. It was hypothesized that women who complete a self-

compassion induction would show lower scores in the modified version of the Self-

Objectification Questionnaire, as compared to the control group. This research study found that

the effect of group membership (self-compassion induction vs. control) was not significant with

respect to scores on the modified version of the Self-Objectification Questionnaire. However, it

was found that the mean score for objectification of other women was higher than the mean score

92
for self-objectification for the total sample. This meant that women were objectifying other

women more than they were self-objectifying.

A 2010 study by Tylka and Sabik found that women who are focused on their own bodies

are more attentive to the appearance of other women’s bodies. This body comparison may act as

a feedback loop to their own body, perpetuating heightened body surveillance and objectification

of other women’s bodies. Given the research on the positive outcomes of increasing one’s self-

compassion (Neff, 2003a; 2009, 2011), especially related to body self-kindness (Berry et al.,

2010; Magnus et al., 2010), it was hypothesized that a self-compassion induction would not only

lead to less self-objectification but also less objectification of other women. Similar to the

results from the first research question, it may be that this new concept of body or physical self-

compassion (Berry et al., 2010; Magnus et al., 2010, respectively) cannot be raised using a

traditional self-compassion induction or that the online data collection method did not actually

raise self-compassion. If the self-compassion induction did not significantly impact self-

objectification, it would naturally be the case that it would not significantly impact the

objectification of others.

The finding that women were objectifying other women more than they were self-

objectifying was consistent with the findings of Stelan and Hargreaves (2005). This may

indicate that women place more importance on the appearance of other women than they do on

their own appearance. Women are constantly bombarded with body image messages

emphasizing the thin-ideal (Waterhouse, 1997) and deflecting these messages onto others may

serve as a protective factor for women.

93
Similar to research question one, future research implications may be found for this

research question. Future research should restrict this methodology to in-person data collection

to ensure that participants are spending adequate amounts of time on the self-compassion writing

prompts, as one must increase self-compassion before we see compassion for other women.

Future research should also include a manipulation check to assess if self-compassion was

significantly different between the induction and control groups. Again, if women are not

experiencing higher levels of self-compassion, we know that they will not experience higher

levels of compassion for other women (Tylka & Sabik, 2010).

Unique Contribution of Self- Compassion

The results of Tylka and Sabik’s (2010) study indicated the need to find the unique

contribution of the self-compassion induction beyond what was accounted for by self-esteem. In

this research sample, no significant effect was found when controlling for self-esteem in self-

objectification or objectification of other women.

Similar to the findings above for the two research questions, we may hypothesize that this

new concept of body or physical self-compassion (Berry et al., 2010; Magnus et al., 2010,

respectively) cannot be raised using a traditional self-compassion induction or that the online

data collection method did not result in a true self-compassion induction for the participants. In

either case, self-compassion could not account for anything significant beyond what was already

accounted for by self-esteem.

Again, these findings impact how future research should be conducted on this research

topic. Future research should restrict this methodology to in-person data collection to ensure that

participants are spending adequate amounts of time on the self-compassion writing prompts.

94
Future research should also include a manipulation check to assess if self-compassion was

significantly different between the induction and control groups.

Post-Hoc Analyses

This research study was conducted at the end of the Fall 2011 semester and the beginning

of the Spring 2012 semester. It was hypothesized that there may be differences in motivation and

personality factors between the students who completed the study at the end of the semester

versus the beginning of the semester. The literature points to differences which include: early

semester participants have been found to be more socially responsible (Holden &Reddon, 1987),

more intrinsically motivated (Hom, 1987), more compliant (Masling, O'Neill, & Jayne, 1981),

more academically and achievement oriented with higher ACT and GPA scores, and a more

internal academic locus of control (Evans &Donnerstein, 1974).

Because the students completing the study from the large state university did not have the

same class or extra credit motivation for completing the research as the students from mid-sized

state university, the large state university participants remained in the post-hoc sample. The

control group had 89 participants and the self-compassion induction had 83 participants. The

total number of participants in this post-hoc analysis was 172.

The same analyses for research questions one and two were run using this new sample.

Again, no significant differences were found between the self-compassion and control groups,

similar to the results using the full research sample. It is believed that the same reasons, as

previously described, can account for these findings.

Interestingly, a post-hoc analysis found significant differences among self-esteem scores

between the self-compassion induction and control groups. This meant that self-esteem scores

95
for the self-compassion induction group were significantly different and higher than the self-

esteem scores for the control group. Results were inconsistent with Neff’s (2004) suggestion

that because self-compassion does not require people to adopt an unrealistic view of the self, it

should be easier to raise than self-esteem. Given the emphasis that society has placed on raising

self-esteem (Twenge et al., 2008), it may have been the case that the college student sample was

primed to increase self-esteem more than they were self-compassion. It may also have been the

case that this research sample had higher levels of self-esteem prior to participation in this

research study, consistent with the personality and motivation factors previously discussed

(Evans & Donnerstein, 1974; Holden & Reddon, 1987; Hom, 1987; Masling, O'Neill, & Jayne,

1981). The initial writing prompt, which asked participants to write about a negative body image

experience, may have temporarily lowered self-esteem. Those women who participated in the

self-compassion induction may then have counterbalanced that lowering of self-esteem,

returning them to their normal level of self-esteem. Those women who participated in the

control group may not have returned to their normal levels of self-esteem, showing a significant

difference in levels of self-esteem between the two groups.

Limitations and Future Research

There were several major limitations to this research. First, while women from two

universities were represented in this study, the universities are still in the same geographic area

and the sample was comprised of only undergraduate women. Therefore, this study design did

not allow for an accurate representation of all women within the sampled age groups. Future

research should include research participants in a variety of urban and rural populations.

96
Another major limitation was that this study did not include a manipulation check

meaning that self-compassion was not measured following the self-compassion induction to see

if the induction actually increased self-compassion. The researcher chose to not include a self-

compassion questionnaire because of concern for the time requirements of participation in this

research. Future research should include either a pre- and post-induction evaluation of self-

compassion or at least measure the difference among self-compassion scores between the self-

compassion induction and the control group, regardless of the amount of time required to

complete the extra questionnaire.

