Bender Visual-Motor Gestalt Test in Adolescents1
Bender Visual-Motor Gestalt Test in Adolescents1
Bender Visual-Motor Gestalt Test in Adolescents1
net/publication/259723124
CITATIONS READS
9 6,977
3 authors, including:
Some of the authors of this publication are also working on these related projects:
All content following this page was uploaded by Cintra Isa de Pádua on 09 January 2015.
1
Address correspondence to Teresa Helena Schoen, Centro de Atendimento e Apoio ao Ado-
lescente, Universidade Federal de São Paulo, Rua Botucatu, 715, Vila Clementino, São Paulo,
Brasil CEP 04023-062 or e-mail (rpetrass@uol.com.br).
depends on the age at which sexual hormones begin to affect the brain, as
girls who have their menarche at an early age perform better on verbal tasks
than those who have their menarche later. However, members of the latter
group have better spatial skills. Physical changes due to puberty may inter-
fere with the visual-motor ability of adolescents, because puberty is marked
by nonlinear skeletal growth (which also requires an acceleration and a
deceleration period) and can result in a disproportionate body (Colli, et al.,
2003). It also seems that adolescents lose some ability to coordinate vision
with their body movements (Kolb & Whishaw, 2002; Colli, et al., 2003).
Because of the evidence of the effects of puberty (Colli, et al., 2003), the
resulting possible changes in some cognitive functions (Kolb & Whishaw,
2002; Machado, et al., 2005), and the implications of visual-motor matura-
tion after childhood (Shapiro & Simpson, 1995; Bolen, 2003; Decker, 2008),
the goal was to assess whether changes in visual-motor skills take place
precisely at the time of puberty, this being the time that Bender (1938,
1985) considered visual-motor maturation to be fully developed.
Hypotheses. Visual-motor development, as measured by the
Bender test, will be correlated with sexual maturity in ado-
lescents (small effect size), based on the Tanner Stages, by
using the methodologies of (a) testing associations between
the number of errors committed on the Bender test and demo-
graphic variables (gender, age, and schooling); (b) comparing
the results of the Bender test with the results of Raven's Pro-
gressive Matrices Test; (c) testing the association between the
results of the Bender test and the Tanner maturity stages; and
(d) identifying neurological difficulties associated with matu-
rity that may affect Bender test results.
METHOD
The present descriptive, cross-sectional study received approval from
the Human Research Ethics Committee of the Federal University of Sao
Paulo (Brazil) under process number 842/10.
Participants
Male and female adolescents (N = 134), 10 to 15 years of age (early ado-
lescence: 10 to 12 years; middle adolescence: 13 to 15 years), participated in
the study. They were recruited among patients who had a medical appoint-
ment in the Adolescent Sector of the Department of Pediatrics at the Federal
University of Sao Paulo (Brazil) on the day of the administration of measures.
Measures
Bender Visual-Motor Gestalt Test (Bender test).—This test consists of pre-
senting nine geometric figures printed on cards measuring 10 cm ⫻ 15 cm
6 L. DE F. KEPPEKE, ET AL.
that are to be copied with a B2 pencil onto a sheet of A4 paper without the
assistance of any type of mechanical device. The test lasts 7 to 10 minutes.
Scores are based on the number of errors committed, so lower scores denote
better visual-motor coordination. The test was corrected according to the
instructions proposed by Koppitz (1989), with a possible score of 0 to 30
errors. A standardization study of the Koppitz System involved 1,104 chil-
dren of both sexes, aged 5 years zero months to 10 years 11 months, and
the nine figures were applied individually. All children were from public
schools, from Kindergarten to Grade 5. Among the participants were chil-
dren with emotional difficulties, learning disabilities, brain damage, and
mental retardation. To evaluate reliability, the method of test-retest at inter-
vals of 4 months was used, and the results of the Kendall Degrees correla-
tion coefficients were between .55 and .66, which was statistically significant
(p < .001). Validation was performed by comparing each item with perfor-
mance in the 1st and 2nd degrees as measured by the Metropolitan Readi-
ness Test. One hundred sixty-five students participated; 99 were from the
first degree (59 were above average and 40 were below average) and 66
were from the second degree (45 were above average and 21 were below
average). The chi-square tests were statistically significant (p < .001) (Kop-
pitz, 1989). The results indicate adequacy of the test for children ages 6 to
10 years.
