Cohen Yuval 2015
Cohen Yuval 2015
Cohen Yuval 2015
Project report submitted in part fulfilment of the requirements for the degree of
therein.
Acknowledgements
advice at critical junctures in the project; Joanne Moore and Jiri Jerabek
at BBC R&D who shared their rich knowledge and experience in the field
grandparents, without whose support I would not have been able to pursue
Finally, I want to thank Dorota for her patience, calm, and unwavering
-2-
Abstract
delivered, and do not take social and individual contextual factors into
account.
This exploratory study aims to identify contextual factors that affect news
in-situ
burden, especially in regard to data entry, and was evaluated as
Findings indicated a range of contextual factors that are not accounted for
and apparatus were evaluated and found to have a generally low burden on
-3-
Contents
Chapter 1. Introduction 7
1.1. Aims 9
Potential Contributions 10
2.4. Mobility 17
3.1. Autoethnography 19
Diary Studies 20
3.5. Triangulation 25
Grounded Theory 26
-4-
Thematic Analysis 26
Chapter 4. Methods 27
4.1. Autoethnography 27
Participants 29
Apparatus 31
Procedure 34
Ethical Considerations 37
Chapter 5. Findings 41
Chapter 6. Discussion 79
-5-
Consumption characteristics are shaped by momentary 82
needs
Consumption ‘niches’ 83
6.4. Limitations 87
Chapter 7. Conclusion 89
-6-
Chapter 1. Introduction
facto
distribution channels for news stories. This effectively wrests control
how users’ physical, social and personal environments affect their news
[10,16,81]
. However, these contextual elements do not account for the
broader social, cultural and individual aspects that might shape a user’s
context of use
[34]
.
-7-
To study a particular context, user actions and habits must be examined
within the situation that they occur. User actions can be dynamic and
unpredictable, and are often an interplay with events that happen while an
the individual and social practices that develop within that environment
[32]
. The way in which individuals interpret their environment can often
lives of individuals
[83]
, and specifically, the patterns of news
to urban spaces
[37]
, and more specifically, the sense of isolation and
[22,28,64]
apparatus. Experience sampling method (ESM) has been used
in-situ
to obtain information that is of more real-time quality
[18,20,24]
,
-8-
[14,17,59,61]
. Additionally, the HCI community has been making
empathise with and gain better understanding of user needs and contexts
of use
[38,67]
.
The significant body of work into both news consumption and situated
Despite this, previous research has not combined the two in order to better
news consumers.
in-situ
Similarly, the technological apparatus used to conduct studies has
custom web platforms with SMS for diary and experience sampling studies
[20,78]
, but these do not provide an integrated software environment that
1.1. Aims
The current study aims to address two research questions. First, it aims to
of a more qualitative and experiential nature, which have been of less focus
in previous research. Second, the study aims to explore the effect of any
experience as a whole.
-9-
A further aim of this research is to examine the use of new apparatus for
in-situ
data collection. A commercially available app was appropriated for
use in the study, and its effectiveness for data collection was evaluated.
Potential Contributions
Results of the current study may be of potential use to members of the HCI
This research hopes to add social, cultural and individual elements to the
in-situ
conduct such studies, making research easier, more efficient and
1
https://www.wunderlist.com/
- 10 -
Chapter 2. Literature Review
[31,36,71]
.
people may have for a given situation might differ from their actual
- 11 -
of situated approaches has also expanded into social environments that are
provide. Dourish
[35]
argues, in relation to ethnography, that focusing as
an end result on what many HCI studies term ‘Implications for Design’, is
users and the context in which they operate. He argues that ‘discount
ethnography’ techniques and other field study methods are not necessarily
carried out in a methodically sound manner, and are conducted for the
in-the-wild
between observational, studies that involve the presence of a
in-situ
researcher, and self-reported studies that rely on
- 12 -
experience, context and environment. Observational studies offer the
and can uncover details that would otherwise not have been accurately
articulated by participants
[72]
. However, they present issues of scalability
[17]
– issues that are pertinent to the current study, and are also
in-situ
way to obtain insights
[20]
. Indeed, many exploratory studies have
Dourish
[34]
describes context in HCI as “human action and the
information for later use as retrieval cues, and more commonly, the
use, which is also the relevant approach to the current study. Dourish
describes two divergent theories in social sciences that are relevant for the
positivist
understanding context in relation to technology and HCI: and
phenomenological
.
positivist
In the approach, mathematics and statistics play a prime role,
Dey
[30]
, who presents a description of context that relies in large part on
- 13 -
role of information as a descriptor of states – of “people, groups or
phenomenological
The approach to the definition of context, as described
phenomenology
relates to the definition of by Winograd & Flores
[87]
in
situated action
[80]
. Indeed, Dourish notes them as theoretical
phenomenological
underpinnings of his view of context, as well as for
Embodied Interaction
[32]
– a conceptual framework that grounds the
phenomenological
elements as important pillars in the definition of
features should account for them. The current study largely adopts the
phenomenological
view of context, with the hope of gaining a better
experience.
- 14 -
Spaces and Places
phenomenological
Even within the approach to context, there is a question
between a physical space and the interactions that occur within it.
which user actions can take place. They describe the physical
aspects, which is a ‘place’. While not cited as a basis for the work of
situations. They found that, for the purposes of privacy, context was
- 15 -
“regulated by individual perceptions, exclusively shared knowledge,
While the current study is itself designed to explore the context around
news reading, usage data and related work can begin to paint a picture of
trails desktop use, but is rapidly growing, and is seen as the future focus
“supplementary” to news-reading
[84]
. The report describes ‘pathways’
important to the context in which a user consumes news, they are merely
consume news. This study aims to focus on those influences rather than on
specific content.
- 16 -
Previous work relating to contextually-aware news consumption
systems that ‘learn’ user habits and use patterns, and adapt a user
with live news items while simultaneously logging frequency, time spent
and location of reading, as well as speed and article completion rate using
scroll-tracking. The goal of this research was to profile and classify reading
them. Carreira
[16]
also focused on profiling patterns of use, though this
was done for customisation of content rather than interface. Other notable
2.4. Mobility
‘mobility turn’
[83]
. This school of thought in social sciences recognises the
result. Urry
[83]
makes a claim for the study of mobility as a social science
- 17 -
Building on Urry’s definition of a mobilised social paradigm, Dourish et al.
practice”. Dourish & Bell focus on mobility in the context of urbanism, and
[5,45]
. Earlier work by sociologist Georg Simmel described this behaviour
as such:
mobile work have the potential for generalisable insights. Perry et al.
[71]
note the existence of ‘dead times’ – periods of time which workers spend
devices
[31]
. Other studies have focused on issues such as battery life,
- 18 -
Chapter 3. Methodology and Approaches
approaches and reasoning used for the study and the analysis of its results.
the formative part of the study. It then continues to the techniques used
approach for the use of multiple methods in the study, and the approaches
analysis
[13]
.
3.1. Autoethnography
aims to represent voices and subtleties that might have not been
discovered otherwise
[27]
.
- 19 -
autoethnography as a method that allows researchers to empathise with
user experiences and form the basis for further research. While
news-reading, and also informed the design of the main study, its content
main research questions. The situated study was limited to two weeks per
longer studies
[11]
.
Diary Studies
in-situ
other relevant information. They are intended to minimise the
ethnography.
- 20 -
Carter & Mankoff
[17]
discuss two forms that a diary study can usually
has been of particular focus as digital devices gain traction as diary study
conducting situated studies with widely used devices, which are therefore
pointed out that Palen & Salzman’s participants could call at their
- 21 -
While some situated studies looked into news consumption within the
[20,28,55,64]
, they did not explore the specific area of the news
study to explore the various media used on a daily basis to access news,
and found that mobile channels largely did not displace traditional media,
A particular focus of several diary studies has been to lower the data entry
studies, especially when entries are done under mobile conditions. Brandt
et al.
[12]
proposed a ‘snippet’ technique in which participants chose an
input modality (text, image, video) that they were most comfortable with,
using a mobile device that they carry around with them, to capture small
in-situ
pieces (‘snippets’) of information about their experience . These
later served as cues and reminders for a more detailed web-based diary.