Finally, it was hypothesized that the methodology used for data collection was not

adequate to induce self-compassion among all group participants. Due to the nature of online

data collection, some of the research participants in the self-compassion induction group may not

have spent enough time thinking about and then writing about the three components of self-

compassion (Neff, 2003a). This may have prevented a true self-compassion induction from

taking place. Future research should restrict this methodology to in-person data collection to

ensure that participants are spending adequate amounts of time on the self-compassion writing

prompts.

Conclusion

Sexual objectification is harmful to women’s psychosocial health. It leads to increased

body surveillance, body shame, and decreased self-esteem. For centuries, women’s worth has

been linked with physical attractiveness. Sexual objectification perpetuates social injustice,

depriving women of opportunities to feel comfortable in their bodies and to experience life

without the added strain of needing to monitor or enhance physical appearance. Szymanski and

97
Carr (2011) called for social justice initiatives surrounding empowerment of female clients and

Szymanksi et al. (2011) suggested the need for further research about women who objectify

other women.

Recent critics of self-esteem (Baumeister et al., 1996; Baumeister et al., 2003; Crocker et

al., 2003; Harter, 1999; Twenge et al., 2008) have opened the doors to a new concept of a

healthy attitude and relationship to oneself in the construct of self-compassion (Neff, 2003a).

Given the recent research on self-compassion (Neff, 2003a; 2009, 2011) and the new concept of

body or physical self-compassion (Berry et al., 2010; Magnus et al., 2010, respectively), one can

hypothesize that women with higher self-compassion will be less likely to self-objectify and

objectify other women. We also know from the research that self-compassion can be

experimentally induced (Leary & Adams, 2007; Leary at al., 2007; Zabelina& Robinson, 2010).

Research has establish the advantages of self-compassion to include greater life-

satisfaction, emotional intelligence, social connectedness, learning goals, wisdom, personal

initiative, curiosity, happiness, optimism, and positive affect, as well as less self-criticism,

depression, anxiety, fear of failure, thought suppression, perfectionism, performance goals, and

disordered eating behaviors (Neff, 2009, for a review). Using body self-compassion, Berry et al.

(2010) used an empirical phenomenological approach to interview five women and found themes

that suggested that as women experienced more body self-compassion, they appreciated their

unique body, took ownership of their body and engaged in less social comparison. While these

findings suggest the positive influence of increased body self-compassion, the literature has not

extended a body self-compassion induction on women who experience self-objectification and

women who objectify other women.

98
The purpose of this research study was to examine the impact of a self-compassion

induction on the self-objectification of women and how women objectify other women. This

research study found that there was no significant difference between the self-compassion and

control groups with respect to self-objectification or objectification of other women. Post-hoc

analyses found similar findings with respect to the two research questions, but the smaller

research sample had significant differences among self-esteem scores between the self-

compassion induction and control groups.

This chapter discussed the two research questions, with an explanation of the research

findings. Post-hoc analyses were then discussed. Limitations of this research study were

identified and suggestions for future research were examined.

99
References

Adams, C. E., & Leary, M. R. (2007). Promoting self-compassionate attitudes toward

eating among restrictive and guilty eaters. Journal of Social and Clinical

Psychology, 26(10), 1120-1144. doi:10.1521/jscp.2007.26.10.1120

American Psychological Association (2007). Guidelines for psychological practice with

girls and women. American Psychologist, 62, 949-979.

Archibald, A. B., Graber, J. A., & Brooks-Gunn, J. (1999). Associations among parent-

adolescent relationships, pubertal growth, dieting, and body image in young

adolescent girls: A short-term longitudinal study. Journal of Research on

Adolescence, 9(4), 395-415. doi:10.1207/s15327795jra0904_2

Bandura, A. (1977). Social learning theory. New Jersey: Prentice-Hall, Inc.

Baumeister, R. F., Campbell, J. D., Krueger, J. I., &Vohs, K. D. (2003). Does high

self-esteem cause better performance, interpersonal success, happiness, or

healthier lifestyles? Psychological Science in the Public Interest,4(1), 1–44.

doi:10.1111/1529-1006.01431

Baumeister, R. F., Smart, L., &Boden, J. M. (1996). Relation of threatened egotism to

violence and aggression: The dark side of high self-esteem. Psychological

Review, 103(1), 5-33. doi:10.1037/0033295X.103.1.5

Bearman, S., Korobov, N., & Thorne, A. (2009). The fabric of internalized sexism.

Journal of Integrated Social Sciences, 1, 10-47.

100
Becker, A. (1994). Nurturing and Negligence: Working on Other’s Bodies in Fiji. In

T. Csordas (Ed.), Embodiment and experience. (pp. 100-115). Cambridge: Cambridge

University Press.

Becker, A. (2004). Television, disordered eating, and young women in Fiji: Negotiating

body image and identity during rapid social change. Culture, Medicine and

Psychiatry 28, 533-559. doi:10.1007/s11013-004-1067-5

Benedikt, R., Wertheim, E. H., & Love, A. (1998). Eating attitudes and weight-loss

attempts in female adolescents and their mothers. Journal of Youth and

Adolescence, 27(1), 43-57. doi:10.1023/A:1022876715005

Berry, K., Kowalski, K. C., Ferguson, L. J., & McHugh, T. F. (2010). An empirical

phenomenology of younbg adult women exercisers’ body self-compassion. Qualitative

Research in Sport and Exercise, 2(3), 293-312. doi:10.1080/19398441.2010.517035

Birch, L. L., Fisher, J. O., Grimm-Thomas, K., Markey, C. N., Sawyer, R., & Johnson,

S. L. (2001). Confirmatory factor analysis of the Child Feeding Questionnaire:

A measure of parental attitudes, beliefs and practices about child feeding and

obesity proneness. Appetite, 36, 201-210. doi:10.1006/appe.2001.0398

Bishop, S. R., Lau, M., Shapiro, S., Carlson, L., Anderson, N. D., Carmody, J., et al. (2004).

