[go: up one dir, main page]

Academia.eduAcademia.edu
Perception, 2013, volume 42, pages 187 – 197 doi:10.1068/p6909 Preferences for facial and vocal masculinity in homosexual men: the role of relationship status, sexual restrictiveness, and self-perceived masculinity Jaroslava Valentová1, S Craig Roberts2, Jan Havlíček3 1 Center for Theoretical Study, Charles University and the Academy of Sciences of the Czech Republic, Jilska 1, 11000 Prague, Czech Republic; e-mail: valentova@cts.cuni.cz; 2 School of Natural Sciences, University of Stirling, Stirling, Scotland, United Kingdom; 3 Department of Zoology, Faculty of Science, Charles University, Prague, Czech Republic Received 18 January 2011, in revised form 16 January 2013 Abstract. Studies on mate preferences usually examine heterosexual attraction; comparatively little is known about preferences of individuals whose sexuality is aimed at the same sex. We examined preferences of two groups of androphilic individuals—homosexual men and heterosexual women— for male facial and vocal level of masculinity. Facial images of 58 men and vocal recordings of 30 men were rated by 51 heterosexual women and 33 homosexual men for their attractiveness and masculinity– femininity. In both groups of raters, ratings of vocal attractiveness and masculinity were positively correlated, but there was no overall preference for facial masculinity. After splitting raters according to their relationship status, sexual restrictiveness, and self-rated masculinity, we found signiicant preferences for masculine voices only in single homosexual men and coupled heterosexual women, while a preference for feminine male faces was found in coupled homosexual men. Furthermore, homosexual men describing themselves as relatively masculine signiicantly preferred masculine voices but also more feminine male faces. Our results demonstrate that conditional mate preferences are not restricted to heterosexual interactions, and homosexual men prefer a mixture of masculine and feminine traits in their potential male partners. Keywords: attractiveness, face, voice, sexual orientation 1 Introduction A large body of recent empirical research has focused on aspects of human physical attractiveness and its social and evolutionary implications. Although it is only part of the broader picture, physical appearance undoubtedly plays a key role in mate choice decisions, with signiicant sex differences in preferences for different traits which, more often than not, are robust across different cultures (Andersson and Iwasa 1996). In addition to characteristics such as symmetry, averageness, and adiposity, sex-typical physical traits are perceived as attractive by individuals of the opposite sex (Perrett et al 1998; Rhodes et al 2000). Expression of these physical sex-typical traits are thought to be mediated by sex hormone action during prenatal and early childhood, and/or at pubertal development (Johnston et al 2001), and to reliably indicate an individual’s health, genetic quality and immunocompetence (Folstad and Karter 1992; Gangestad and Thornhill 2003; Yeo et al 1994; review in Roberts and Little 2008). 1.1 The effect of male sex-typical vocal and facial traits in opposite-sex preferences One of the sexually dimorphic traits which develops under the inluence of pubertal testosterone is voice pitch (a perceptual correlate of fundamental frequency). On average, adult men produce lower-pitched voices than women and children (Hollien et al 1994). The development of preferences for low voice pitch is linked to pubertal changes and only becomes fully developed as adulthood approaches and mate choice decisions become relevant (Saxton et al 2006, 2009b, 2009c). Lower-pitched male voices are rated as masculine and attractive by female raters (Berry 1992; Collins 2000; Feinberg et al 2005a, 188 J Valentová, S C Roberts, J Havlíček 2005b; Hodges-Simeon et al 2010; Oguchi and Kikuchi 1997; Puts 2005; Riding et al 2006; Zuckermann et al 1995), are associated with perceived maturity of their owners (Childers and Wu 1991; Wu and Childers 1991), perceived muscularity, age, and weight (Collins 2000), actual body size (Evans et al 2006, 2008; Puts et al 2012, Zuckermann et al 1995), and actual level of testosterone (Dabbs and Mallinger 1999). Low voice pitch is also linked to self-reported number of sexual partners (Puts 2005), and self-reported reproductive success (Apicella et al 2007). Moreover, attractiveness of male voices predicted sexual behaviour in males, such as lower age of irst sexual intercourse and higher number of extra-pair sexual relationships (Hughes et al 2004). These relationships may arise as a result of selection pressures on male vocal display (Puts et al 2006, 2012), such that higher reproductive success of men with lower-pitched voices relect female preferences for male dominance (Wells et al 2009). Another extensively studied domain of physical attractiveness is facial appearance. As with voices, since human faces display sex-dimorphic morphological traits, such as larger jaws and prominent eyebrow ridges in men, it has been suggested that male facial masculinity is related to attractiveness and mating success (Johnston et al 2001), and sociosexuality in men (Boothroyd et al 2008). Although preferences for sex-typical traits occur across