Since its debut in 2007, the snippet technique has been used on several
in-situ
occasions to support explorations of user context and behaviour.
Sohn et al.
[78]
used an adaptation of the technique to conduct a two-week
[20]
conducted a three month study with 108 participants, in which the
- 22 -
proactively probe participants for relevant experiences several times a day,
for situated studies in psychology. Being a form of diary study itself, ESM
examine the link between an external context and the contents of the
scheduled or event-based
[24]
. Owing to the spontaneous nature of
- 23 -
triggering. Consolvo and Walker note, however, that scheduled and
event-based carry the risk of cognitive bias, which this study hopes to
ESM also plays an important role in the diary study component of the
sampling data that was captured every morning was used as snippets for
in-situ
media as memory cues. Mancini et al. and Nagel et al.
[59] [61]
of technological platforms
[18,40]
. However, development of custom
- 24 -
applications has been scarce since the late 2000s, notably since the rise to
data-entry platforms, but this has been done mainly for diary studies, as
hopefully lower the ‘barrier of entry’ for future situated research, and will
3.5. Triangulation
2
https://metricwire.com/
3
http://nativeye.com/
- 25 -
3.6. Data Analysis Approach
data. A short overview of both methods in the context of the current study
Grounded Theory
the current study called upon key elements of grounded theory, such as
started to emerge
[41]
and collection of further data as needed, memo
Thematic Analysis
and Clarke
[13]
mention this precise ability to find meaning across a range
Clarke as one of two primary ways in thematic analysis fits within the
- 26 -
Chapter 4. Methods
This chapter describes the implementation of the methodologies set out in
the previous chapter. It details how the study was designed and conducted,
and describes the procedures used to collect and analyse participant data.
4.1. Autoethnography
contexts and settings. News consumption devices of large and small form
factors were used, as well as newspapers. Emphases during this study were
(during work, study, social activities) and triggers for reading, such as
social media posts and notifications from news apps mobile devices.
Potential methods and apparatus for the main study were examined as
well. Two commercially available apps were selected for testing – Evernote
4
, a note-taking app, and Wunderlist, a task management app with
study were conducted in the form of notes, tasks and reminders within
preset reminder each day within the two apps, and experience sampling
4
https://evernote.com/
- 27 -
(i.e. the London Underground). Additionally, Wunderlist featured a more
A pilot study was conducted to simulate the combined snippet and diary
collection tool, with participants other than the researcher. Two of the
female aged 31 took part in the pilot. All were acquaintances of the author.
The study took place over a period of four days that included both
- 28 -
4.4. Main Study
Participants
Wunderlist app compatibility), and read the news on a regular basis using
confirm that participants meet the inclusion criteria. This information was
also used to diversify the study sample in terms of age, gender and
students vs. non-students. It was also used to gauge commute time, as this
with £10 in cash or transfer upon completion of the study, and one £50
Seventeen participants were recruited for the main user study, ten are
SD=8). Fourteen participants are currently living in London, and three live
participants are students, and six are professionals. Table 4.1 provides
5
Form URL: http://bit.ly/UCLICnews
- 29 -
P# Age Gender Locale Commute Technical
(Home) Time Proficiency
(Self-assessed)
- 30 -
Apparatus
The Wunderlist platform was used to implement the diary study and
Wunderlist provides a basic tier of its platform and apps free of charge,
the app.
questions, which they could tap in order to view and respond to each of
- 31 -
Figure 4.1 – Wunderlist main screens (from the top-left clockwise):
the Wunderlist app with the ‘UCL News Study’ list on the bottom and a
- 32 -
The app allows users to enter text in several fields: the ‘task’ fields adjacent
and a comments thread. Users were not instructed where to answer, and
software for Mac, with each participant being added as a ‘list’ that was
shared by the researcher with the participant. Both the researcher and the
participant could freely add, edit and annotate items on the list.
Figure 4.2: list creation and management from the Wunderlist app for Mac.
Each shared list in the left column represents a participant, and the area on
the right shows items on a specific list, or in the context of the current study
- 33 -
Procedure
1. Instruction email
3. Exit interview
Instruction email
Participants who signed up via the screener form received an email (see
sign-up instructions for the Wunderlist app, and explanations on the types
and timing of the questions that would be sent to them along with
Participants were sent two sets of questions every day – snippets and diary
questions, over a period of two weeks. Snippets were sent every morning,
news-reading environment that day. Sending times varied from one day to
- 34 -
In following days, the examples were removed so not to lead participant
responses.
Four to five diary questions were sent every evening. Questions were
and were usually open ended so as not to limit the scope of responses.
While questions varied in wording and order from one day to another so
they would not seem repetitive to participants, they were focused on four
well as several examples for questions participants were asked about them.
- 35 -
Question Topic Examples
Triggers for news-reading experiences What made you want to read (or listen to,
or watch) the news today?
Why did you choose to read, watch or
listen to the news at this specific time?
(more than one answer is ok)
Environment and surroundings (e.g. What did you like about your reading
concurrent activities, distractions, public environment? What did you dislike?
or social settings) Were you around others while reading? If
yes – did this affect your reading in any
way?
Did anything bother or distract you while
reading? If so – what was it?
Feelings about news consumption Did you find the experience: positive,
experience as a whole negative or neither? (please explain if
possible)
Did reading the news affect your mood in
any way? If yes – in what way?
In the early days of the study, all participants received the same questions,
which were modified from day to day in order to attain more detailed
that in order to stay true to the study’s exploratory goal and to discover as
Exit Interviews
Skype, and were audio-recorded for later transcription and analysis. Each
- 36 -
interview lasted between 20 to 30 minutes, and included three parts:
Ethical Considerations
The study was approved by the UCL Interaction Centre Ethics Chair, under
collected snippets and diary responses were sampled on a daily basis and
compared to concepts and insights that had begun to emerge. Memos were
used to note and highlight concepts that were repeated across different
participants or days.
- 37 -
Figure 4.3: Memos noting emerging concepts from study data
the situated study phase, they were not used between the situated phase
retrospective method
[63,74]
.
concepts and categories that emerged during the open coding stage,
- 38 -
contextual and intervening conditions, as these were essentially the
purpose of the research, and would be the basis for later thematic
analysis.
Coding and Line-by-line coding of snippets, 160 codes and initial themes
initial themes diaries and interviews using in-vivo
or descriptive codes
Collation and Codes and themes grouped into Themes consolidated to high-level
Categorisation conceptually-related hierarchies, categories
compared to earlier insights
Definition and Categories reviewed, final themes Five main themes defined:
naming of final and concepts defined triggers, positive/conducive factors,
themes negative/distracting factors, barriers
to use, methodology and apparatus
(see full results in chapter 5)
- 39 -
Figure 4.4: Affinity diagram
- 40 -
Chapter 5. Findings
Wunderlist
implementation using the platform as an appropriated
in-situ
apparatus for data collection.
Participation rates
Fourteen out of the seventeen participants took part for the full length of
the study, and only one participant took part for less than half of the
study’s two week period. All participants took part in exit interviews. Five
entering text.
- 41 -
In-situ data entry burden
particular focus during the study. An effort was made to design both the
snippet and the diary questions in a manner that would attain the desired
information, but not place a demand on participants’ time that would deter
them from participating. Therefore, the author evaluated the issue during
exit interviews.
When asked about the snippets and diary questions, there was a consensus
among participants that the burden placed on them in terms of time and
effort was not only acceptable, but that it was low in most cases, and did
not have an impact on their daily routine. Some participants indicated that
they responded when they had a few minutes in which they were not
otherwise busy.
snippet, which I thought was nice for just 'on the go', when I'm reading
the news on my phone, and was convenient; The other ones – those were
as the primary focus of their effort, rather than data entry itself.
“It was more that I had to be aware and think about "oh right, I've just
read some news, this is where I am right now", but it wasn't a big effort.”