Mindfulness: A proposed operational definition. Clinical Psychology: Science and

Practice, 11, 191-206. doi:10.1093/clipsy.bph077

Block, K. & Block, J. H. (1980). The role of ego-control and ego-resiliency in the

organization of behavior. In W. A. Collins (Ed.), Minnesota symposia on child

psychology (pp. 39-101). New York: Erlbaum.

101
Blythe, D. A., Simmon, R. G., &Zakin, D. F. (1985). Satisfaction with body image

for early adolescent females: The impact of pubertal timing within different

school environments. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 14, 207-223.

Bowen, M. (1978). Family theory in clinical practice. New York: Jason Aronson, Inc.

Brown, B. (1999). Soul without shame: A guide to liberating yourself from the judge

within. Boston: Shambala.

Bulik, C. M., Wade, T. D., Heath, A. C., Martin, N. G., Stunkard, A. J., Eaves, L. J.

(2001). Relating body mass index to figural stimuli: Population based normative

data for Caucasians. International Journal of Obesity Related Metabolic

Disorders 25(10), 1517-1524. doi:10.1038/sj.ijo.0801742

Calogero, R. M., &Jost, J. T. (2011). Self-subjugation among women: Exposure to

sexist ideology, self-objectification, and the protective function of the need to

avoid closure. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 100(2), 211-228.

doi: 10.1037/a0021864

Cash, T. F. & Henry, P. E. (1995). Women’s body images: The results of a national

survey in the U.S.A. Sex Roles, 33(1/2), 19-28. doi:10.1007/BF01547933

Cash, T. F., Winstead, B. A., &Janda, J. H. (1986). Body image survey report: The

great American shape-up. Psychology Today, April, 30-37.

Center for Disease Control and Prevention (2011). Healthy weight- it’s not a diet, it’s a

lifestyle! Retrieved from

http://www.cdc.gov/healthyweight/assessing/bmi/adult_bmi/index.html

102
Childress, A. C., Brewerton, T. D., Hodges, E. L., & Jarrell, M. P. (1993). The Kids’

Eating Disorder Survey (KEDS): A study of middle school students. Journal of

the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 32, 843-850.

doi:10.1097/00004583-199307000-00021

Chodorow, N. J. (1989). Feminism and psychoanalytic theory. New Haven:

Yale University Press.

Citrin, L. B., Roberts, T. A., & Fredrickson, B. L. (2004) Objectification theory and

emotion: A feminist psychological perspective on gendered affect. In L. Z.

Tiedens& C. W. Leach (Eds.) The social life of emotions (pp. 203-223).

Cambridge, United Kingdom: Cambridge University Press.

Clarke, L. H., & Griffin, M. (2007). Becoming and being gendered through the body:

Older women, their mothers and body image. Ageing & Society, 27, 701-718.

doi:10.1017/S0144686X0700623X

Collins, M. E. (1991). Body figure perceptions and preferences among preadolescent

children. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 10, 199–208.

doi:10.1002/1098-108X(199103)10:2<199::AID-EAT2260100209>3.0.CO;2-D

Cooley, E., Toray, T. (2001). Disordered eating in college freshman women: A

prospective study. Journal of American College Health, 49, 229-235.

doi:10.1080/07448480109596308

Cooley, E., Toray, T., Wang, M. C., & Valdez, N. N. (2008). Maternal effects on

daughters’ eating pathology and body image. Eating Behaviors, 9, 52-61.

doi:10.1016/j.eatbeh.2007.03.001

103
Cooper, P., Taylor, M., Cooper, Z., & Fairburn, C. (1987). The development and

validation of the Body Shape Questionnaire. International Journal of Eating

Disorders, 6, 485-494. doi:10.1002/1098-108X(198707)6:4<485::AID-

EAT2260060405>3.0.CO;2-O

Costanzo, P. R. (1992) External socialization and the development of adaptive

individuation and social connection. In D. N. Ruble, P.R. Costanzo, & M. E.

Oliveri (Eds.) The social psychology of mental health (pp. 55-80). New York:

Guilford.

Crocker, J., Luhtanen, R. K., Cooper, M. L., &Bouvrette, A. (2003). Contingencies of

selfworth in college students: Theory and measurement. Journal of Personality

and Social Psychology, 85, 894–908. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.85.5.894

Crocker, J., & Wolfe, C. T. (2001). Contingencies of self-worth. Psychological

Review, 108, 593-623. doi:10.1037//0033-295X.108.3.593

Davison, K. K., Markey, C. N., & Birch, L. L. (2000). Etiology of body

dissatisfaction and weight among 5-year-old girls. Appetite 35(2), 143-151.

doi:10.1006/appe.2000.0349

Demarest, J. & Allen, R. (2000). Body image: Gender, ethnic, and age differences. The

Journal of Social Psychology, 140(4), 465-472. doi:10.1080/00224540009600485

Diener, E., Emmons, R., Larsen, J., & Griffin, S. (1985). The satisfaction with life scale.

Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 11, 89-97.

doi:10.1207/s15327752jpa4901_13

104
Dion, K. L., Dion, K. K., &Keelan, J. P. (1990). Appearance anxiety as a dimension

of social-evaluative anxiety: Exploring the ugly duckling syndrome. Contemporary

Social Psychology, 14, 220–225.

Engel, M. (n.d.) BMI formula. Retrieved November 2, 2008, from

http://www.epic4health.com/bmiformula.html.

Erikson, E. H. (1968). Identity: Youth and crisis. New York: Norton.

Evans , R. Donnerstein , E. (1974). Some implications for psychological research of

early versus late term participation by college students. Journal of Research in

Personality, 8, 102-109.

Fallon, A. E., &Rozin, P. (1985). Sex differences in perception of desirable shape.

Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 94, 102-105. doi:10.1037//0021-843X.94.1.102

Fenigstein, A., Scheier, M. F. & Buss, A. H. (1975). Public and private self-

consciousness: Assessment and theory. Journal of Consulting and Clinical

Psychology, 43, 522-527. doi:10.1037/h0076760

Festinger, L. (1954). A theory of social comparison processes. Human Relations, 7,

117-140. doi:10.1177/001872675400700202

Fisher, E. & Thompson, J. K. (1998). Social comparison and body image: An

investigation of body comparison processes using multidimensional scaling.

Unpublished manuscript, University of South Florida.

105
Forbes, G. B., Jobe, R. L., & Richardson, R. N. (2006). Associations between having

a boyfriend and the body satisfaction and self-esteem of college women: An

extension of the Lin and Kulik Hypothesis. Journal of Social Psychology,

146(3), 381-384. doi:10.3200/SOCP.146.3.381-384

Fraenkel, J. R. &Wallen, N. E., (2009). How to design and evaluate research in

education. New York: McGraw-Hill.

Francis, L. A. & Birch, L. L. (2005). Maternal influences on daughters’ restrained

eating behavior. Health Psychology, 24(6), 548-554. doi:10.1037/0278- 6133.24.6.548

Frederick, D. A., Peplau, L. A., & Lever, J. (2006). The swimsuit issue: Correlates of

body image in a sample of 52,677 heterosexual adults. Body Image, 3, 413-419.

doi:10.1016/j.bodyim.2006.08.002

Fredrickson, B. L. & Roberts, T. A. (1997). Objectification theory: Towards women’s

lived experiences and mental health risks. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 21,

173-206. doi:10.1111/j.1471-6402.1997.tb00108.x

Fredrickson, B. L., Roberts, T. A., Noll, S. M. Quinn, D. M., &Twenge, J. M.

(1998). That swimsuit becomes you: Sex differences in self-objectification,

restrained eating, and math performance. Journal of Personality and Social

Psychology, 75,269-284. doi:10.1037//0022-3514.75.1.269

Garner, D. M. (1991). Eating Disorders Inventory- 2. Odessa, FL: Psychology

Assessment Resources.

106
Garner, D. M., &Garfinkel, P. E. (1979). The Eating Attitudes Test: An index of the

symptoms of anorexia nervosa. Psychological Medicine, 10, 647-656.

doi:10.1017/S0033291700030762

Garner, D. M., Garfinkel, P. E., Schwartz, D., & Thompson, M. (1980). Cultural

expectations of thinness in women. Psychological Reports, 47, 483-491.

doi:10.1017/S0033291700054945

Garner, D. M. & Olmsted, M. P. (1984). Manual for Eating Disorder Inventory (EDI).

Odessa, FL: Psychology Assessment Resources.

Garner, D. M., Olmsted, M. P., Bohr, Y., &Garfinkel, P. (1982). The Eating Attitudes

Test: Psychometric features and clinical correlates. Psychological Medicine, 12,

871-878. doi:10.1017/S0033291700049163

Greenleaf, C., &McGreer, R. (2006). Disordered eating attitudes and self-

objectification among physically active and sedentary female college students. The

Journal of Psychology, 2006, 140(3), 187–198. doi:10.3200/JRLP.140.3.187-198

Gilbert, P., & Procter, S. (2006). Compassionate mind training for people with high

shame and self-criticism: Overview and pilot study of a group therapy approach.

Clinical Psychology and Psychotherapy, 13, 353–379. doi:10.1002/cpp.507

Grogan S. (2008). Body Image: Understanding body dissatisfaction in men, women and

children (second edition). New York: Routledge.

107
Hahn-Smith, A. M. and Smith, J. E. (2001). The positive influence of maternal

identification on body image, eating attitudes, and self-esteem of Hispanic and

Anglo girls. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 29(4), 429-440.

doi:10.1002/eat.1039

Harper, B., &Tiggemann, M. (2008). The effect of thin ideal media images on women’s

self-objectification, mood, and body image. Sex Roles, 58, 649-657.

doi:10.1007/s11199-007-9379-x

Harter, S. (1999). The construction of the self: A developmental perspective. New York:

Guilford Press.

Hatcher, J. (2007). The state of measurement of self-esteem of African American

women. Journal of Transcultural Nursing, 18(3), 224-232.

doi:10.1177/1043659607301299

Heinberg, L. J. & Thompson, J. K. (1995). Body image and televised images of

thinness and attractiveness: A controlled laboratory investigation. Journal of

Social and Clinical Psychology, 14, 325-338. doi:10.1521/jscp.1995.14.4.325

Heinberg, L., Thompson, J., &Stormer, S. (1995). Development and validation of the

Sociocultural Attitudes Towards Appearance Questionnaire. International

Journal of Eating Disorders, 17, 81-89. doi:10.1002/1098- 108X(199501)17:1<81::AID-

EAT2260170111>3.0.CO;2-Y

Heatherton, T. F., &Polivy, J. (1991). Development and validation of a scale for

measuring state self-esteem. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 60,

895-910. doi:10.1037//0022-3514.60.6.895

108
Hill, M. S., & Fischer, A. R. (2008). Examining objectification theory: Lesbian and

heterosexual women’s experiences with sexual and self-objectification. The

Counseling Psychologist, 36, 745–776. doi:10.1177/0011000007301669

Holden , R. R. Reddon , R. R. (1987). Temporal personality variations among

participants from a university subject pool. Psychological Reports, 60, 1247-1254.

Hom , H. J. , Jr. (1987). A methodological note: time of participation effects on

intrinsic motivation. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 13, 210-215.

Howard, J. A. & Hollander, J. (1997). Gendered situations, gendered selves.

Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications, Inc.

Hutchinson, M. (1982). Transforming body image. Women and Therapy, 1, 59-67.

Jhally, S. (2001). Killing us softly 3: Advertising’s image of women. Northampton,

MA: Media Education Foundation.