Raven's Progressive Matrices Test.—This is a non-verbal intelligence
test that assesses the g factor and is designed for individuals aged 13 years
and older. The test is made up of five sets (A, B, C, D, and E) arranged in
increasing order of difficulty, with a total of 60 items. The individual is
instructed to complete the part of the drawing that is missing by choos-
ing from among the items at the bottom of the page. A point is awarded
for each correct response. The total score is a simple sum of the scores on
each problem and ranges from 0 to 60 points. In the present study, because
some of the participants were less than 13 years of age, the results were
expressed as (a) an unadjusted result, and (b) an age-adjusted result. The
test-retest reliability of this instrument ranges from .83 to .93, depending
on age. The correlation with the Terman-Merril scale was .86. Raven (2000)
established a saturation of .82 on factor g.
Demographic chart.—Data on variables affecting visual development
were collected from the medical charts of the participants according to
the literature, as follows: (a) Tanner stage; (b) developmental conditions
(mental retardation, dyslexia, attention deficit disorder and hyperactiv-
ity, global learning disability, global developmental disability, epilepsy,
Kozlowski syndrome, arthrogryposis multiplex congenita, growth hor-
mone deficiency, and myelomeningocele); (c) age; (d) gender; and (e)
schooling, including having repeated a school year. Determination of sex-
VISUAL-MOTOR SKILLS IN ADOLESCENTS 7
ual maturation stage was routine for medical appointments at the clinic.
The childhood stages of development are G1 (gonads), M1 (mammary
glands), and P1 (pubic hair). Girls were considered to be in the growth
spurt phase when they exhibited the stages of P2 to P4 and M2 to M4 (the
emergence of long, downy hair to adult-type hair that has not yet reached
the inner face of the thigh, and an increase in areola diameter under the
formation of the second mound above the contour of the breast). Boys
were considered to be in the growth spurt phase when they exhibited the
stages of P3 to P4 and G3 to G4 (emergence of long, downy hair to adult-
type hair that has not yet reached the inner face of the thigh, and an initial
increase in testicular volume and in penile diameter). Girls in P5 and M5
and boys in P5 and G5 have the sexual characteristics of adults.
Procedure
After authorization by the head of the clinic, medical appointment
hours for potential study candidates were identified. Adolescents in the
waiting room were invited to participate in the study. After an explana-
tion of the project, the adolescents and parents/guardians who agreed to
participate signed a statement of informed consent. In a separate, quiet
room, the participants individually copied the figures on the Bender test
and took Raven's Progressive Matrices. Each participant took the tests
during a single session. After the participants attended their medical
appointment and took the tests, their medical charts were analyzed and
the study's demographic charts were filled out. The data were organized
on spreadsheets and analyzed.
Analysis
The Kolmogorov-Smirnov Test was employed to assess the normal-
ity of the data. The chi-square test, analysis of variance (ANOVA), and
the Pearson's correlations were used to compare the results of the Bender
test, and the other variables analyzed (Tanner stage, age, and Raven's test
results). Alpha was set to .05. “Power and Sample Size” on Minitab V16
was used to analyze the data, and the power of the sample to detect dif-
ferences was 92.22%.
RESULTS
The majority of participants in the present study were male. The mean
age of the overall sample was 12.7 yr. (SD = 1.6). Middle school (Grades
6 to 9) was the predominant level of schooling. Most of the participants
had never repeated a school year, and a small percentage exhibited devel-
opmental disorders. The P5, G1, and M5 Tanner stages predominated.