[2] P4 – Interview
- 42 -
One participant even described the process of answering the snippets and
Two issues were raised, however, as possible burdens. First, some reported
which field to enter their answers; Section 5.5.3 explores this in more
detail. Second, several participants reported that they were not sure how to
One participant was worried his answers were repetitive from one day to
another. While this was the intention of some of the diary questions, aimed
“I don't really know how helpful it is when I'm just answering the same
thing each time; And I guess that was my only concern [...] it's a little bit
annoying to type the same things sometimes, but in the end I was just a
bit like ‘I feel like I'm doing something wrong here, I feel like I'm giving
briefing. However, due to the use of multiple data collection methods and
the remote nature of the study, the author did not conduct a pre-study
- 43 -
Participants were divided as to whether such a preliminary briefing would
actually alleviate the issue of being unsure about their answers. One
changed his feelings about his answers, however his response does seem to
instructions]
, people are still kind of wary, because they know they are
part of a study, and they don't want to keep writing the same thing,
because they feel like they're not being useful, so I don't actually think
Other participants did feel that the availability of the researcher to answer
any questions and provide guidance did help them and was a conducive
“I felt that you were there to guide me if I needed you.. You didn't just
throw me into the deep end and say "you need to do this, you need to
work it out, that’s wrong", because I knew you were always there... To
anything or if I mess it up, I can just email you and you will get back to
me very quickly in a really nice, friendly way, so that [...] really made the
- 44 -
5.1.2.
In-Situ
Methods
in-situ
gauge whether the methods achieved their intended effect, and how
in-situ
experience sampling, as well a low-burden capturing of cues for
One participant described how the snippets served as a reminder, but also
“They
[the snippets]
were pretty good, because that was my schedule [...]
flexible, you can't time it just when I’m pretty much done with my reading
[...] they were convenient and it was really good to have them as a
Another participant noted that strong emotions about a certain news story
- 45 -
“ [...] there were a couple of times that I remember that I had just read
something that had upset me, and so I actually went and answered
One of the participants noted the utility of snippets as memory cues for
“ [...] the snippets were just like… This is a diary I'm keeping of what I've
when you ask questions.. It worked for me, I liked that structure.”
Response aggregation
entries for multiple days at the same time. While the content of the entries
did supposedly reflect their actual activities, it was more likely to suffer
snippets and diary entries in this way. This participant did not use
by email.
when it was convenient.. So, at the end of the day I would check my
- 46 -
emails and see if there was a question, and I would do the question then...
And sometimes the previous day I might not had checked it, so I think
“I'm just not very good at keeping up with answering it daily [...] Like
yesterday I just didn't read news and I just totally forgot about filling it
in-situ
technological apparatus in self-reported studies.
Participants’ perspectives
this, while others simply chose the field most convenient for them. This
- 47 -
was not an issue in terms of data analysis, as all fields could be exported
“I guess there were a few places where you could plug your answer [...] I
was a bit unsure where you wanted it, like straight after the question or
diary questions from the Wunderlist mobile app, they could not see the
bottom ‘notes’ text field if there were multiple questions in the task
checkbox area. This meant that they had to scroll multiple times from one
- 48 -
Figure 5.1: diary ‘task’ in the Wunderlist iPhone app, with multiple
questions. When the page is scrolled to the top, the bottom ‘notes’ field is
not visible (left), and when the page is scrolled to the bottom, part of the the
One participant noted this had an effect on navigability within the app.
“I found it hard to really find my way around the app and to answer the
questions. It was on my phone [...] so the screen wasn't big enough for me
to see that there is [...] at the bottom of the page where I can put in the
answer”
- 49 -
Another participant described that scrolling between the questions and the
‘notes’ field meant that she did not always remember which question she
was answering and then click into the notes, answer my question, and
but a couple of participants reported that they did not receive the email.
Researcher’s perspectives
On the researcher side, the Wunderlist platform had several issues relating
display name that is separate from the title participants see. This meant
that in order for participants to be presented with a short title when they
received notifications, all lists had to be named ‘UCL news study’, making
it difficult for the author to differentiate between them within the software.
copying of items from one list to another, meaning that the author had to
enter all of the snippets and diary questions manually. While tedious, it
- 50 -
was manageable in a study of 17 participants, but studies of a larger sample
Participants valued the fact that Wunderlist’s mobile app allowed them to
choosing, indicating that the ability to provide full responses ‘on the go’
“I did it
[respond in Wunderlist]
on the overground and the bus, because
moment, and once I get the connection back it will push the updates, so.. I
[15] P8 – Interview
“I think a lot of the times I used it when I was in the tube, so I think I still
managed to use it, even without the wifi.. I don't know. But I didn't have a
- 51 -
point where I was worried about it, because I felt like it will save my
Triggers are specific drivers for news consumption in a given situation, and
are defined in this study as the reason stated by a participant that directly
underlying drivers for these triggers can range from momentary needs and
level of interest in news to boredom and the level of usage for personal
computing devices.
A theme with nearly universal prevalence among participants was the use
listening or watching, was usually cited as the way in which news was
definitely go back whenever I feel the need to take a break from work.”
[17] P3 – Interview
- 52 -
The reason cited for choosing news-reading as a break activity also enjoyed
almost the same level of universality. While this reason was described
relatively similar, the same kind of concentration is involved, but it’s still
different enough that it provides a rest from what I was working on”
[18] P2 – Interview
by several participants, with some saying that their news-reading will not
If I'm really burnt out I won't absorb any of the news, but it gives me
focus my brain on. Reading the news isn't that mentally tasking.. Since I
use it as a break task, I don't necessarily absorb the news very well... I
don't recall it once I go back to work and start doing that; A few hours
- 53 -
“ [...] if you're working in an office you generally need to break, rest your
eyes for a few minutes, look at something else, focus on something that’s a
different from your computer screen [...] a bit of a refresher [...] and then
you'll have a renewed perspective when you jump back into it.”
type of break. They noted the fact that news stories have a defined
“ [...] it also has a beginning and the end – when I finish reading an
article I go back to work. I can obviously read another article, but that
break task might be an indication for this process of ego depletion and
- 54 -
5.2.2. External triggers
consumption experience.
few seconds while widgets permanently reside on the user’s screen until
they are removed. As triggers for news consumption, both elicit the same
action from users and are therefore classified together in this category.
It should be noted that both notifications and widgets can be tapped by the
user to read more about a given story, giving a choice whether to pursue a
One participant indicated that this decision depends on the type of story,
and whether she is otherwise busy. At times she will be content with
- 55 -
“The push notifications from the Guardian app keep me informed without
“ [...] 'England won the Ashes'.. I don't actually need more information
than that, for example [...] It's completely dependent on how interested I
am in the story and also what I'm doing at the time [...] they pop up at
any time during the day, and I don’t always have the liberty to check it
what it was before I've come back [...] it just depends completely on [...]
Participants described a logic for news consumption via social media that
is similar to the one they described for notifications and widgets – i.e. a
“I pick up a lot of stuff that I read through the news feed of my Facebook
page, [...] I find that more convenient way of accessing it, because it's sort
“
[I]
scroll down and see if there's anything interesting [...] If you're
interested then you'll click on it [...] If not then just on to the next one. So
it's just a quick scan of things to see if something really interesting has
- 56 -
Waiting for people or technology
main study. Two main types of waiting were identified – waiting for people
program, to finish its course. This can possibly be viewed in light of earlier
the effect on the type of content he will read and his receptiveness to it.
“[...] you're waiting for a friend for like an hour or something [...] it
becomes a bit tedious, because I know that I'm using this just to fill time,
sometimes I really enjoy it when I'm actually saying ‘ok, I actually want
to see what's going on today’, but other times it's just because I don't have
anything to do... It's just to check whatever is going on there; And usually
[25] P8 – Interview
- 57 -
“ [...] it's really just killing time until something happens or until someone
will meet me; It can be a bit frustrating, because if it's something that you
are waiting for and you know it starts at a certain time, you can kind of
judge what you will read, but if you're waiting for someone, it's
something... Because I tend not to go back to things as well, I think it's not
person, participants could better gauge the beginning and end time of the
waiting experience.
long processes on a computer and wait for them to finish. I always use
Media multitasking
consuming another type of media, television for example, and they will
- 58 -
primary content source was one that did not elicit the author’s full
attention or interest.
some point it didn’t really interest me anymore; Even though I was still
show together with his partner, and is not particularly interested in its
content.
it – I'll pick up my tablet and read the news or do something else as well.”