cultures and are thus robust, there is nonetheless substantial individual variation in strength of preference for such traits, which has been particularly well-described in the face literature. Positive relationships have been found between rated facial masculinity in males and attractiveness (Perrett et al 1998), their actual health (Rhodes et al 2003), symmetry (Little et al 2008), and level of salivary testosterone (Penton-Voak and Chen 2004). Relative strength of women’s preferences for male traits are context-dependent, ie being inluenced by perception of females’ own attractiveness (Little et al 2001), phase of menstrual cycle (Jones et al 2008), relationship status (Little et al 2002), or sexual restrictiveness (Burt et al 2007). Overall, it has been shown that women who perceive themselves as more attractive, coupled women, those who seek short-term relationships, and those who are less sexually restricted, show stronger preferences for male facial masculinity. 1.2 Effect of male masculinity–femininity in same-sex preferences In contrast with the extensive investigations of heterosexual partner preferences (Barber 1995; Enquist et al 2002; Penton-Voak and Perrett 2000; Rhodes 2006; Roberts and Little 2008), there is very limited research on partner preferences of homosexual individuals. It has been reported that homosexual men show male-typical mating psychology, including interest in casual sex and visual sexually explicit material (Bailey et al 1994); they also report maletypical mate retention behaviour (Vanderlaan and Vasey 2008), prefer potential partners who are younger than themselves (Hayes 2001; Silverthorne and Quinsey 2000) and, again similarly to heterosexual men, value physical attractiveness in their potential partners more than heterosexual women (Howard et al 1987). On the other hand, homosexual men prefer as partners men who describe themselves as rather masculine, and this is dependent on their own level of masculinity (Bailey et al 1997; Muscarella 2002). This is further supported by a recent study that found that homosexual men prefer digitally masculinised male facial pictures over feminised pictures (Glassenberg et al 2010). Except for the last-mentioned study, the previous research on partner preferences of homosexual men is methodologically limited to questionnaire data or data based on personal advertisements. To investigate preferences for masculinity–femininity of homosexual men in more detail, in this study, we use non-manipulated male facial pictures and vocal recordings. Preferences for facial and vocal masculinity in homosexual men 189 1.3 Aims of the current study The main aim of the current study was to investigate preferences for male facial and vocal masculinity in androphilic individuals, ie men and women sexually attracted to men (thus, we intentionally did not include ratings of heterosexual men and lesbian women, since they are not sexually attracted to men), and to test effects of potentially modulating variables. In line with the aforementioned indings, we expect that lower-pitched male voices perceived as masculine will in general be rated as attractive by both homosexual male and heterosexual female raters, and because of individual condition-dependence, that there is no general overall preference for facial masculinity. Based on indings described above for heterosexual preferences, we test for possible inluence of relationship status, sexual restrictiveness, and self-perceived masculinity on preferences for both facial and vocal masculinity in homosexual males and heterosexual females. 2 Materials and methods 2.1 Participants Facial photos and vocal recordings of 61 men (targets; mean age 23.3 years; SD 3.82 years; range 18–35 years) were obtained for this study; 27 men were identiied as homosexual, and 34 as heterosexual. There were no signiicant differences between the two groups of target men in age, type and level of education, and religious belief, but homosexual men reported higher income (t55 = 5.37; p < 0.001). Except for two participants who were of Slovak origin (both of them were homosexual and were excluded from the voice rating study), all of the targets were Czech, Caucasian. The target sample was mainly recruited by distribution of information lyers in various faculties of Charles University in Prague, on gay web pages, in bars, and by the snowball method; ie through social networks of the irst author who sent an information e-mail about the research to her heterosexual and homosexual friends and colleagues, and asking them to spread the information further. Data collection was performed by the irst author during two summer months of 2006 (ie within one season), in order to reduce possible seasonal effects (eg on skin colour due to tanning). 2.2 Sampling procedure All data were collected under standard conditions at the Laboratory of Human Ethology and the participants were each reimbursed by 300 CZK (approximately 17 USD) in compensation for their time. All participants signed a consent form having been informed about the procedure, and assured that data would be treated conidentially and for scientiic purposes only. They further completed a battery of questionnaires collecting information on basic demographic data, sexual orientation, and self-reported attractiveness. 