No female participants were in stages M1 or M2 (Table 1). In the ANOVA
analyses, in which all participants were included, the degrees of freedom
8 L. DE F. KEPPEKE, ET AL.
TABLE 1
DISTRIBUTION OF SAMPLE BY GENDER, AGE, SCHOOLING, DEVELOPMENTAL DISORDER,
AND SEXUAL MATURATION STAGE
Variable n %
Gender Male 76 56.7
Female 58 43.3
Age, yr. 10 22 16.4
11 8 6.0
12 24 17.9
13 33 24.6
14 29 21.7
15 18 13.4
Schooling Elementary1 28 20.9
Middle2 93 69.4
HS3 13 9.7
Repeated school Yes 30 22.4
year No 104 77.6
Developmental Present 18 15.8
disorders Absent 96 84.2
Tanner P1 27 20.2
P2 14 10.4
P3 18 13.4
P4 30 22.4
P5 45 33.6
G1 23 30.7
G2 12 16.0
G3 19 25.3
G4 11 14.7
G5 10 13.3
M3 5 8.9
M4 21 37.5
M5 30 53.6
1st to 5th grades; 26th to 9th grades; 3high school (HS).
1
(df ) were 133; for females in their growth spurt, df = 57, and for Tanner M,
df = 55; for males in their growth spurt, for Tanner G, df = 74.
The results of the Bender test were analyzed using descriptive sta-
tistics for the treatment of the data. Table 2 displays the descriptive sta-
tistics and F ratios as well as the statistical significance for the results of
the Bender test according to gender and age. The literature on the Bender
indicates no difference between the sexes, but the number of errors that
VISUAL-MOTOR SKILLS IN ADOLESCENTS 9
TABLE 2
BENDER TEST SCORES ACCORDING TO GENDER AND AGE
Variable n Min Max M SD F p ES
Gender Male 76 0 12 2.59 2.87
0.69 .41 .002
Female 58 0 12 3.03 3.28
Age, yr. 10 22 0 8 2.68 2.66
11 8 0 7 3.50 2.44
12 24 0 8 2.33 2.23
1.39 .55 .014
13 33 0 9 2.03 2.35
14 29 0 12 3.90 4.21
15 18 0 12 2.78 3.42
the Bender test generates diminishes with age. In this sample, there were
no significant differences between the sexes, nor by age.
Table 3 displays the ANOVA results for the Bender scores according
to level of schooling and having or not having repeated a school year. A
higher level of education was expected to decrease the number of errors on
the Bender test; adolescents with learning disabilities, here measured by
grade repetition, would have higher scores on the Bender test. The results
did not indicate a statistically significant improvement in performance on
the Bender test with regard to schooling level. In contrast, having repeated
a school year resulted in statistically significantly worse performance in
comparison to those who did not repeat a school year.
Table 4 displays the Bender test scores in relation to developmental
disorders and sexual maturation stage. It was hypothesized that adoles-
cents with organic developmental problems would commit more errors
on the Bender test. The adolescents with developmental disorders per-
formed statistically significantly less well on the Bender test in compar-
ison to those without them. The main hypothesis of this study was that
puberty, and specifically the growth spurt, influences visual-motor abil-
ity. However, the changes stemming from puberty and related to the
growth spurt (according to Tanner's Stages), such as the development of
TABLE 3
BENDER TEST SCORES ACCORDING TO ACADEMIC LEVEL
Variable n Min Max M SD F p ES
Elementary1 28 0 12 3.4 3.5
Schooling Middle2 93 0 12 2.8 3.1 1.95 .15 .014
HS3 13 0 4 1.4 1.3
Repeated year 30 0 12 4.5 4.1
12.75 < .001* .081
Did not repeat year 104 0 12 2.3 2.5
1
1st to 5th years; 26th to 9th years; 3high school (HS). *p < .05.