[29] P3 – Interview
evaluate the perceived benefit from each source and compare between
“I will definitely scan a bit between the two [...] I guess it depends on how
engrossed I am in either; I guess if the movie [...] has a slow part, then I'll
move back to the news, and if the news is really interesting, then I'll get
engrossed in that and focused on that, and then once I'm done I'll look
back up and say ‘this is going on’ in the movie [...] I'll go back and forth.
will make me go back and forth [...] I suck at multitasking [...] I'll be
- 59 -
listening for keywords, cues, or if something loud happens on TV, but
much. With that said, it does cause me to scan back and forth
[30] P3 – Interview
Morning habit
Two reasons were generally cited for this morning habit. The first is, to use
the world’, in other words – feeling a need to be updated about the news.
“When I just wake up and I want to know what's going on in the world,
so in the morning I always check it [...] it's a habit, because I wake up... I
always wake up quite early in the morning, so I can take my time to start
easily, have my breakfast, and while I have my breakfast I'll scroll down
This was cited either together or apart from the need to be updated about
the news.
- 60 -
“I don’t know where it comes from
[the habit of reading the news in the
morning]
… Maybe it’s just that I don’t want to start immediately with
[32] P2 – Interview
phone in bed, so yeah... After I'm finished to read and replying to all the
messages that I need to reply to [...] I'll just be lazy in bed and read
Positive conducive
or factors are defined, in this analysis, as contextual
The effect of an individual’s affective state was indicated during the study
in several respects. One of them is the effect on the length of news items
that a participant was consuming, their type – ‘hard news’ vs. ‘soft news’.
‘Hard news’ is thought to be that which informs about such topics as war
- 61 -
open and receptive to this type of reading when they are alert rather than
One participant stated that a low level of alertness has the effect on her of
article and I’m tired I feel like I’m just LOSING IT, I feel like nothing is
[34] P2 – Interview
Another participant stated that while she will sometimes read news before
she goes to sleep at night, she will not want to read stories that might be
upsetting.
- 62 -
“I won't try to read anything too harrowing
[before going to sleep]
, you
know, we're talking just interesting stuff... I try not to read about ISIS
day that can be brought about by stressful events during the day, and can
“It depends on my mood or the level of activity and stress in my life, how
tired I am, even sometimes if the train is packed, or there's a bad energy
on it, or it's really hot, or I'm not seated, you know, things like that.. they
that, in her mind, is associated with a positive affective state and feelings
Sunday is like the one day where I [...] just relax in the morning, because
I just have such a busy life [...] that's my treat for Sunday, to just be able
to lie in bed with a cup of tea and read the papers [...] but I usually have
the radio on as well; So it's about time to relax [...] to me that's a really
big luxury [...] I don't get to do it very often [...] I will read something
much more in-depth, and longer, because I have the luxury of the time.
- 63 -
It is important to note that the aforementioned spectrum is not
between ‘hard news’ and ‘soft news’ can appear the other side of their
usual placing on the spectrum. For example, the same participant quoted
above described her Sunday experience as one in which she consumes ‘soft
news’.
“I always buy the Saturday Guardian and read it the day after, just
because I like the content [...] I read stuff like fashion and lifestyle and
Background activity
background activity, though the types of stimulations that would cause this
One participant noted that listening to music and reading the news will be
a common combination for him while on the train, citing that the music is
- 64 -
when I want to listen to music and read the news, I stay concentrated, it
The same participant noted that a shorter, focused background sound like
news-reading.
“if I'm on the train and it's very crowded, and I need to get a grip or
monitor the next station where I'll be getting off, I have to split my
“ [...] when I'm just walking on the street, I cannot read something and
[41] P1 – Interview
TV. One participant even noted that this switch will have a specific trigger
– she will switch from reading to music when background sounds distract
- 65 -
“I try to then
Participant: [when there is noise]
turn the music on. I'm not
focusing with the distractions, so I just try to listen to music and that's it
Interviewer:
“So you're saying you'll usually choose between reading or
music.”
Participant:
Yes, that's right.
Interviewer:
“ [...] So you're saying, basically, that if there's a noise in the
Participant:
Yeah.
[42] P1 – Interview
“There was a lot of movement in the room, which I caught from the
distracting
[auditory stimulations]
, you can get into a sort of ‘zone’, where
you tune it out, but in that situation I was sitting in a way that I was just
faced with people, and I was looking directly at the path that people were
taking, and I had my head in a way that the noise didn’t get to me, but
- 66 -
“If I'm standing somewhere super-crowded, I'm not going to grab my
phone and read the news, like on the bus [...] same with the overground
during rush-hour, it can get packed and I'm not going to break out my
phone [...] it's just uncomfortable to do so, and also someone can just
“I tend to see other people looking. You get the feeling like someone else is
also watching what you're reading, and that's not really nice and makes
tolerable and are not willing to forego news-reading unless the situation is
extreme.
“ [...] on the tube it's always really really crowded.. Sometimes it's hard to
[...] it's too crowded to even get your phone out and have a look at it [...]
it's loud and it's bumpy [...] you can't really focus on what you're looking
at, but equally it's something to do while you're spending those 12 minutes
- 67 -
5.4. Contextual Factors – Negative/Distracting
Negative distracting
or factors are defined, in this analysis, as contextual
experience, possibly causing them to alter the experience in some way but
not end it. This can be done by changing media or device they use to
topic).
Connectivity
digital device even when offline, switching to paper form, or avoiding the
an item ‘hoarding’ of sorts, though one user did note his frustration at the
“Usually on the public transport, I open news sites in different tabs and I
activate it; So I find that not very comfortable, because in fact, I need to
[47] P6 – Interview
6
https://getpocket.com/
- 68 -
Another participant noted the use of the use of the ‘offline mode’ in the
“ [...] the Guardian app actually works offline, you just can't get the
pictures and there's certain content that you can't get, but you can
actually get the stories, even if you haven't got a signal, which is
of no connectivity.
“ [...] if I'm on the tube as well, I tend to pick up the free papers... I read
the news that way, so there’s not much point in me looking at the BBC
website when I'm on the tube.. And also, I can't get reception [...] ”
“If I'm in the tube, I cannot access the Internet, so I don't think I will do
- 69 -
Smartphone = inconvenient experience?
while unavoidable on many occasions, did not provide what they perceived
as the optimal experience. A prevalent reason cited for this is that the
One participant noted that she feels it is not as easy for her to actively
Participant:
“If I want to explore more about the news, of course it's
because it's easier to look for more information, because on the phone
you're not really going to look up everything, it takes too much time.”
Interviewer:
“It takes too much time in terms of interaction with the
Participant:
“Yeah, to go from the news app, to Google, to search for
[51] P5 – Interview
that was hampered by the form factor and presentation of content on the
phone.
The actual reading experience is fine when you're reading an article that
you want to read and it's just text [...] but I feel like it doesn’t facilitate
easy links into other similar things [...] You can kind of scroll down
through the article and there are related items on certain websites, at the
- 70 -
bottom. I think it's something to do with the screen size, that it just feels
very claustrophobic [...] Once things are translated from a website down
into a small screen, it just feels very dense [...] there's no sort of space to...
I don't know... It feels very tight and kind of difficult to navigate to other
things, you know? If there was something in there that you wanted to
read more about, it's not as simple as opening another tab in your
phone was that certain websites were not customised for viewing on the
small screen of the phone. One participant noted this during the study in
went to were links from Facebook, and didn't have mobile sites or apps”
[53] P4 – Diary
“ [...] it's also frustrating when people haven't [...] translated things
properly for mobile, which happens quite a lot, where text doesn't resize
properly. All of that sort of stuff makes it less, you know, comfortable.”
- 71 -
device to consume news. One participant noted that when having the
choice between her phone and a larger form-factor device such as a laptop,
Participant:
“ [...] for reading at home, it's not on the computer; Basically,
I will use my mobile phone [...] while I'm using the computer and doing
have some leisure time, I will take my mobile phone and send some
message to my friends and also look for some news on my mobile phone.
Interviewer:
“When you're taking that break, if you're reading, would you
Participant:
“I will prefer to use the mobile phone.”