Kernis, M. (2005). Measuring self-esteem in context: The importance of stability of

self-esteem in psychological functioning. Journal of Personality, 73, 1–37.

doi:10.1111/j.1467-6494.2005.00359.x

Kerr, M. E. & Bowen, M. (1988). Family evaluation: An approach based on Bowen

theory. New York: W.W. Norton & Company.

109
Killen, J. D., Taylor, C. B., Hayward, C., Wilson, D. M., Haydel, F., Hammer L. D.,

Simmonds, B., Robinson, T. N., Litt, I., Varady, A., & Kraemer, H. (1994).

Pursuit of thinness and onset of eating disorder symptoms in a community sample

of adolescent girls: A three-year prospective analysis. International Journal of

Eating Disorders, 16(3), 227-238. doi:10.1002/1098-

108X(199411)16:3<227::AID-EAT2260160303>3.0.CO;2-L

Killen, J. D., Taylor, C. B., Hayward, C., Haydel, Wilson, D. M., F., Hammer L. D.,

Kraemer, H., Blair-Greiner, A., Strachowski, D. (1996). Weight concerns

influence the development of eating disorders: A 4-year prospective study.

Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 6, 936-940. doi:10.1037/0022-

006X.64.5.936

Klemchuk, H. P., Hutchinson, C. B., & Frank, R. I. (1990). Body dissatisfaction and

eating-related problems on the college campus: Usefulness of the Eating Disorder

Inventory with a nonclinical population. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 37,

297-305. doi:10.1037//0022-0167.37.3.297

Leary, M. R., Tate, E. B., Adams, C. E., Allen, A. B., & Hancock, J. (2007). Self–

compassion and reactions to unpleasant self–relevant events: The implications of

treating oneself kindly. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 92, 887–

904. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.92.5.887

Liechty, T., Freeman, P. A., &Zabriskie, R. B. (2006). Body image and beliefs about

appearance: Constraints on the leisure of college-age and middle-age women.

Leisure Sciences, 28, 311-330. doi:10.1080/01490400600745845

110
Magnus, C. M. R, Kowalski, K. C., & McHugh, T. F. (2010). The role of self-

compassion in women's self-determined motives to exercise and exercise-related

outcomes. Self and Identity 9(4), 363-382. doi:10.1080/15298860903135073

Maloney, M., J., McGuire, J., Daniels, S. R. (1988). Reliability testing of a children’s

version of the Eating Attitudes Test. Journal of the American Academy of Child

and Adolescent Psychiatry, 27, 541-543. doi:10.1097/00004583-198809000-00004

Maloney, M., J., McGuire, J., Daniels, S. R., &Specker, B. (1989). Dieting behavior

and eating attitudes in children. Pediatrics, 84, 482-487.

Marshall W. A., Tanner J. M. (1969) Variations in pattern of pubertal changes in girls.

Archives of Disease in Childhood, 44, 291–303. doi:10.1136/adc.44.235.291

Masling , J.,O'Neill , R. Jayne , C. (1981). Orality and latency of volunteering to serve

as experimental subjects. Journal of Personality Assessment, 45, 20-22.

McKinley, N. M. & Hyde, J. S. 1996). The Objectified Body Consciousness Scale:

Development and validation. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 20, 181-215.

doi:10.1111/j.1471-6402.1996.tb00467.x

Media. (2009). Dictionary.com. Lexico Publishing Group. Retrieved March 2, 2009,

from http://dictionary.reference.com/browse/media

Mendelson, B. K., & White, D. R. (1982). Relation between body-esteem and self-

esteem of obese and normal children. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 54, 899-905.

Mercurio, A. E.&Landrey, L. J. (2008). Self-objectification and well-being: The impact

of self-objectification on women’s overall sense of self-worth and life

satisfaction. Sex Roles, 58, 458-466. doi:10.1007/s11199-007-9357-3

111
Mills, J. S., Polivy, J., Herman, C. P., &Tiggemann, M. (2002). Effects of exposure to

thin media images: Evidence of self-enhancement among restrained eaters.

Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 28, 1687-1699.

doi:10.1177/014616702237650

Moradi, B., & Huang, Y., (2008). Objectification theory and psychology of women: A

decade of advances and future directions. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 32,

277-398. doi:10.1111/j.1471-6402.2008.00452.x

Neff, K. D. (2003a). Self-compassion: An alternative conceptualization of a healthy attitude

toward oneself. Self and Identity, 2, 85-101. doi:10.1080/15298860309032

Neff, K. D. (2003b). The development and validation of a scale to measure self-compassion.

Self and Identity, 2, 223-250. doi:10.1080/15298860309027

Neff, K. D. (2004). Self-compassion and psychological well-being. Constructivism in the

Human Sciences, 9, 27-37.

Neff, K. D. (in press). Self-compassion. In M. R. Leary & R. H. Hoyle (Eds.), Handbook of

Individual Differences in Social Behavior. Guilford Press.

Neff, K. D. (2008). Self-compassion: Moving beyond the pitfalls of a separate self-

concept. In J. Bauer & H. A. Wayment (Eds.) Transcending Self-Interest:

Psychological Explorations of the Quiet Ego (pp. 95 – 106). APA Books,

Washington DC. doi:10.1037/11771-009

Neff, K. D. (2011). Self-compassion, self-esteem, and well-being. Social and Personality

Compass, 5, 1-12. doi:10.1111/j.1751-9004.2010.00330.x

112
Neff, K. D., Hsieh, Y., &Dejitterat, K. (2005). Self-compassion, achievement goals, and

coping with academic failure. Self and Identity, 4, 263-287.

doi:10.1080/13576500444000317

Neff, K. D., Kirkpatrick, K., & Rude, S. S. (2007). Self-compassion and adaptive

psychological functioning. Journal of Research in Personality, 41, 139-154.

doi:10.1016/j.jrp.2006.03.004

Neff, K. D. &McGeehee, P. (2010). Self-compassion and psychological resilience among

adolescents and young adults. Self and Identity, 9, 225-240.

doi:10.1080/15298860902979307

Neff, K. D., Rude, S. S., & Kirkpatrick, K. (2007). An examination of self-compassion in

relation to positive psychological functioning and personality traits. Journal of Research

in Personality, 41, 908-916. doi:10.1016/j.jrp.2006.08.002

Neff, K. D., &Vonk, R. (2006). Self-compassion: A healthier alternative to high self-esteem.