10 L. DE F. KEPPEKE, ET AL.
TABLE 4
BENDER TEST SCORES ACCORDING TO DEVELOPMENTAL DISORDERS AND SEXUAL MATURATION STAGE
Variable n Min Max M SD F p ES
Developmental Present 18 0 12 5.4 4.6
disorder 17.03 <.001* .148
Absent 96 0 11 2.3 2.6
Tanner P1 27 0 9 2.9 2.8
P2 14 0 12 2.3 3.5
P3 18 0 12 4.2 3.3 1.78 .14 .023
P4 30 0 9 1.9 2.4
P5 45 0 12 2.9 3.2
G1 23 0 8 2.6 2.6
G2 12 0 8 2.3 2.7
G3 19 0 12 3.2 2.9 0.69 .60 .017
G4 11 0 9 1.6 2.8
G5 10 0 8 1.8 3.0
M3 6 0 12 5.0 5.1
M4 20 0 7 2.5 2.1 1.21 .31 .003
M5 30 0 12 3.3 3.7
Female growth Spurt 28 0 12 3.1 3.1
spurt 0.02 .89 .018
Post-spurt 30 0 12 3.3 3.7
Male growth Pre-spurt 35 0 8 2.5 2.6
spurt Spurt 30 0 12 2.6 2.9 0.32 .73 .019
Post-spurt 10 0 8 1.8 3.0
*p ≤ .05.
the gonads, breasts, and pubic hair growth, were not associated with the
mean Bender test scores. Sexual maturity did not appear to affect visual-
motor skills in the present sample.
The variables that exhibited statistically significant differences among
the participants with regard to the results of the Bender test were hav-
ing repeated a school year and having developmental disabilities. A chi-
square test was then applied to these variables, which demonstrated a
statistically significant association between these variables (X2 = 7.82,
p = .005). The proportion of participants with developmental disabilities
was greater among those having repeated a school year (40%) in compari-
son to those not having repeated a school year (14.6%).
Pearson's correlation was used to assess the relationship between the
Bender and Raven's tests (unadjusted results and results adjusted for age).
The authors had hypothesized that higher results on Raven's test would
be associated with lower results on the Bender test. Figure 1 shows that
the correlation between these measures was negative. The results of the
VISUAL-MOTOR SKILLS IN ADOLESCENTS 11
12
(r = –.473)* (r = –.468)*
9
Bender
20 40 60 5 10 15 20
Raven Adjusted Raven
FIG. 1. Correlation between Bender and Raven tests. Raven score and adjusted-for-age
Raven score show negative correlation with Bender score. *p < .001.
Bender test are based on the number of errors, and the Raven's test results
are based on the number of correct answers, so the inverse association is
logical. The correlation between the two tests was statistically significant
(ps < .001) for both the unadjusted Raven's test score (r = –.47) and the
Raven's test score controlled for age (r = –.47).
DISCUSSION
This study attempted to assess whether there is a correlation between
visual-motor development, as measured by the Bender test, and sexual
maturity in adolescents, based on the Tanner Stages. It also sought to
investigate other variables (gender, age, schooling, intelligence, and devel-
opmental disabilities) that may have contributed to or interfered with the
purposes of the study.
With a duration of approximately 2 to 4 years (Lourenço & Queiroz,
2010), puberty, and especially the growth spurt, manifests itself in ado-
lescents who are 10 to 15 years old. Therefore, if it is possible to detect
visual-motor changes in adolescence, this 5-year period should be suffi-
cient for the study. Since the acceleration and deceleration of musculoskel-
etal growth occurs concomitantly during sexual maturation (Lourenço &
Queiroz, 2010), the study used the Tanner stages to analyze the influence
that sex hormones and the growth spurt could have on visual-motor skills.