[55] P1 – Interview
however she did indicate that her consumption will usually be focused on
shorter pieces.
the study]
, they were read on the laptop, especially the ones when I'm at
work [...] but other than that I mainly read news on my phone. [...] not
too much in the office, but when I'm at home or on the go, so yeah, it's on
Interviewer:
And would you say that you usually go for shorter pieces or
Participant:
I think shorter pieces. [56] P15 – Interview
- 72 -
Multitasking
were varied in both type and location, but generally had the effect of
while she is making tea, making the case why news-reading, specifically, is
“I almost always read the news when I’m making tea, it’s a practical
thing, you can’t do anything else; And if you go away for like two
The same participant noted the effect of a concurrent task on the type of
where something else is going on, reading as a way to pass the time [...]
If it’s a story that requires me to think, to process what’s going on, what
the page is telling me, then I can’t really get into it that much”
[58] P2 – Interview
- 73 -
Another participant noted the productivity aspect as a driver for
multitasking while consuming news, and reiterated the effect it has on the
the news when I'm doing something else [...] I tend to feel like I'm being
channel on while I'm sort of tidying up, or doing something that doesn't
require [...] intellectual focus [...] when I'm consuming news while I'm
cognitive effort.
“Generally I would stop reading the news for almost everything else. I
The same participant later elaborated on the issue, noting that this
multitasking and the secondary role that news occupies in terms of his
consumption.
- 74 -
“I don't think that there are many news items
[for which]
I would
completely block everything out and not quite notice there's something
else happening, it's like [...] just a distraction half the time. If I'm going to
The sum of these statements indicate that there are instances in which
are not devoting their full attention to the news consumption experience,
specific context.
Barriers are factors that lead to a situation where a user who would
otherwise be open to news consumption will choose not to do so. This can
‘Me time’
transport as one in which they do not want to engage in any form of news
- 75 -
when discussing nature as a central part of a wider framework for
“ [...] sometimes when you're walking and you're on public transport, you
populated train.
“Sometimes I'll just be ‘this is me time’ [...] no one can get me, I'm not
around me, my head's spinning with stuff, I just can't put anymore stuff
in it, even if it's a distraction, I need to relax my mind, and the train, for
me, is the only place I can actually do that; Because nobody can get me,
and nobody knows me, and if suddenly I bump into people I know it's hell
[...] It's nice to just stare out the window and empty my head”
- 76 -
News overload
negative nature. While all participants who attested to this chose to stop
“there are days when [...] especially if there's been a barrage of [...] Bad
news, recently... Sometime you just want to put your head in the sand
“most of the news I don't find very interesting, like who killed who this
Kinetosis
One participant noted that she resolves the issues by only reading books,
This could possibly indicate a desire on the part of the user to carry on
with the reading experience despite the physical obstacle, finding solutions
- 77 -
“If I'm on public transportation I'll read a book, I don't usually read
news. [...] I have a tendency to get motion sickness, and reading a book...
Another participant indicated that she will avoid reading while she is
cause dizziness and nausea, however this does not occur when she is
seated.
“ [...] if it's too crowded then I wouldn't have a place to sit, I will have to
hang on to something like hold the rails or just try to balance myself, and
I don’t want to read while I'm doing that, and usually I get this dizziness
when I'm trying to read while I'm balancing... So, I switch to music and I
“If I was on the bus, if I read news or if I read anything, I will feel dizzy,
- 78 -
Chapter 6. Discussion
The current study discovered a variety of contextual factors that play a role
in news consumption. True to its initial goal, the study revealed factors
The study discovered that these contextual factors are not fixed or
affect each other, with user action being a result of a ‘balancing act’
This study also supports the notion that mobility plays an ever increasing
- 79 -
in relation to the space in which they are being used, such as participants’
said, some participants did critique elements of the experience that mobile
content.
and environments for news consumption that suit specific and dynamic
[46]
to the appropriation of spaces and technology as creators of context.
The current study also illustrated the value of situated methods for the
dependent on the number of methods used, but rather on the way in which
and apparatus.
- 80 -
6.1. News consumption is opportunistic
consumption.
factors such as kinetosis (motion sickness) and the desire for ‘me time’
positivist
connectivity, support a more more informational and approach
- 81 -
the factors cited as influencing news consumption, such as alertness,
the different factors, lending even further support to the approach set out
consumption habits were not only numerous, but also changed within an
in-situ
that they changed their actions as a result of both internal and
waiting for other people, they will continuously adapt the type of content
they read in terms of topic and length, in order to suit the waiting time and
an ‘interest/engrossment tradeoff’.
- 82 -
they perform when they want to keep their mind busy, fill otherwise ‘dead
purposes, the interplay between the momentary needs that drove these
effect both on the type of news they consumed and on their level of
Consumption ‘niches’
opportunity to realise that need, thereby creating their own unique news
interstices in which users ‘fit’ their news consumption, such as with their
positivist
elements inherent in the approach to concept
[34]
, rather than
phenomenological
those of the one. Indeed, Dimmick concludes his paper
- 83 -
The results of the current study add to Dimmick’s work by adding the
opportunism
activity. The formal definition of the term carries a negative
meaning
[68]
. However, the author of this paper would argue in favour of
of
in-situ
data collection
in-situ
inherent methodical biases, while lowering the data-entry burden.
Despite the supposed conflict between the number of methods used and
indicated the study was not demanding in terms of time or effort, therefore
in-situ
achieving the purpose of a low data entry burden. This proved to be
in-situ
platforms for the purpose of data collection is not without its
- 84 -
However, it is of note that these issues did not hinder the collection of data
[18,40]
. However, such endeavours are not always practical, as evident by
and iterated on routinely offers a path for research that is both expeditious
and resource-efficient.
Design of
in-situ
methods is key to managing participation burden
participation burden. While the cause for this was not explored directly as
part of the study, it is the author’s view that the question design and the
Even though the study combined several different methods, each of them
designed with the underlying assumption that any gaps in the data can be
- 85 -
6.3. Directions for future research
Seeing as the current study was an exploratory one, the author aims for the
elements such as user alertness, mood and distraction are not nearly as
easy to identify.
Second, and more importantly – just as GPS data is put to use within
whether they will be more open to ‘soft news’, essentially expanding the
habits, preferences and affective state. This will be of particular use, for
- 86 -
Wunderlist API7 for participant, snippet and diary management, thereby
6.4. Limitations
There were several limitations to the design and results of the current
study. First, some of the methods used carry the potential for certain
longer after participants’ actions have taken place. However, the use of
in-situ
closely to the study as possible
[74]
were designed to alleviate this.
answered them all at once. While not rendering the collected data
qualities of ESM and the value of snippets as memory cues, leaving the
following day after they were sent. Seeing as uncued memory lasts for
7
Wunderlist API: https://developer.wunderlist.com/
- 87 -
sampling, it would be ideal to further explore and gauge the effectiveness
- 88 -
Chapter 7. Conclusion
range of social, cultural and individual factors that drive the manner in
which users consume news, and contextual factors. Most notably, the
appropriation of spaces
[46]
. Participation rates and participant
in-situ
herald further work into the design of methods that lower
in-situ
software applications for the purpose of research.
- 89 -
References
1. Gregory D. Abowd, Anind K. Dey, Peter J. Brown, Nigel Davies, Mark Smith,
and Pete Steggles. 1999. Towards a Better Understanding of Context and
Context-Awareness. Proceedings of the 1st International Symposium on
Handheld and Ubiquitous Computing , Springer-Verlag, 304–307.
2. Gregory D. Abowd, Elizabeth D. Mynatt, and Tom Rodden. 2002. The
Human Experience. IEEE pervasive computing . Retrieved from
http://echo.iat.sfu.ca/library/abowd_02_human-experience.pdf
3. Anne Adams and Anna L. Cox. 2008. Questionnaires, in-depth interviews
and focus groups. In Research Methods for Human Computer Interaction ,
Paul Cairns and Anna L. Cox (eds.). Cambridge University Press, Cambridge,
UK, 17–34.
4. Alia Amin, Sian Townsend, Jacco van Ossenbruggen, and Lynda Hardman.
2009. Fancy a Drink in Canary Wharf?: A User Study on Location-Based
Mobile Search. In Human-Computer Interaction – INTERACT 2009.