2.3 Facial photographs All targets were dressed in white T-shirts of appropriate size (which we provided) when photos were taken, in order to standardise dress and reduce shadows in faces caused by coloured clothes. Each participant used a black hairband to remove hair from the forehead. Further, the participants were asked to remove earrings, facial jewellery, and to adopt a neutral facial expression. The portraits were taken with a Canon 350D camera with the focus Canon EF 50/1.8 II from a distance of 1.5 m. A light blue background was used to optimise white balance, following digital adjustments. In order to eliminate possible inluences of hairstyle, only faces with scalps covered were used for the ratings (procedure used in previous studies, eg Pivonkova et al 2011). The scalps were covered using Photoshop 7.0 software, and faces were placed on a black background. 190 J Valentová, S C Roberts, J Havlíček 2.4 Vocal recordings Vocal samples were recorded with a digital recorder Olympus WS310M with an external Sennheiser E845-S microphone. Seated targets were asked to read aloud a standard paragraph of an emotionally neutral text describing various concepts of a rainbow (Jacobs et al 2006) which was translated into the Czech language. Each participant was familiarised with the text before recordings were taken. To avoid potential effects of stress or fatigue, we extracted an intermediate section (approximately 20 s out of 90 to 120 s) of the recording, similar to the research of Jacobs and colleagues. SoundForge 8.0 software was used for the extraction and the volume standardisation. All 59 voice samples (two individuals of Slovak origin were excluded from the analyses and the voice of one participant was not recorded) were analysed by Praat software (www.praat.org) for average fundamental frequency (F0) that ranged from 86.4 to 191.8 Hz. Fundamental frequency was measured using Praat’s autocorrelation algorithm with parameters set to a pitch loor of 75 Hz and a pitch ceiling of 300 Hz, with all other values set to default. Fundamental frequencies were averaged across recordings for each speaker. 2.5 Raters The sample consisted of 50 heterosexual female raters (mean age 24.8 years, SD = 5.57 years, range 17–42 years) and 33 homosexual male raters (mean age 28.7 years, SD = 6.08 years, range 19–48 years). Women were recruited in various public places in Prague (mostly open-air cafés) with access to sit at the computer. Ratings took place during afternoons, and only individuals who had not been drinking alcoholic beverages were recruited, since there is some evidence that alcohol consumption may affect attractiveness ratings (Parker et al 2008). Before rating, raters completed a short questionnaire on basic demographic data, including age, use of hormonal contraception, and sexual orientation as assessed on a 7-point Kinsey scale (0 = heterosexual; 6 = homosexual). Of 62 females who answered this question, 50 (81%) rated themselves as heterosexual (ie scores of 0 and 1 on the Kinsey scale), and 12 (19%) as bisexual (ie 2–4 on the Kinsey scale). For all analyses, we only included data from women identifying as heterosexual. The average number of reported male sexual partners was 9.4 (with median 6). 6 women stated they had had at least one female sexual partner during their lifetime. 20 women (39.2 %) reported being in a long-term relationship at the time of the study and 29 female raters (56.9 %) were using hormonal contraception at the time of the study. 5 women stated they had at least one child. Male raters were recruited in gay bars, by the snowball method, and by distributing lealets in gay internet sites. 3 men did not complete the questionnaires. 33 men (89 %) rated themselves as homosexual (5–6 on the Kinsey scale) and 4 (11 %) as bisexual (ie scores of 3–5). All the analyses are based on male raters who identiied themselves as homosexual. The average number of reported male sexual partners was 68.1 (with median 20.0). 11 men (29.7 %) reported having a long-term partner at the time of the study. 20 men (54.1 %) stated they had had at least 1 female sexual partner during their lifetime. Raters also answered a question on how masculine or feminine they would rate themselves (answers on a 7 point scale, 1 = masculine, 7 = feminine). Recruitment locations were chosen in order to minimise the between-location variability (for factors such as lighting or surrounding noise). We also asked participants to complete ratings while sitting at the researcher’s table in order to reduce possible distraction by their friends. We note that differences across locations are more likely to reduce the chance of inding signiicant effects than to generate them. None of the raters was paid for his/her participation. Preferences for facial and vocal masculinity in homosexual men 191 2.6 Rating procedure In this study we chose to use unmanipulated voices and faces to explore preferences instead of experimental manipulation (eg manipulation of vocal fundamental frequency, or masculine facial traits) of facial and vocal masculinity. By using this method, we can estimate the effect of perceived masculinity in faces and voices with other acoustic or visual parameters that are freely varying. It was shown that vocal fundamental frequency correlates with other acoustic