Visual-motor changes could occur in the Tanner stages M2, M3 (girls), G3,
and G4 (boys), when the adolescent growth spurt generally occurs (Colli,
et al., 2003; Saito, et al., 2008).
As expected, no prepubescent girls were found in the sample, as
female development occurs earlier than male development, beginning on
average at 8 or 9 years of age (Monte, Longui, & Calliari, 2001; Papalia,
et al., 2001; Lourenço & Queiroz, 2010). A large proportion of the adoles-
cents in the study were still in elementary school due to having repeated
school years, and they therefore exhibited delays in age-grade relation.
12 L. DE F. KEPPEKE, ET AL.
in mind that the present study only investigated visual-motor skills and
inferred the action of hormones through secondary sexual characteristics
and the growth spurt. Is important to emphasize the small number of par-
ticipants in this study, who were also classified into subgroups according
to the Tanner stages, generating small groups. Studies with similar popula-
tions and with a larger number of participants in each pubertal stage would
provide more reliable results, including the validation of the evidence by
others.
This study's results also disagreed with Decker (2008), who suggested
a rapid visual-motor maturation during the middle period of adolescence,
13 to 15 years old, and with Shapiro and Simpson (1995) and Bolen (2003),
who also found evidence of visual-motor development after 11 years. The
current study accords with Lauretta Bender's statement that at approxi-
mately 11 years of age, visual-motor maturity becomes fully developed
(Bender, 1938, 1985). Most studies involving adolescents seem to corrobo-
rate this finding. In the current sample, visual-motor maturation appeared
to be stabilized before sexual maturity (or at least at the beginning of
it) and before other alterations resulting from puberty, and this did not
change during the growth spurt, even during the period of the rapidly
changing relationship between the upper limbs and trunk that results in
transient motor incoordination (Papalia, et al., 2001; Colli, et al., 2003). The
coordination of vision with fine motor movement that is required in the
transcription of the figures seems not have been affected by the rapid, dis-
proportionate growth that occurs during the spurt.
As reported in previous studies (Rueda, et al., 2006; Bartholomeu &
Sisto, 2008), Bender scores were correlated with Raven's test. Although
the two tests evaluate different constructs, there is a relationship between
the general factor of intelligence and visual-motor organization in ado-
lescents. Shapiro and Simpson (1995) and Di Blasi, et al. (2007) found the
same association in adolescents, indicating that it is not restricted to child-
hood, although the Bender test is standardized in most countries for this
period of development. Therefore, the ability to perform complex men-
tal operations such as logical and abstract reasoning is affected by visual-
motor development, and it is possible to verify this association using the
Bender test during adolescence, also. The results of this study suggest that
the Bender test, when corrected with the Koppitz system, contains visual
and spatial components related to those found in Raven's test. The same
visual-motor skills are likely required for both tests. It is possible that spa-
tial abilities and cognitive performance may have a similar basis in infor-
mation processing (Di Blasi, et al., 2007).
It is notable that previous studies finding statistically significant
correlations between visual-motor maturity and intelligence reported
VISUAL-MOTOR SKILLS IN ADOLESCENTS 15
hypothesis of this study. However, there were adolescents who had errors
in copying stimulus cards. Although the Bender test was designed for
children up to 11 years of age, it proved sensitive to developmental dis-
abilities in both early (10 to 12 years of age) and middle (13 to 15 years of
age) adolescence. Because developmental disturbances seem to affect per-
formance on the Bender test among adolescents and because a correlation
between intelligence and visual-motor skills was observed in adolescence,
further studies with a larger sample are needed to verify the possibility
using this instrument in age groups of participants who are older than
those used in the current, standardized testing regime, including sample
populations of individuals with different types of pathologies.
REFERENCES
ALLEN, R. A., & DECKER, S. L. (2008) Utility of the Bender Visual-motor Gestalt Test – sec-
ond edition in the assessment of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Percep-
tual & Motor Skills, 107(3), 663-675.