Paper presented at the 36th annual meeting of the Jean Piaget Society, Baltimore.

Neff, K. D. &Vonk, R. (2009). Self-compassion versus global self-esteem: Two different ways

of relating to oneself. Journal of Personality, 77, 23-50. doi:10.1111/j.1467-

6494.2008.00537.x

Neumark-Sztainer, D., Bauer, K. W., Friend, S., Hannan, P. J., Story, M., & Berge, J. M.

(2010). Family weight talk and dieting: How much do they matter for body

dissatisfaction and disordered eating behaviors in adolescent girls? Journal of

Adolescent Health, 47, 270-276. doi:10.1016/j.jadohealth.2010.02.001

113
NHLBI Expert Panel. (1998). Classification of overweight and obesity. Retrieved

from http://www.sparrow.org/weightmanagement/NHLBIguidelines.pdf

Noll, S. M. & Fredrickson, B. L. (1998). A mediational model linking self-

objectification, body shame, and disordered eating. Psychology of Women

Quarterly, 22, 623-636. doi:10.1111/j.1471-6402.1998.tb00181.x

Park, J. & Beaudet, M. P. (2007). Eating attitudes and their correlates among Canadian

women concerned about their weight. European eating disorders review, 15,

311-320. doi:10.1002/erv.741

Pennebaker, J. W., Colder, M., & Sharp, L. K. (1990). Accelerating the coping process.

Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 58, 528–537. doi:10.1037//0022-

3514.58.3.528

Perera, K. (n.d.) What is self-esteem? Retrieved March 2, 2009, from

http://www.more-selfesteem.com/whatisselfesteem.htm

Petersen, A. C., Schulenberg, J. E., Abramowitz, R. H., Offer, D., & Jarcho, H. D.

(1984). A self-image questionnaire for young adolescents (SIQYA): Reliability

and validity studies. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 13, 93-111

doi:10.1007/BF02089104

Piers, E. V. (1984). Piers-Harris Children’s Self-Concept Scale: Revised manual. Los

Angeles: Western Psychological Services.

Reed, D. L., Thompson, J. K., Brannick, M. T., & Sacco, W. P. (1991). Development and

validation of the Physical Appearance State and Trait Anxiety Scale (PASTAS).

Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 5, 323-332. doi:10.1016/0887-6185(91)90032-O

114
Rogers, C. R. (1951). Client centered therapy. Boston: Houghton Mifflin.

Rolnik, A., Engeln-Maddox, R., & Miller, S.A. (2010). Here’s looking at you: Self-

objectification, body image disturbance, and sorority rush. Sex Roles. 63, 6-17.

doi:10.1007/s11199-010-9745-y

Rosenberg, M. (1965). Society and the adolescent self-image. Princeton, NJ: Princeton

University Press.

Sanford, L. T. & Donovan, M. E. (1985). Women & self-esteem: Understanding and

improving the way we think and feel about ourselves. Harrisonburg, Pennsylvania: R.R.

Donnelley & Sons Company.

Sanftner, J. L., Crowther, J. H., Crawford, P. A., & Watts, D. D. (1996). Maternal

influences (or lack thereof) on daughters’ eating attitudes and behaviors. Eating

Disorders: The Journal of Treatment and Prevention, 4(2), 147-159.

doi:10.1080/10640269608249182

Schulken, E. D., Pinciaro, P. J., Sawyer, R. G., Jensen, J. G., & Hoban, M. T.

(1997). Sorority women’s body size perceptions and their weight-related

attitudes and behaviors. Journal of American College Health, 46 (2), 69-

74. doi:10.1080/07448489709595590

Shisslak, C., Renger, R., Sharpe, T., Crago, M., McKnight, K., Gray, N., et al. (1999).

Development and evaluation of the McKnight Risk Factor Survey for assessing

potential risk and protective factors for disordered eating in preadolescent and

adolescent girls. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 25(2), 195-214.

doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1098-108X(199903)25:2<195::AID-EAT9>3.0.CO;2-B

115
Skinner, B. F. (1938). The behavior of organisms. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.

Skinner, B. F. (1974) About behaviorism. New York: Knopf.

Smith, A., Rainie, L., &Zickuhr (2011). College students and technology. Retrieved

from the Pew Internet and American Life Project.

http://pewinternet.org/Reports/2011/College-students-and-technology/Report.aspx

Spangler, D. L. &Stice, E. (2001). Validation of the beliefs about appearance scale.

Cognitive Therapy and Research, 25, 813-827. doi:10.1023/A:1012931709434

Strahan, E. J., Lafrance, A., Wilson, A. E., Ethier, N., Spencer, S. J., &Zanna, M. P.

(2008). Victoria’s dirty little secret: How sociocultural norms influence

adolescent girls and women. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 34(4),

288-301.

Stunkard, A. J., &Messick, S. (1985). The three-factor eating questionnaire to measure

dietary restraint, disinhibition and hunger. Journal of Psychosomatic Research,

29,71-83.

Stunkard, A. J., Sorenson, T., &Schlusinger, F. (1983). Use of the Danish adoption

register for the study of obesity and thinness. In S. Kety, L. P. Rowland, R. L.

Sidman, & S. W. Matthysse (Eds.), The genetics of neurological and psychiatric

disorders (pp. 115-120). New York: Raven Press.