parameters (Hodges-Simeon et al 2010) but can be used as a proxy measure of overall vocal masculinity (Puts 2006). Furthermore, we were not only interested in objectively measured vocal or facial masculinity, which can be experimentally manipulated, but also in the relationship between perceptions of attractiveness, and perceptions of facial and vocal masculinity. We randomly chose only half of the vocal recordings because of time constraints for raters (the rating took approximately 20 min in total) and each rater rated only 1 parameter to avoid carry-over effects. Each rater judged irstly the vocal recordings and then the whole sample of facial images (each photo and vocal recording of a heterosexual individual was alternated by a photo or vocal recording of a homosexual individual) for attractiveness or masculinity on a 7 point scale (1 = attractive/masculine, 7 = not attractive/feminine). Of 50 female raters, 33 rated both facial and vocal stimuli and 17 rated only facial stimuli. All homosexual male raters rated both facial and vocal stimuli. Image ratings were carried out on a laptop screen with resolution of 1280 × 800 pixels using ImageRater 1.3 software speciically developed for our purposes. Vocal recordings were rated with the same laptop with Koss headphones. 2.7 Statistical analyses Analyses were carried out with SPSS v.16.0. Initially, data normality was tested by visual examination of histograms, and the assumptions of univariate normality for rating scores was checked with Shapiro–Wilks’s W test. In several cases data violated the assumption of normality, we have thus used nonparametric correlations to test for relationships between the variables. Moreover, to compute possible differences between the two correlations, we used Fisher’s Z. We then did some exploratory analyses to prepare for the planned inferential testing. First, using Mann–Whitney U tests, we tested for possible differences between homosexual and heterosexual targets in judged attractiveness and masculinity. The ratings of attractiveness and masculinity for both vocal and facial stimuli did not signiicantly differ between homosexual and heterosexual targets as rated either by male or female raters (all p s > 0.12); therefore the ratings were further analysed for both groups of targets together. Furthermore, both female and male raters showed high consistency in their facial attractiveness judgments (Cronbach’s a = 0.965 and 0.940, respectively) and vocal attractiveness judgments (Cronbach’s a = 0.627 and 0.763, respectively); they also showed high consistency in their facial masculinity judgments (Cronbach’s a = 0.938 and 0.942, respectively) and vocal masculinity judgments (Cronbach’s a = 0.897 and 0.620, respectively). Ratings of vocal (r = 0.646; N = 30; p < 0.001) and facial (r = 0.752; N = 61; p < 0.001) attractiveness among heterosexual females and homosexual males were highly correlated. Similarly, ratings of vocal (r = 0.891; N = 30; p < 0.001) and facial (r = 0.775; N = 61; p < 0.001) masculinity were highly correlated among heterosexual females and homosexual males. The preferences for facial and vocal masculinity were analysed by Spearman’s correlations, with mean ratings for each target as the unit of analysis. In order to test for a possible effect of relationship status, we split the raters into two groups based on their selfreport. Similarly, to test for possible inluence of sexual restrictiveness, we split the raters by the median of their number of reported sexual partners (we describe the group below the median as restricted and those above the median as unrestricted). There was no signiicant 192 J Valentová, S C Roberts, J Havlíček difference in age between coupled and single heterosexual women (Mann–Whitney U = 296; N = 51; p = 0.786), nor in age or self-ascribed masculinity–femininity between coupled and single homosexual men (Mann–Whitney U = 79.5; N = 32; p = 0.715; Mann–Whitney U = 75; N = 32; p = 0.552, respectively). However, in both women and men, those with a higher number of sexual partners were signiicantly older than more restricted individuals (Mann–Whitney U = 160.5; N = 51; p = 0.002; Mann–Whitney U = 38.5; N = 29; p = 0.004, respectively). Male raters were also categorised according to their self-rated masculinity (also using a median split), with those below the median being described as feminine and those above as masculine. To adjust for multiple comparisons, Bonferroni corrections were applied. Finally, to test for the possible inluence of voice pitch on general vocal attractiveness and masculinity ratings, we used simple linear regression with rated parameters (attractiveness or masculinity) as dependent variables, and age and voice pitch as predictors. 3 Results 3.1 General preferences To test our main hypothesis, we ran a series of non-parametric correlations between rated masculinity–femininity and attractiveness. In homosexual male raters, rated vocal masculinity was strongly and positively correlated with vocal attractiveness (Spearman’s t = 0.745; N = 30; p < 0.001), and the same effect was found in heterosexual female raters (Spearman’s t = 0.536; N = 30; p = 0.002). There was no signiicant difference between the correlation coeficients ( p = 0.18). Concerning facial ratings, we found a non-signiicant trend towards a negative correlation between rated facial masculinity and attractiveness in homosexual male raters (Spearman’s t = – 0.246; N = 61; p = 0.056), but no evidence of a signiicant relationship between these variables in heterosexual female raters (Spearman’s t = 0.064; N = 61; p = 0.625). These correlations did not differ signiicantly ( p = 0.08). 