Springer Berlin Heidelberg, 736–749.
5. Marc Augé. 1995. Non-places : introduction to an anthropology of
supermodernity.London : Verso.
6. Lisa Feldman Barrett and Daniel J. Barrett. 2001. An Introduction to
Computerized Experience Sampling in Psychology. Social science computer
review 19, 2: 175–185.
7. R. F. Baumeister, E. Bratslavsky, M. Muraven, and D. M. Tice. 1998. Ego
depletion: is the active self a limited resource? Journal of personality and
social psychology 74, 5: 1252–1265.
8. Genevieve Bell, Mark Blythe, Bill Gaver, Phoebe Sengers, and Peter Wright.
2003. Designing culturally situated technologies for the home. C HI ’03
Extended Abstracts on Human Factors in Computing Systems , ACM,
1062–1063.
9. Alice Jane Bernheim Brush. 2009. Ubiquitous Computing Field Studies. In
Ubiquitous Computing Fundamentals , John Krumm (ed.). Chapman and
Hall/CRC, 161–202.
10. Daniel Billsus and Michael J. Pazzani. 2007. Adaptive News Access. In T he
Adaptive Web . Springer Berlin Heidelberg, 550–570.
11. Alan Blackwell, R. Jones, N. Milic-Frayling, and Kerry Rodden. 2005.
Combining logging with interviews to investigate web browser usage in the
workplace. Position paper, CHI2005 Workshop .
12. Joel Brandt, Noah Weiss, and Scott R. Klemmer. 2007. Txt 4 L8R: Lowering
the Burden for Diary Studies Under Mobile Conditions. CHI ’07 Extended
Abstracts on Human Factors in Computing Systems , ACM, 2303–2308.
13. Virginia Braun and Victoria Clarke. 2006. Using thematic analysis in
psychology. Qualitative research in psychology 3, 2: 77–101.
14. Barry A. T. Brown, Abigail J. Sellen, and Kenton P. O’Hara. 2000. A Diary
Study of Information Capture in Working Life. Proceedings of the SIGCHI
Conference on Human Factors in Computing Systems , ACM, 438–445.
15. Graham Button and Paul Dourish. 1996. Technomethodology: paradoxes and
possibilities.
Proceedings of the SIGCHI Conference on Human Factors in
- 90 -
Computing Systems , ACM, 19–26.
16. Ricardo Carreira, Jaime M. Crato, Daniel Gonçalves, and Joaquim A. Jorge.
2004. Evaluating Adaptive User Profiles for News Classification. Proceedings
of the 9th International Conference on Intelligent User Interfaces , ACM,
206–212.
17. Scott Carter and Jennifer Mankoff. 2005. When Participants Do the
Capturing: The Role of Media in Diary Studies. Proceedings of the SIGCHI
Conference on Human Factors in Computing Systems , ACM, 899–908.
18. Scott Carter, Jennifer Mankoff, and Jeffrey Heer. 2007. Momento: support
for situated ubicomp experimentation. Proceedings of the SIGCHI
Conference on Human Factors in Computing Systems , ACM, 125–134.
19. Chaomei Chen and Roy Rada. 1996. Modelling Situated Actions in
Collaborative Hypertext Databases. J ournal of computer-mediated
communication: JCMC 2, 3: 0–0.
20. Karen Church, Mauro Cherubini, and Nuria Oliver. 2014. A Large-scale
Study of Daily Information Needs Captured in Situ. ACM Trans. Comput.
-Hum. Interact. 21, 2: 10:1–10:46.
21. Karen Church and Nuria Oliver. 2011. Understanding mobile web and mobile
search use in today’s dynamic mobile landscape. Proceedings of the 13th
International Conference on Human Computer Interaction with Mobile
Devices and Services , ACM, 67–76.
22. Karen Church and Barry Smyth. 2009. Understanding the intent behind
mobile information needs. P roceedings of the 14th international conference
on Intelligent user interfaces , ACM, 247–256.
23. Sunny Consolvo, Beverly Harrison, Ian Smith, et al. 2007. Conducting in situ
evaluations for and with ubiquitous computing technologies. I nternational
journal of human-computer interaction 22, 1-2: 103–118.
24. Sunny Consolvo and Miriam Walker. 2003. Using the Experience Sampling
Method to Evaluate Ubicomp Applications. IEEE pervasive computing /
IEEE Computer Society [and] IEEE Communications Society 2, 2: 24–31.
25. Marios Constantinides, John Dowell, David Johnson, and Sylvain Malacria.
2015. Exploring mobile news reading interactions for news app
personalisation. MobileHCI ’15 - Proceedings of the 17th international
conference on Human-computer interaction with mobile devices & services,
University College London.
26. M. Csikszentmihalyi and R. Larson. 1987. Validity and reliability of the
Experience-Sampling Method. The Journal of nervous and mental disease
175, 9: 526–536.
27. Sally Jo Cunningham and Matt Jones. 2005. Autoethnography: A Tool for
Practice and Education. Proceedings of the 6th ACM SIGCHI New Zealand
Chapter’s International Conference on Computer-human Interaction:
Making CHI Natural , ACM, 1–8.
28. David Dearman, Melanie Kellar, and Khai N. Truong. 2008. An Examination
of Daily Information Needs and Sharing Opportunities. Proceedings of the
2008 ACM Conference on Computer Supported Cooperative Work,ACM,
679–688.
29. Norman K. Denzin and Yvonna S. Lincoln. 1994. Introduction: Entering the
field of qualitative research. In Handbook of qualitative research, Norman
K. Denzin and Yvonna S. Lincoln (eds.). Sage, 1–17.
30. Anind K. Dey. 2001. Understanding and Using Context. Personal and
Ubiquitous Computing 5, 1: 4–7.
- 91 -
31. John Dimmick, John Christian Feaster, and Gregory J. Hoplamazian. 2010.
News in the Interstices: The niches of mobile media in space and time. New
Media & Society . http://doi.org/10.1177/1461444810363452
32. Paul Dourish. 2001. W here the Action is: The Foundations of Embodied
Interaction. MIT Press.
33. Paul Dourish. 2001. Seeking a foundation for context-aware computing.
Human-Computer Interaction 16, 2: 229–241.
34. Paul Dourish. 2004. What We Talk About when We Talk About Context.
Personal and Ubiquitous Computing 8, 1: 19–30.
35. Paul Dourish. 2006. Implications for design. P roceedings of the SIGCHI
Conference on Human Factors in Computing Systems , ACM, 541–550.
36. Paul Dourish, Ken Anderson, and Dawn Nafus. 2007. Cultural Mobilities:
Diversity and Agency in Urban Computing. In Human-Computer Interaction
– INTERACT 2007 . Springer Berlin Heidelberg, 100–113.
37. Paul Dourish and Genevieve Bell. 2011. Mobility and Urbanism. In D ivining
a Digital Future: Mess and Mythology in Ubiquitous Computing.MIT Press,
117–136.
38. Margot Duncan. 2004. Autoethnography: Critical appreciation of an
emerging art. International Journal of Qualitative Methods 3, 4: 28–39.
39. Carolyn Ellis, Tony E. Adams, and Arthur P. Bochner. 2010.
Autoethnography: An Overview. Forum Qualitative Sozialforschung /
Forum: Qualitative Social Research; Vol 12, No 1 (2011): The KWALON
Experiment: Discussions on Qualitative Data Analysis Software by
Developers and Users . Retrieved from
http://www.qualitative-research.net/index.php/fqs/article/view/1589
40. Jon Froehlich, Mike Y. Chen, Sunny Consolvo, Beverly Harrison, and James
A. Landay. 2007. MyExperience: A System for in Situ Tracing and Capturing
of User Feedback on Mobile Phones. Proceedings of the 5th International
Conference on Mobile Systems, Applications and Services , ACM, 57–70.
41. Dominic Furniss, Ann Blandford, and Paul Curzon. 2011. Confessions from a
grounded theory PhD: experiences and lessons learnt. Proceedings of the
SIGCHI Conference on Human Factors in Computing Systems , ACM,
113–122.