Szymanski, D. M., & Carr, E. R. (2011). Underscoring the need for social justice

initiatives concerning the sexual objectification of women. The Counseling

Psychologist, 39(1), 164-170. doi: 10.1177/0011000010384512

116
Szymanski, D. M., Moffitt, L. B., & Carr, E. R. (2011). Sexual objectification of women:

Advances to theory and research. The Counseling Psychologist, 39(1), 6-38. doi:

10.1177/0011000010378402

Tantleff-Dunn, S., Thompson, J. K., & Dunn, M. E. (1995). The Feedback on Physical

Appearance Scale (FOPAS): Questionnaire development and psychometric

evaluation. Eating Disorders: The Journal of Treatment and Prevention, 3, 332-

341. doi:10.1080/10640269508250063

Thompson, M. A., & Gray, J. J. (1995). Development and validation of a new body-

image scale. Journal of Personality Assessment, 64, 258-269.

doi:10.1207/s15327752jpa6402_6

Tiggemann, M. & Lynch, J. E. (2001). Body image across the life span in adult women:

The role of self-objectification. Developmental Psychology, 37(2), 243-253.

doi:10.1037//0012-1649.37.2.243

Tiggemann, M., &Slater,A. (2001). A test of objectification theory in former dancers

and non-dancers. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 25, 57–63. doi:10.1111/1471-

6402.00007

Twenge, J. M., Konrath, S., Foster, J. D., Campbell, W. K., & Bushman, B. J. (2008).

Egos inflating over time: A cross-temporal meta-analysis of the narcissistic

personality inventory. Journal of Personality, 76(4), 875-902. doi:10.1111/j.1467-

6494.2008.00507.x

117
Tylka, T. L., &Sabik, N. J. (2010). Integrating Social Comparison Theory and self-

esteem within Objectification Theory to predict women’s disordered eating. Sex Roles,

63, 18-31. doi: 10.1007/s11199-010-9785-3

Usmiani, S. &Daniluk, J. (1997). Mothers and their adolescent daughters: Relationship

between self-esteem, gender role identity, and body image. Journal of Youth and

Adolescence 26(1), 45-62. doi:10.10232/A:1024588112108

Van Strien, T., Fritjers, J. E., Bergers, G. P. A., &Defares, P. B. (1986). The Dutch

Eating Behavior Questionnaire (DEBQ) for assessment of restrained, emotional,

and external eating behavior. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 5, 295-

315. doi: 10.1002/1098-108X(198602)

Waterhouse, D. (1997). Like mother, like daughter: How women are influenced by their

mothers’ relationship with food- and how to break the pattern.. New York:

Hyperion.

Watson, J. B. (1924). Behaviorism. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

Wertheim, E. H., Martin, G., Prior, M., Sanson, A., & Smart, D. (2002). Parent

influences in the transmission of eating and weight related values and behaviors.

Eating Disorders, 10, 321-331. doi: 10.1080/10640260214507

Westkott, M. (1986). The feminist legacy of Karen Horney. New Haven, CT: Yale

University Press.

Wylie, R. C. (1989). Measures of self-concept. Lincoln, NE: University of Nebraska

Press.

118
Zabelina, D. L., & Robinson, M. D. (2010). Don't be so hard on yourself: Self-

compassion facilitates creative originality among self-judgmental individuals.

Creativity Research Journal, 22(3),288-293. doi:10.1080/10400419.2010.

503538

Zion, L. C. (1965). Body concept as it relates to self-concept. Research Quarterly,

36(4), 490-495.

119
APPENDIX A

The Self-Objectification Questionnaire

We are interested in how people think about their bodies. The questions below identify 10
different body attributes. We would like you to rank order these body attributes from that which
has the greatest impact on your physical self-concept (rank this a "9"), to that which has the least
impact on your physical self-concept(rank this a "0").

Note: It does not matter how you describe yourself in terms of each attribute. For example,
fitness level can have a great impact on your physical self-concept regardless of whether you
consider yourself to be physically fit, not physically fit, or any level in between.

Please first consider all attributes simultaneously, and record your rank ordering by writing the
ranks in the rightmost column.

IMPORTANT: Do Not Assign The Same Rank To More Than One Attribute!

9 = greatest impact
8 = next greatest impact
When considering your physical self-concept . . . 1 = next to least impact
0 = least impact

1 . . . .what rank do you assign to physical coordination? _____


2. . . .what rank do you assign to health? _____
3. . . .what rank do you assign to weight? _____
4. . . .what rank do you assign to strength? _____
5. . . .what rank do you assign to sex appeal? _____
6. . . .what rank do you assign to physical attractiveness? _____
7. . . .what rank do you assign to energy level (e.g., stamina)? _____
8. . . .what rank do you assign to firm/sculpted muscles? _____
9. . . .what rank do you assign to physical fitness level? _____
10. . . .what rank do you assign to measurements (e.g., chest, waist, hips)? _____

In administering the measure, the title is not included. Scores are obtained by separately
summing the ranks for appearance-based items (3, 5, 6, 8 and 10) and competence-based items
(1, 2, 4, 7 and 9), and then subtracting the sum of competence ranks from the sum of appearance
ranks. Scores may range from -25to 25, with higher scores indicating a greater emphasis on
appearance, interpreted as higher trait self-objectification.

Copyright 1998 by Barbara L. Fredrickson. Individuals who wish to reprint all or part of the
Self-Objectification Questionnaire should contact Barbara L. Fredrickson.

120
Fredrickson, B. L., Roberts, T. A., Noll, S. M. Quinn, D. M., & Twenge, J. M. (1998). That
swimsuit becomes you: Sex differences in self-objectification, restrained eating, and math
performance. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 75, 269-284.

121
APPENDIX B

Objectification of Other Women

This section is concerned with how women think about other women’s bodies. Listed below are
ten different body attributes. When you think about, or look at other women, which of these
body attributes are most important? Please rank the attributes in order from 1 (least important)
to 10 (most important) in other women.