3.2 The effect of relationship status We then split the raters according to their relationship status (for all subsequent analyses we applied Bonferroni correction, α = 0.0125). The positive relationship between vocal attractiveness and masculinity remained signiicant only in single homosexual male raters (Spearman’s t = 0.594; N = 30; p = 0.001), but no signiicant results were found in coupled homosexual males and heterosexual females ( p s > 0.06). There was a signiicantly higher correlation between vocal attractiveness and masculinity in single homosexual male raters than in coupled homosexual males (Z = 2.49; p = 0.012), and in coupled heterosexual females than in single females (Z = 2.36; p = 0.018). Concerning facial ratings, we found a signiicant negative relationship between rated facial masculinity–femininity and attractiveness only in coupled homosexual men (Spearman’s t = – 0.371; N = 61; p = 0.003), while it remained non-signiicant in other groups of raters ( p s > 0.21). Fisher’s r-to-z test revealed that this relationship was signiicantly stronger in coupled than in single homosexual male raters (Z = 2.98; p = 0.002), but there was no signiicant difference in female ratings ( p = 0.36). 3.3 The effect of sexual restrictiveness After dividing the sample according to the median number of sexual partners, signiicant positive relationships between vocal attractiveness and masculinity–femininity were found in both restricted (Spearman’s t = 0.724; N = 30; p < 0.001) and unrestricted (Spearman’s t = 0.603; N = 30; p < 0.001) homosexual male raters. There was also a trend (α = 0.0125) in restricted (Spearman’s t = 0.404; N = 30; p = 0.027), and a signiicant relationship in unrestricted, female raters (Spearman’s t = 0.542; N = 30; p < 0.002). There was no signiicant difference between correlation coeficients among any of the groups of raters (all p s > 0.07). Preferences for facial and vocal masculinity in homosexual men 193 In facial ratings, unrestricted homosexual men tended (α = 0.0125) to prefer less masculine faces (Spearman’s t = – 0.294; N = 61; p = 0.022), while there was a trend towards a positive correlation in women, with unrestricted women tending to prefer more masculine faces (Spearman’s t = 0.284; N = 61; p = 0.027). The relationship was not signiicant in the other groups of raters ( p > 0.112). Sexually unrestricted homosexual men preferred signiicantly more feminine male faces than unrestricted heterosexual women (Z = 3.2; p = 0.001). 3.4 Effect of self-rated masculinity in male raters Correlations between vocal masculinity and attractiveness were positive and signiicant in both masculine and feminine homosexual men (Spearman’s t = 0.750; N = 30; p < 0.001; Spearman’s t = 0.496; N = 30; p = 0.005, respectively), and these coeficients did not differ signiicantly. Furthermore, rated facial masculinity was negatively associated with attractiveness in homosexual raters who described themselves as masculine (Spearman’s t = – 0.364; N = 61; p = 0.004), while this relationship remained non-signiicant in feminine homosexual raters (Spearman’s t = 0.067; N = 61; p = 0.608). Fisher’s r-to-z test revealed that this relationship was signiicantly different in feminine homosexual men than in masculine men (Z = 2.42; p = 0.015). 3.5 Voice pitch as a predictor of rated vocal attractiveness and masculinity To test for an effect of voice pitch of the rated individuals on rated attractiveness and masculinity, we ran linear regressions with voice pitch of the target individuals entered as independent variable, and rated attractiveness and masculinity as separate dependent variables. Because voice pitch was positively correlated with age, age of the target participants also entered as another independent variable. Rated vocal attractiveness was signiicantly predicted by voice pitch in both male and female ratings (men: t = 2.623; b = 0.476; p = 0.014; women: t = 3.990; b = 0.649; p < 0.001). Similarly, rated vocal masculinity was signiicantly predicted by voice pitch in both male and female ratings (t = 3.955; b = 0.650; p < 0.001; t = 5.078; b = 0.746; p < 0.001). 3.6 Additional analyses We also tested the relationship between ratings of attractiveness and masculinity between the two studied modalities (ie vocal and visual). We found no signiicant correlation between facial and vocal stimuli either in ratings of attractiveness or masculinity, in either group of raters (all p s > 0.3). Finally, to test whether other-rated attractiveness from facial and vocal stimuli predicts self-rated attractiveness of the targets, we ran a linear regression with self-rated attractiveness as the dependent variable and facial and vocal attractiveness (as rated by both groups of raters) entered as independent variables. This model explained 27% of the variance, and we found that self-rated attractiveness was signiicantly predicted only by facial attractiveness as rated by homosexual male raters (t = 2.130; b = 0.597; p = 0.043). 