ARAUJO, R. B., PRACOWNIK, A., & SOARES, L. S. D. (1999) Trabalhando a coordenação motora
[Working motor coordination]. Carapicuíba, Brazil: Pró-fono. [in Portuguese]
ARCHER, R. P., & NEWSOM, C. R. (2000) Psychological test usage with adolescent clients:
survey update. Assessment, 7, 227-235.
BARTHOLOMEU, D., & SISTO, F. F. (2008) Visual-motor maturity and intelligence: a correla-
tional study. Psicologia: Ciência e Profissão, 28, 362-373. [in Portuguese]
BENDER, L. (1938) A visual motor Gestalt test and its clinical use. Research Monographs,
American Orthopsychiatric Association, n3(xi), 176.
BENDER, L. (1985) Test guestaltico visomotor: usos y aplicaciones clínicas [Visual-motor Gestalt
Test: clinical uses and applications]. Vol. 10. Buenos Aires: Paidós Iberica Ediciones,
SA. [in Spanish]
BÖHM, B., LUNDEQUIST, A., & SMEDLER, A. C. (2010) Visual-motor and executive functions
in children born preterm: the Bender Visual-motor Gestalt Test revisited. Scandi-
navian Journal of Psychology, 51(5), 376-384.
BOLEN, L. M. (2003) Constructing local age norms based on ability for the Bender-Gestalt
Test. Perceptual & Motor Skills, 97(2), 467-476.
BRAUN, C. I., FREIDLIN, V., SPERDUTO, R. D., MILTON, R. C., & STRAHLMAN, E. R. (1996) The
progression of myopia in school age children: data from the Columbia Medical
Plan. Ophthalmic Epidemiology, 3(1), 13-21.
CHAUDHARI, S., OTIV, M., CHITALE, A., PANDIT, A., & HOGE, M. (2004) Pune Low Birth
Weight Study: cognitive abilities and educational performance at twelve years.
Indian Pediatrics, 41(2), 121-128. [in English]
CHIPKEVITCH, E. (2001) Clinical assessment of sexual maturation in adolescents. Jornal de
Pediatria, 77(2), 135-142. [in Portuguese]
CLAWSON, A. (1992) Bender infantil: Manual de Diagnóstico Clínico [Bender child: manual of
clinical diagnosis]. Porto Alegre, Portugal: Artes Médicas. [in Portuguese]
COLLI, A. S., COATES, V., & GUIMARÃES, E. M. B. (2003) Monitorização do crescimento
e desenvolvimento físico [Monitoring growth and physical development]. In V.
Coates, G. W. Beznos, & L. A. Françoso (Eds.), Medicina do adolescente. São Paulo:
Sarvier. Pp. 66-73. [in Portuguese]
VISUAL-MOTOR SKILLS IN ADOLESCENTS 17
COUSINO, L., & WILDER, H. (1978) Lafunción viso-motora en niños de Santiago de Chile
[Visual-motor function in children of Santiago de Chile]. Revista Latinoamericana de
Psicologia, 10(3), 363-375. [in Spanish]
CUNHA, C. A., & MACHADO, F. (2006) Reconhecimento de palavras e a maturação per-
ceptomotora num grupo de alunos do Ensino Fundamental [Word recognition
and perceptual motor maturation in a group of elementary school students]. In C.
Machado, L.S. Almeida, M. Golçalves, & V. Ramalho (Eds.), XI Conferência Interna-
cional: avaliação psicológica: formas e contextos. [International Conference: psychological
evaluation: forms and contexts]. Braga, Portugal: Psiquilíbrios Edições. Pp. 200-220.
[in Portuguese]
DECKER, S. L. (2008) Measuring growth and decline in visual-motor processes with the
Bender-Gestalt second edition. Journal of Psychoeducational Assessment, 26(1), 3-15.