1 . . . .what rank do you assign to physical coordination? _____


2. . . .what rank do you assign to health? _____
3. . . .what rank do you assign to weight? _____
4. . . .what rank do you assign to strength? _____
5. . . .what rank do you assign to sex appeal? _____
6. . . .what rank do you assign to physical attractiveness? _____
7. . . .what rank do you assign to energy level (e.g., stamina)? _____
8. . . .what rank do you assign to firm/sculpted muscles? _____
9. . . .what rank do you assign to physical fitness level? _____
10. . . .what rank do you assign to measurements (e.g., chest, waist, hips)? _____

122
APPENDIX C

Rosenberg Self-Esteem Scale

Instructions: Below is a list of statements dealing with your general feelings about yourself. If
you strongly agree, circle SA. If you agree with the statement, circle A. If you disagree, circle
D. If you strongly disagree, circle SD.

1. On the whole, I am satisfied with myself. SA A D SD


2.* At times, I think I am no good at all. SA A D SD
3. I feel that I have a number of good qualities. SA A D SD
4. I am able to do things as well as most other people. SA A D SD
5.* I feel I do not have much to be proud of. SA A D SD
6.* I certainly feel useless at times. SA A D SD
7. I feel that I’m a person of worth, at least on an equal SA A D SD
plane with others.
8.* I wish I could have more respect for myself. SA A D SD
9. * All in all, I am inclined to feel that I am a failure. SA A D SD
10. I take a positive attitude toward myself. SA A D SD

Scoring: SA = 3, A = 2, D = 1, SD = 0. Items with an asterisk are reverse scored, that is, SA = 0,


A = 1, D = 2, SD = 3. Sum the scores for the 10 items. The higher the score, the higher one’s
self-esteem.

Rosenberg, M. (1965). Society and the adolescent self-image. Princeton, NJ: Princeton
University Press.

123
APPENDIX D

Demographics Questionnaire

What is your age? ____________________

What is your ethnicity? (please circle one)

White/Caucasian Native American/American Indian Black/African American

Pacific Islander Asian Multi-ethnic Other (please describe): ___________

What is your weight? __________________lbs

What is your height? _____feet _____ inches

What is your class year? (please circle one)

Freshman Sophomore Junior Senior

Have you ever been diagnosed with an eating disorder (anorexia or bulimia) by a mental health
professional or a physician? (please circle one)

Yes No

124
APPENDIX E

Informed Consent for Mid-Sized State University

You are being asked to participate in a study about objectification of women. Specifically, about
the way women self-objectify and objectify other women.

If you agree to participate, we ask that you complete all of the writing prompts and the survey
questions. All data will be presented anonymously in final form. Any information obtained in
connection with this study that can be linked to you will be kept confidential. The risks in
participating in this study are no greater than those experienced in everyday life. There are no
direct benefits to you from participating in this study other than reflecting on your self-
objectification and your objectification of other women.

Your decision whether to participate will not affect your future relations with Radford
University. If you decide to participate, you are free to discontinue participation at any time
without affecting such relationships.

If you have any questions about this study, please feel free to contact Alysia Hoover-Thompson
___[insert phone number]___ or Sarah Hastings ___[insert phone number]___.

This study was approved by the Radford University Committee for the Review of Human
Subjects Research. If you have questions or concerns about your rights as a research subject or
have complaints about this study, you should contact Dr. Dennis Grady, Dean, College of
Graduate and Professional Studies, Radford University,
dgrady4@radford.edu, 540-831-7163.

You will be offered a copy of this form to keep.

Thank You

You are making a decision whether to participate. Your signature indicates that you have read
the information provided above and have decided to participate. You may withdraw at any time
without prejudice after signing this form should you choose to discontinue participation in this
study.

_______________________________ ________________
Signature Date

_______________________________
Signature of Investigator

125
APPENDIX F

Informed Consent for Large State University

You are being asked to participate in a study about objectification of women. Specifically, about
the way women self-objectify and objectify other women.

If you agree to participate, we ask that you complete all of the writing prompts and the survey
questions. All data will be presented anonymously in final form. Any information obtained in
connection with this study that can be linked to you will be kept confidential. The risks in
participating in this study are no greater than those experienced in everyday life. By
participating in this study, you may reflect on your self-objectification and your objectification of
other women. You will also be entered into a drawing to receive one of two $25 Amazon.com
gift card.

Your decision whether to participate will not affect your future relations with Virginia Tech. If
you decide to participate, you are free to discontinue participation at any time without affecting
such relationships.

If you have any questions about this study, please feel free to contact Alysia Hoover-Thompson
___[insert phone number]___ or Sarah Hastings ___[insert phone number]___.

This study was approved by the Radford University Committee for the Review of Human
Subjects Research. If you have questions or concerns about your rights as a research subject or
have complaints about this study, you should contact Dr. Dennis Grady, Dean, College of
Graduate and Professional Studies, Radford University,
dgrady4@radford.edu, 540-831-7163.

You will be offered a copy of this form to keep.

Thank You

You are making a decision whether to participate. Your signature indicates that you have read
the information provided above and have decided to participate. You may withdraw at any time
without prejudice after signing this form should you choose to discontinue participation in this
study.

_______________________________ ________________
Signature Date

_______________________________
Signature of Investigator

126
APPENDIX G

Writing Prompt for All Participants

We would like you to think about a negative event that you experienced in high school or college
that made you feel badly about your body – something that involved failure, humiliation or
rejection. Please describe the event including details regarding what led to the event, who was
present, what happened during the event and how you felt and behaved at the time.

127
APPENDIX H

Self-Compassion Induction Writing Prompts

1. Do you know other women who experienced a similar event? What was their experience
like?

2. If your best friend came to you having experienced this event, what would you say to
comfort her?

3. Now we would like for you to rewrite the story of your event in an objective and
unemotional way.

128
APPENDIX I

Control Group Writing Prompts

1. We would like you to describe the factors that lead to your choice of college.

2. We would like you to describe your favorite class in high school or college.

3. We would like you to describe your least favorite class in high school or college.

129

You might also like