4 Discussion In this study, we aimed to test whether vocal and facial preferences of androphilic men and women are linked to male sex-typicality in targets. As predicted, we found that both homosexual men and heterosexual women prefer male voices which are perceived masculine, and which are low in pitch. In contrast, there was no general preference for masculinity in facial stimuli. Next, we investigated the possible inluences of relationship status, sexual restrictiveness, and self-perceived masculinity on preference for masculinity. We found a signiicant effect of relationship status on preferences for masculinity in voices which, however, showed opposite patterns in homosexual male and heterosexual female raters. In facial ratings, we also found this opposing direction in preference among sexually 194 J Valentová, S C Roberts, J Havlíček unrestricted women and men. Finally, the results suggest that homosexual men describing themselves as relatively masculine prefer masculine male voices but more feminine male faces. Our results, in general, indicate that androphilic individuals, irrespective of their sex, prefer sex-typical male voices over high-pitched, feminine voices. This result is in agreement with previous studies reporting similar preferences in English-speaking female raters (Collins 2000; Feinberg et al 2005a, 2005b; Riding et al 2006), and suggests that this preference is not speciic to a particular language, sex or sexual orientation. Nevertheless, this preference is modulated by the raters’ relationship status. It is suggested that masculinity in men provides a cue to underlying genetic quality, and is thus preferred by women. However, the faces of masculine men are associated with perceived traits that indicate lower investment in potential offspring, and masculinity might be thus relatively preferred by women who already have a long-term relationship with a man willing to invest in their offspring. However, it should be noted that the evidence supporting this assumption is mixed (eg Gangestad and Simpson 2000; Pawlowski and Zelazniewicz 2012). Furthermore, this effect could also be due to an exposure effect, as coupled women are exposed on a daily basis to their male partner, which might shift their preference towards more masculine faces (eg Saxton et al 2009c). In contrast, we found an opposite pattern in homosexual male raters: single homosexual men preferred more masculine male voices and faces than coupled men, which would be inconsistent with an exposure effect. Previous research has shown general preferences in homosexual men for potential sexual partners who describe themselves as relatively masculine (Bailey et al 1997) and also preferences for masculinised over feminised male faces (Glassenberg et al 2010). However, none of the previous studies tested possible effects of relationship status on these preferences, and we show that these preferences might be more pronounced in single homosexual men. Furthermore, we also found an effect of self-perceived masculinity on facial and vocal preferences, showing that homosexual men who describe themselves as relatively masculine prefer more masculine male voices, but also more feminine male faces. To our knowledge, only one other study has investigated this, in personal advertisements, where more masculine homosexual men preferred men describing themselves as masculine (Bailey et al 1997). Here we show that self-perceived masculinity also inluences preferences of homosexual men, but rather than seeking either sex-typical or atypical traits in their potential partners, it seems they prefer a mosaic of masculine and feminine traits. This is consistent with our result showing that there was no correlation between judged attractiveness across the two modalities. Several previous studies have claimed that different cues in one individual signal the same underlying quality, particularly in men (eg Feinberg et al 2008; Roberts et al 2011; Saxton et al 2006, 2009a). However, in our study, facial attractiveness was not associated with vocal attractiveness, and facial masculinity did not correlate with vocal masculinity. Thus, our targets were rated differently in the two modalities by both groups of raters. This inding is consistent with Collins (2000), who found no correlations between male voice pitch and either measured or estimated bodily characteristics. In her study, voices with low frequency were rated as attractive, and their owners as heavier, older, and more likely to be hairy-chested. Nevertheless, these estimations did not relate to the measured or self-reported qualities of the rated individuals. Thus, facial and vocal masculinity–femininity in men might be associated with different qualities—while vocal masculinity might cue to positively valued social dominance or maturity (Wells et al 2009), masculine faces might be rather associated with aggressiveness or impulsivity (Perrett et al 1998). It is also worth noting that our results show that, with increasing age, male faces were rated as more masculine but not as more attractive, which is in agreement with a previous study (Boothroyd et al 2005). Consequently, masculine physiognomy in faces (and not low voice pitch) might evoke perceptions of higher age or dominance, rather than attractiveness. Preferences for