DI BLASI, F. D., ELIA, F., BUONO, S., RAMAKERS, G. J., & DI NUOVO, S. F. (2007) Relationships
between visual-motor and cognitive abilities in intellectual disabilities. Perceptual
& Motor Skills, 104(3), 763-772.
FERNÁNDEZ, T., & TUSET, A. M. (2007) Bender performance and socioeconomic status in
Mexican children: a cross-cultural study. Perceptual & Motor Skills, 105(3), 906-914.
FERREIRA, R. B., FEIL, C. F., & NUNES, M. L. T. (2009) Bender Visual-motor Gestalt Test in
the children's clinical assessment. Psico-USF, 14(2), 185-192. [in Portuguese]
GEMIGNANI, E. Y. M. Y., & CHIARI, B. M. (2000) The Koppitz Developmental Bender Test
scoring system in a deaf children group. Pró-fono, 12(2), 49-53. [in Portuguese]
GHASSEMZADEH, H., TAHVILDAR, M., REZAI, M., & DANEKAR, M. (2004) Rendimiento de una
muestra de niños Iraníes entre 5 y 11 años en el Test Bender-Gestalt, Matrices de
Raven y una tarea de razonamiento inferencial [Performance of a sample of Ira-
nian children between 5 and 11 years in the Bender-Gestalt Test, Raven's Matrices,
and inferential reasoning task]. Boletín de Psicología, 81, 89-112. [in Spanish]
HAMID, N., & GHAFARI, M. (2009) An investigation of the brain-damaged patient's func-
tion in the Bender Visual-motor Gestalt Test in comparison with their brain MRI
portraits and normal subjects. Scientific Medical Journal, 8(61), 185-191.
JOESTING, J. (1977) Correlations of scores on Bender Visual-motor Gestalt Test and WISC–
R. Perceptual & Motor Skills, 45, 980.
KOLB, B., & WHISHAW, I. Q. (2002) Neurociência do comportamento [Behavioral neuroscience].
São Paulo: Manole. [in Portuguese]
KOPPITZ, E. M. (1968) El test gestáltico visual-motor para niños [The Visual-motor Gestalt test
for children]. Buenos Aires: Guadalupe. [in Spanish]
KOPPITZ, E. M. (1989) O Teste Gestáltico Bender para crianças [The Bender Gestalt Test for chil-
dren]. R. N. Piccoli, Transl. Porto Alegre, Portugal: Artes Médicas. [in Portuguese]
LACKS, P. (1999) Bender Gestalt screening for brain dysfunction. (2nd ed.) New York: Wiley.
LOURENÇO, B., & QUEIROZ, L. B. (2010) Growth and puberal development in adolescence.
Revista de Medicina, 89(2), 70-75. [in Portuguese]
MACHADO, D. C. D., BASTOS, V. H. V., SILVA, P. A. P., ANDRADE, U. F., SILVA, J. G., FURTADO, V.,
& REBEIRO, P. (2005). Encephalic sexual diferences and attention levels in men and
women. Fitness & Performance Journal, 4(4), 232-235.
MONTE, O., LONGUI, C. A., & CALLIARI, L. E. P. (2001) Puberdade precoce: dilemas no
diagnóstico e tratamento [Precocious puberty: dilemmas in diagnosis and treat-
ment]. Arquivos Brasileiros de Endocrinologia & Metabologia, 45(4), 321-330. [in Por-
tuguese]
18 L. DE F. KEPPEKE, ET AL.
MURAYAMA, N., ISEKI, E., YAMAMOTO, R., KIMURA, M., ETO, K., & ARAI, H. (2007) Utility of
the Bender-Gestalt Test for differentiation of dementia with Lewy bodies from
Alzheimer's disease in patients showing mild to moderate dementia. Dementia and
Geriatric Cognitive Disorders, 23(4), 258-263.
NIELSON, S., & SAPP, G. L. (1991) Bender-Gestalt developmental scores: predicting read-
ing and mathematics achievement. Psychological Reports, 69(1), 39-42.