42. Thomas F. Gieryn. 2000. A Space for Place in Sociology. Annual review of
sociology 26: 463–496.
43. Barney G. Glaser and Anselm L. Strauss. 1967. The discovery of grounded
theory : strategies for qualitative research . Aldine Pub. Co., Chicago.
44. E. Goffman. 1963. Behavior in public places: notes on the social
organization of gatherings . Free Press of Glencoe.
45. Simon Gottschalk and Marko Salvaggio. 2015. Stuck Inside of Mobile:
Ethnography in Non-Places. Journal of contemporary ethnography 44, 1:
3–33.
46. Steve Harrison and Paul Dourish. 1996. Re-place-ing Space: The Roles of
Place and Space in Collaborative Systems. Proceedings of the 1996 ACM
Conference on Computer Supported Cooperative Work , ACM, 67–76.
47. J. M. Hektner, J. A. Schmidt, and M. Csikszentmihalyi. 2007. Experience
Sampling Method: Measuring the Quality of Everyday Life . SAGE
Publications.
48. Jan Hess and Volker Wulf. 2009. Explore Social Behavior Around
Rich-media: A Structured Diary Study. P roceedings of the Seventh European
Conference on European Interactive Television Conference , ACM, 215–218.
- 92 -
49. Joyce Ho and Stephen S. Intille. 2005. Using Context-aware Computing to
Reduce the Perceived Burden of Interruptions from Mobile Devices.
Proceedings of the SIGCHI Conference on Human Factors in Computing
Systems , ACM, 909–918.
50. Nicholas L. Holt. 2003. Representation, Legitimation, and Autoethnography:
An Autoethnographic Writing Story. I nternational Journal of Qualitative
Methods 2, 1: 18–28.
51. James M. Hudson, Jim Christensen, Wendy A. Kellogg, and Thomas
Erickson. 2002. I’d be overwhelmed, but it's just one more thing to do:
Availability and interruption in research management. P roceedings of the
SIGCHI Conference on Human factors in computing systems , ACM, 97–104.
52. Anthony Jameson. 2009. Adaptive interfaces and agents. Human-Computer
Interaction: Design Issues, Solutions, and Applications 105. Retrieved from
https://books.google.co.uk/books?hl=en&lr=&id=tRVRK8UhuacC&oi=fnd&
pg=PA105&dq=Jameson+Adaptive+Interfaces+And+Agents&ots=DaEojcjM
hN&sig=wEebbXGpJkAKEuVXq3SoWkTL8nE
53. Steve Benford Joel E. Fischer. 2009. Inferring Player Engagement in a
Pervasive Experience. GROUP ...: ] proceedings of the International ACM
SIGCHI Conference on Supporting Group Work. ACM SIGCHI
International Conference on Supporting Group Work . Retrieved August 20,
2015 from
http://citeseerx.ist.psu.edu/viewdoc/download?doi=10.1.1.178.8465&rep=re
p1&type=pdf
54. Stephen Kaplan. 1995. The restorative benefits of nature: Toward an
integrative framework. Journal of environmental psychology 15, 3: 169–182.
55. Melanie Kellar, Carolyn Watters, and Michael Shepherd. 2007. A field study
characterizing Web-based information-seeking tasks. Journal of the
American Society for Information Science and Technology 58, 7: 999–1018.
56. Sam N. Lehman-Wilzig and Michal Seletzky. 2010. Hard news, soft news,
“general” news: The necessity and utility of an intermediate classification.
Journalism 11, 1: 37–56.
57. Catriona Macaulay, David Benyon, and Alison Crerar. 2000. Ethnography,
theory and systems design: from intuition to insight. International journal of
human-computer studies 53, 1: 35–60.
58. Wendy E. Mackay and Anne-Laure Fayard. 1997. HCI, Natural Science and
Design: A Framework for Triangulation Across Disciplines. Proceedings of
the 2Nd Conference on Designing Interactive Systems: Processes, Practices,
Methods, and Techniques , ACM, 223–234.
59. C. Mancini, K. Thomas, Y. Rogers, and B. A. Price. 2009. From spaces to
places: emerging contexts in mobile privacy. Proceedings of the 11th .
Retrieved from http://dl.acm.org/citation.cfm?id=1620547
60. Vicki G. Morwitz, Eric Johnson, and David Schmittlein. 1993. Does
Measuring Intent Change Behavior? The Journal of consumer research 20,
1: 46–61.
61. Kristine S. Nagel, James M. Hudson, and Gregory D. Abowd. 2004.
Predictors of Availability in Home Life Context-mediated Communication.
Proceedings of the 2004 ACM Conference on Computer Supported
Cooperative Work , ACM, 497–506.
62. Nic Newman and David Levy. 2015. The Reuters Institute Digital News
Report 2015: Tracking the Future of News . Reuters Institute for the Study of
Journalism, University of Oxford.
- 93 -
63. David G. Novick, Baltazar Santaella, Aaron Cervantes, and Carlos Andrade.
2012. Short-term Methodology for Long-term Usability. Proceedings of the
30th ACM International Conference on Design of Communication , ACM,
205–212.
64. Stina Nylander, Terés Lundquist, Andreas Brännström, and Bo Karlson.
2009. “It’s Just Easier with the Phone” – A Diary Study of Internet Access
from Cell Phones. In Pervasive Computing . Springer Berlin Heidelberg,
354–371.
65. Ofcom. 2014. News consumption in the UK: 2014 report . Ofcom.
66. Ofcom. 2015. Adults’ media use and attitudes Report 2015.Ofcom.
67. Aisling Ann O’Kane, Yvonne Rogers, and Ann E. Blandford. 2014. Gaining
Empathy for Non-routine Mobile Device Use Through Autoethnography.
Proceedings of the 32Nd Annual ACM Conference on Human Factors in
Computing Systems , ACM, 987–990.
68. Oxford Dictionaries. 2015. Meaning of opportunism. Oxford Dictionaries .
Retrieved September 11, 2015 from
http://www.oxforddictionaries.com/definition/english/opportunism
69. Leysia Palen and Marilyn Salzman. 2002. Voice-mail Diary Studies for
Naturalistic Data Capture Under Mobile Conditions. Proceedings of the 2002
ACM Conference on Computer Supported Cooperative Work , ACM, 87–95.
70. J. Pascoe. 1998. Adding generic contextual capabilities to wearable
computers. Wearable Computers, 1998. Digest of Papers. Second
International Symposium on , ieeexplore.ieee.org, 92–99.
71. Mark Perry, Kenton O’Hara, Abigail Sellen, Barry Brown, and Richard
Harper. 2001. Dealing with Mobility: Understanding Access Anytime,
Anywhere. ACM Trans. Comput. -Hum. Interact. 8, 4: 323–347.
72. Y. Rogers, H. Sharp, and J. Preece. 2011. I nteraction Design: Beyond
Human - Computer Interaction . Wiley.
73. Yvonne Rogers. 2011. Interaction Design Gone Wild: Striving for Wild
Theory. Interactions 18, 4: 58–62.
74. Daniel M. Russell and Ed H. Chi. 2014. Looking Back: Retrospective Study
Methods for HCI. In Ways of Knowing in HCI . Springer New York, 373–393.
75. Bill Schilit, Norman Adams, and Roy Want. 1994. Context-aware computing
applications . IEEE.
76. Phoebe Sengers and Bill Gaver. 2006. Staying open to interpretation:
engaging multiple meanings in design and evaluation. Proceedings of the 6th
conference on Designing Interactive systems , ACM, 99–108.
77. Georg Simmel. 1950. The Isolated Individual and the Dyad. In T he Sociology
of Georg Simmel , Kurt H. Wolff (ed.). Free Press, 118–144.
78. Timothy Sohn, Kevin A. Li, William G. Griswold, and James D. Hollan. 2008.
A Diary Study of Mobile Information Needs. Proceedings of the SIGCHI
Conference on Human Factors in Computing Systems , ACM, 433–442.
79. Anselm Strauss and Juliet M. Corbin. 1990. Basics of Qualitative Research:
Grounded Theory Procedures and Techniques . Sage Publications.
80. L. A. Suchman. 1987. Plans and Situated Actions: The Problem of
Human-Machine Communication . Cambridge University Press.