facial and vocal masculinity in homosexual men 195 The current study has also several limitations. For example, although raters were asked about their relationship status, other details such as their relationship length, general and sexual satisfaction within the relationship, and extra-pair sexual activities might have explained additional variation in raters’ responses. In summary, our results show that conditional mate preferences are not restricted to heterosexual interactions. Circumstances such as relationship status or self-perceived masculinity inluence preferences for male sex-typical traits in both females and homosexual males. Acknowledgments. We would like to thank to all participants of this study and in particular to the gay community in Prague who helped us with participants’ recruitment. We also wish to thank our colleagues and friends, particularly to Martin SVU Cech, for their help with data collection. JV was supported by the Czech Science Foundation (GACR 13-16959P), JH was supported by the Czech Science Foundation grant (P407/10/1303) and both JV and JH were supported by Charles University Research Center (UNCE 204004). References Andersson M, Iwasa Y, 1996 “Sexual selection” Trends in Ecology and Evolution 11 53–58 Apicella C L, Feinberg D R, Marlowe F W, 2007 “Voice pitch predicts reproductive success in male hunter-gatherers” Biology Letters 3 682–684 Bailey J M, Gaulin S, Agyei Y, Gladue B A, 1994 “Effects of gender and sexual orientation on evolutionarily relevant aspects of human mating psychology” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 66 1074–1080 Bailey J M, Kim P, Hills A, Linsenmeier J, 1997 “Butch, femme, or straight acting? Partner preferences of gay men and lesbians” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 73 960–973 Barber N, 1995 “The evolutionary psychology of physical attractiveness: sexual selection and human morphology” Ethology and Sociobiology 16 395–424 Berry D S, 1992 “Vocal types and stereotypes: Joint effects of vocal attractiveness and vocal maturity on person perception” Journal of Nonverbal Behavior 16 41–54 Boothroyd L G, Jones B C, Burt D M, Cornwell R E, Little A C, Tiddeman B P, Perrett D I, 2005 “Facial masculinity is related to perceived age but not perceived health” Evolution and Human Behavior 26 417–431 Boothroyd L G, Jones B C, Burt D M, DeBruine L M, Perrett D I, 2008 “Facial correlates of sociosexuality” Evolution and Human Behavior 29 211–218 Burt D M, Kentridge R W, Good J M M, Perrett D I, Tiddeman B P, Boothroyd L G, 2007 “Q-cgi: New techniques to assess variation in perception applied to facial attractiveness” Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B 274 2779–2784 Childers D G, Wu K, 1991 “Gender recognition from speech. Part II: Fine analysis” Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 90 1841–1856 Collins S A, 2000 “Men’s voices and women’s choices” Animal Behaviour 60 773–780 Dabbs J M, Mallinger A, 1999 “High testosterone levels predict low voice pitch among men” Personality and Individual Differences 27 801–804 Evans S, Neave N, Wakelin D, 2006 “Relationships between vocal characteristics and body size and shape in human males: an evolutionary explanation for a deep male voice” Biological Psychology 72 160–163 Evans S, Neave N, Wakelin D, Hamilton C, 2008 “The relationship between testosterone and vocal frequencies in human males” Physiological Behavior 93 783–788 Enquist M, Ghirlanda S, Lundquist D, Wachtmeister C-A, 2002 “An ethological theory of attractiveness”, in Facial Attractiveness: Evolutionary, Cognitive, and Social Perspectives. Advances in Visual Cognition Eds G Rhodes, L Zebrowitz (Westport, CT: Ablex) pp 127–151 Feinberg D R, DeBruine L M, Jones B C, Little A C, 2008 “Correlated preferences for men’s facial and vocal masculinity” Evolution and Human Behavior 29 233–241 Feinberg D R, Jones B C, Burt D M, Perrett D I, 2005a “Manipulations of fundamental and formant frequencies inluence the attractiveness of human male voice” Animal Behaviour 69 561–568 196 J Valentová, S C Roberts, J Havlíček Feinberg D R, Jones B C, Smith M J L, Moore F R, DeBruine L, Cornwell R E, Hillier S G, Perrett D I, 2005b “Menstrual cycle, trait estrogen level, and masculinity preferences in the human voice” Hormones and Behavior 49 215–222 Folstad I, Karter A J, 1992 “Parasites, bright males and the immunocompetence handicap” The American Naturalist 139 603–622 Gangestad S W, Simpson J A, 2000 “The evolution of human mating: trade-off and strategic pluralism” Behavioral and Brain Sciences 23 573–644 Gangestad S W, Thornhill R, 2003 “Facial masculinity and luctuating asymmetry” Evolution and Human Behavior 24 231–241 Glassenberg A N, Feinberg D R, Jones B C, Little A C, DeBruine L M, 2010 “Sex-dimorphic face shape preference in heterosexual and homosexual men and women” Archives of Sexual Behavior 39 1289–1296 Hayes A F, 2001 “Age preferences for same- and opposite-sex partners” Journal of Social Psychology 135 125–133 Hodges-Simeon C R, Gaulin S J, Puts D A, 2010 “Different vocal parameters predict perceptions of dominance and attractiveness” Human Nature 21 406–427 Hollien H, Green R, Massey K, 1994 “Longitudinal research on adolescent voice change in males” Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 96 2646–2654 Howard J