NORONHA, A. P. P., BERALDO, F. N. M., & OLIVEIRA, K. L. (2003) Well-know psychological
instruments used by psychology students and professionals. Psicologia Escolar e
Educacional, 7(1), 47-56. [in Portuguese]
NORONHA, A. P. P., SANTOS, A. A. A., & SISTO, F. F. (2007) Evidences of validity of Bender-
gradual scoring system (B–SPG). Psicologia: Reflexão e Crítica, 20(2), 335-341. [in
Portuguese]
NUNES, M. L. T., FERREIRA, R. B., & LOPES, F. (2007) Evaluation's systems and interpreta-
tion of the Bender Visual-motor Gestalt Test. Psic: Revista da Vetor Editora, 8(1),
41-49. [in Portuguese]
OLIVEIRA, M. S. (1985) O Teste de Bender e o Desenho da Figura Humana como predi-
tores do rendimento escolar [The Bender test and the Draw-a-person test as
predictors of the school performance]. Unpublished master's thesis, Pontifícia
Universidade Católica, Porto Alegre, Brazil.
Özer, S. (2011) Turkish children's Bender-Gestalt Test performance: differences in public
and private school children. Psychological Reports, 108(1), 169-181.
PACANARO, S. V., SANTOS, A. A. A. d., & SUEHIRO, A. C. B. (2008) Assessment of cognitive
and visuomotor habilities in people with Down Syndrome. Revista Brasileira de
Educação Especial, 14(2), 311-326. [in Portuguese]
PAPALIA, D. E., OLDS, S. W., & FELDMAN, R. D. (2001) Human development. (8th ed.) Porto
Alegre: ArtMed. [in Portuguese]
PICQ, L., & VAYER, P. (1988) Educação psicomotora e retardo mental [Psychomotor education
and mental retardation]. (5th ed.) São Paulo: Manole. [in Portuguese]
PINTO, L. P., & NORONHA, A. P. P. (2010) Maturidade perceptomotora e sua relação com
idade e variáveis contextuais: um estudo com o Bender (B–SPG) [Maturity per-
ceptual motor and its relation to age and contextual variables: a study of the
Bender (B–SPG)]. Encontro: Revista de Psicologia, 13(19), 145-155. [in Portuguese]
RAJABI, G. (2009) Normalizing the Bender Visual-motor Gestalt Test among 6-10 year-
old children. Journal of Applied Sciences, 9(6), 1165-1169.
RAVEN, J. C. (2000) Teste das Matrizes Progressivas: Escala Geral [Progressive Matrices Test:
general scale]. Rio de Janeiro: CEPA. [in Portuguese]
ROMI, S., & MAROM, D. (2005) Dropout delinquent adolescents: psycho-diagnostic
aspects of using the Bender-Gestalt Test and WISC–R. International Journal of Child
& Family Welfare, 8(4), 164-176. [in English]
RUEDA, F. J. M., BARTHOLOMEU, D., & SISTO, F. F. (2006) Perceptual-motor maturity and
intelligence. Psicologia: Ciência e Profissão, 26(3), 490-503. [in Portuguese]
SAITO, M. I., SILVA, L. E. V., & LEAL, M. M. (2008) Adolescência: prevenção e risco [Adoles-
cence: prevention and risk]. (2nd ed.) São Paulo: Atheneu. [in Portuguese]
SANGHAVI, R., & KELKAR, R. (2005) Visual motor integration and learning disabled chil-
dren. The Indian Journal of Occupational Therapy, 37(2), 33-32. [in English]
SANTOS, A. A. A., & JORGE, L. M. (2007) Bender Test with dyslexics: comparison of two
systems of punctuation. Psico–USF, 12(1), 13-21. [in Portuguese]
VISUAL-MOTOR SKILLS IN ADOLESCENTS 19