81. Mozhgan Tavakolifard, Jon Atle Gulla, Kevin C. Almeroth, Jon Espen
Ingvaldesn, Gaute Nygreen, and Erik Berg. 2013. Tailored news in the palm
of your hand: a multi-perspective transparent approach to news
recommendation. P roceedings of the 22nd International Conference on
World Wide Web , International World Wide Web Conferences Steering
- 94 -
Committee, 305–308.
82. Alex S. Taylor. 2009. Ethnography in Ubiquitous Computing. In Ubiquitous
Computing Fundamentals , John Krumm (ed.). Chapman and Hall/CRC,
203–236.
Mobilities
83. J. Urry. 2007. . Wiley.
84. Oscar Westlund. 2008. From Mobile Phone to Mobile Device: News
Consumption on the Go. Canadian Journal of Communication 33, 3.
Retrieved June 26, 2015 from
http://cjc-online.ca/index.php/journal/article/viewArticle/2004
85. L. Wheeler and H. T. Reis. 1991. Self-Recording of Everyday Life Events:
Origins, Types, and Uses. Journal of Personality 59, 3: 339–354.
86. Mikael Wiberg and Fredrik Ljungberg. 1999. Exploring the vision of anytime,
anywhere in the context of mobile work. Knowledge Management and
Virtual Organizations . Retrieved from
http://citeseerx.ist.psu.edu/viewdoc/download?doi=10.1.1.26.9820&rep=re
p1&type=pdf
87. T. Winograd and F. Flores. 1986. Understanding Computers and Cognition:
A New Foundation for Design . Ablex Publishing Corporation.
88. Peter Wright and John McCarthy. 2008. Empathy and Experience in HCI.
Proceedings of the SIGCHI Conference on Human Factors in Computing
Systems , ACM, 637–646.
89. A. D. Yarmey. 1979. The Psychology of Eyewitness Testimony . Free Press.
- 95 -
Appendix 1: Participant Recruitment Advert
- 96 -
Appendix 2: Study Sign-up Form
Created using Google Forms at the following URL:
http://bit.ly/UCLICnews
- 97 -
- 98 -
- 99 -
Appendix 3: Instruction Email
This email confirms your participation in our research study, which is carried out by
researchers at the UCL Interaction Centre in cooperation with the BBC. The study aims
to better understand how people read the news in a variety of daily life situations, using
digital devices such as computers, tablets and smartphones. We hope the insights from
this study will allow us to design smarter, friendlier, and easier to use newsreading
technologies in the future. All participants will receive £10 for completing the study,
and will have chance to win a £50 Amazon voucher*.
Please read the instructions below carefully, as they include important information
about taking part in the study.
We kindly ask that you confirm your participation
with a short reply to this email (see details at the end of this message).
Important! When signingup for Wunderlist, please ensure that you use the same
email address entered when signing up for this study.
- 100 -
After you install and signup to Wunderlist, you will receive a request to join a list
called 'UCLIC News Study'. Please approve this request (this can be done within the
app or by clicking the link you receive from Wunderlist by email). Remember, a ll
information entered by you in the app is seen only by the researchers running this
study, will not be shared, and will be anonymised.
At the end of the study's two week period, we will ask you to take part in a short
interview (2030 minutes), to better understand your experiences during the study. The
interview can take place inperson (a researcher will meet you at your convenience) or
online (via Skype).
We kindly ask that you confirm your participation in the study and your consent for
us to use the information collected during the study, according to the attached informed
consent form. To do so, please reply to this email with the words 'I AGREE'.
Please note: you are receiving this email because you signed up for our newsreading
study at
bit.ly/UCLICnews . You are free to withdraw from this study at any time,
however we kindly ask you to notify us in such an event at yuval.cohen.14@ucl.ac.uk ,
so we can find another participant in your place.
- 101 -
Terms and Conditions
This study has been approved by the UCL Interaction Centre Ethics Chair. Project ID
No: UCLIC/1415/013/Staff+MSc Marshall. By participating in this study you confirm
that you have read the participant information sheet as well as terms and conditions for
payment to participants and the Amazon voucher draw (attached to this email and
available at
http://bit.ly/NewsStudyInfo
), and that you have read and agreed to the
informed consent form attached to this email.
- 102 -
Appendix 4: Sample Exit Interview Schedule
General Questions
- 103 -
How would you characterize your news consumption – in terms of reading
vs. listening vs. watching?
Response-dependent Questions
You noted that you usually read the news first thing in the morning. Is there
a reason for this?
Are there specific times or places when you feel more comfortable to follow
the news? (it’s ok if there aren't)
You mentioned listening to the news while eating dinner or having lunch.
How do you find this experience vs. reading the news in the morning?
concentration, type of news,
(trigger, length of text)
You mentioned that you read a lot on the tube. How do you find this reading
experience compared to other reading experiences (e.g. home)?
(concentration, type of news,
length of text
)
Continuing Re the tube – you mentioned not having connectivity and also
not being able to read because of crowding. Can you elaborate about these
experiences?
You mentioned reading on the train over the weekend (Saturday Guardian,
no WiFi, relaxing). Can you elaborate about this? Specifically – compared
to your reading experience on a weekday?
- 104 -
You noted Facebook, email updates, and notifications on your phone as
(e.g. Guardian) as sources from which you find out about stories. How
much of your newsreading does this make up?
Are there any other sources that inform you about news? (e.g friends or
colleagues) If so – what is the trigger for these types of discussions?
You mentioned being very tired and not wanting to read the news. Is this
something that typically affects your newsreading?
How demanding was it for you? (in terms of time and effort)
- 105 -
Did you feel that you had to answer notifications from the app immediately?
What made you answer a notification immediately vs. leaving it for later?
Were you aware that you could also use the app offline? (without 3G/4G or
Wifi)
Closing
● Thank very much you for participating. your responses will be very
valuable to our research.
● The winner of the £50 Amazon voucher will be announced in September.
● How would you like to receive payment for your participation?
- 106 -
Appendix 5: Participant Information Sheet
Your Participation
Do I have to participate?
- 107 -
You should only participate if you want to. Choosing not to take part will not
disadvantage you in any way. Before you decide whether you want to take
part, please read this information sheet carefully and discuss it with others if
you wish. Ask us if there is anything that is not clear or if you would like more
information.
Please note:
you do not need to be physically present in order to participate,
as all communication and data collection will be done digitally, using your
computer or smartphone.
*
Voucher Drawing – Terms & Conditions
1. One Amazon Gift Voucher at the value of £50 will be given to the
participant whose project ID number will be randomly drawn.
2. There is no cash alternative to the prize.
3. The researcher reserves the right to substitute the prize for an
alternative of the same value.
4. In order to qualify for the prize, participants are expected to complete
all three parts of the study.
5. Participants have the right to withdraw from the study at any time,
however by withdrawing from the study participants forfeit their entry
into the draw.
- 108 -
6. The winner will be contacted via email no later than 14 September
2015.
7. The winner will be selected by members of the research team and staff
from University College London Interaction Centre (UCLIC).
All data will be collected and stored in accordance with the Data Protection Act
1998.
- 109 -
Appendix 6: Informed Consent Form
Title of Project:
Exploring news-reading on digital devices
This study has been approved by the UCL Research Ethics Committee as
Project ID Number: UCLIC/1415/013/Staff+MSc Marshall
Participant’s Statement
I …………………………………………......................................
agree that I have
■ Read the information sheet and/or the project has been explained to
me orally;
■ Had the opportunity to ask questions and discuss the study; and
■ Received satisfactory answers to all my questions or have been
advised of an individual to contact for answers to pertinent questions
about the research and my rights as a participant and whom to contact
in the event of a research-related injury.
■ I understand that my participation will be taped/video recorded, and I
am aware of, and consent to, any use you intend to make of the
recordings after the end of the project.
■ I agree to be contacted in the future by UCL researchers who would
like to invite me to participate in follow-up studies.
■ I understand that the information I have submitted will be published as
a report and I will be sent a copy. Confidentiality and anonymity will be
maintained, and it will not be possible to identify me from any
publications.
- 110 -
Investigator’s Statement
- 111 -