A, Blumstein P, Schwartz P, 1987 “Social or evolutionary theories? Some observations on preferences in human mate selection” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 53 194–200 Hughes S M, Dispenza F, Gallup G G, 2004 “Ratings of voice attractiveness predict sexual behavior and body coniguration” Evolution and Human Behavior 25 295–304 Jacobs G, Smyth R, Rogers H, 2006 “Language and sexuality: Searching for the phonetic correlates of gay- and straight-sounding male voices” Toronto Working Papers in Linguistics 18 46–61 Johnston V S, Hagel R, Franklin M, Fink B, Grammer K, 2001 “Male facial attractiveness—Evidence for hormone-mediated adaptive design” Evolution and Human Behavior 22 251–267 Jones B C, DeBruine L M, Perrett D I, Little A C, Feinberg D R, Law Smith M J, 2008 “Effects of menstrual cycle phase on face preferences” Archives of Sexual Behavior 37 78–84 Little A C, Burt D M, Penton-Voak I S, Perrett D I, 2001 “Self-perceived attractiveness inluences human female preferences for sexual dimorphism and symmetry in male faces” Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B 268 39–44 Little A C, Jones B C, Penton-Voak I S, Burt D M, Perrett D I, 2002 “Partnership status and the temporal context of relationships inluence human female preferences for sexual dimorphism in male face shape” Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B 269 1095–1100 Little A C, Jones B C, Waitt C, Tiddeman B P, Feinberg D R, Perrett D I, Apicella C L, Marlowe F W, 2008 “Symmetry is related to sexual dimorphism in faces: data across culture and species” PLoS ONE 3 e2106 Muscarella F, 2002 “Preferred partner characteristics in homosexual men in relation to speculated patterns of brain differentiation” Neuroendocrinology Letters 23 299–302 Oguchi T, Kikuchi H, 1997 “Voice and interpersonal attraction” Japanese Psychological Research 39 56–61 Parker L L C, Penton-Voak I S, Attwood A S, Munafo M R, 2008 “Effects of acute alcohol consumption on ratings of attractiveness of facial stimuli: Evidence of long-term encoding” Alcohol and Alcoholism 43 636–640 Pawlowski B, Zelazniewicz A, 2012 “Newborn’s condition at birth does not depend on maternal sexual strategy—evidence against the ‛hunting for good genes’ hypothesis” American Journal of Human Biology 24 420–424 Penton-Voak I S, Chen J Y, 2004 “High salivary testosterone is linked to masculine male facial appearance in humans” Evolution and Human Behavior 25 229–241 Penton-Voak I S, Perrett D, 2000 “Consistency and individual differences in facial attractiveness judgements: an evolutionary perspective” Social Research 67 219–244 Perrett D I, Lee K J, Penton-Voak I S, Rowland D, Yoshikawa S, Burt D M, Henzil S P, Castles D L, Akamatsu S, 1998 “Effects of sexual dimorphism on facial attractiveness” Nature 394 884–887 Preferences for facial and vocal masculinity in homosexual men 197 Pivonkova V, Rubesova A, Lindova J, Havlicek J, 2011 “Sexual dimorphism and personality attributions in male faces” Archives of Sexual Behavior 40 1137–1143 Puts D A, 2005 “Mating context and menstrual phase affect women’s preferences for male voice pitch” Evolution and Human Behavior 26 388–397 Puts D A, 2006 “Cyclic variation in women’s preferences for masculine traits: Potential hormonal causes” Human Nature 17 114–127 Puts D A, Apicella C L, Cárdenas R A, 2012 “Masculine voices signal men’s threat potential in forager and industrial societies” Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B 279 601–609 Puts D A, Gaulin S J C, Verdolini K, 2006 “Dominance and the evolution of sexual dimorphism in human voice pitch” Evolution and Human Behavior 27 283–296 Rhodes G, 2006 “The evolutionary psychology of facial beauty” Annual Review of Psychology 57 199–226 Rhodes G, Chan J, Zebrowitz L A, Simmons L W, 2003 “Does sexual dimorphism in human faces signal health?” Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B 270 S93–S95 Rhodes G, Hickford C, Jeffrey L, 2000 “Sex-typicality and attractiveness: Are supermale and superfemale faces super-attractive?” British Journal of Psychology 91 125–140 Riding D, Lonsdale D, Brown B, 2006 “The effects of average fundamental frequency and variance of fundamental frequency on male vocal attractiveness to women” Journal of Nonverbal Behavior 30 55–61 Roberts S C, Little A C, 2008 “Good genes, complementary genes and human mate preferences” Genetica 134 31–43 Roberts S C, Ferdenzi C, Kralevich A, Little A C, Jones B C, DeBruine L M, Havlicek J, 2011 “Body odor quality predicts behavioral attractiveness in humans” Archives of Sexual Behavior 40 1111–1117 Saxton T K, Burriss R P, Murray A K, Rowland H M, Roberts S C, 2009a “Face, body and speech cues independently predict judgments of attractiveness” Journal of Evolutionary Psychology 7 23–35 Saxton T K, Caryl P G, Roberts S C, 2006 “Vocal and facial attractiveness judgments of children, adolescents and adults: the ontogeny of mate choice” Ethology 112 1179–1185 Saxton T K, DeBruine L M, Jones B C, Little A C, Roberts S C, 2009b “Face and voice attractiveness judgments change during adolescence” Evolution and Human Behavior 30 398–408 Saxton T K, Little A C, DeBruine L M, Jones B C, Roberts S C, 2009c “Adolescents’ preferences for sexual dimorphism are inluenced by relative exposure to male and female faces” Personality and Individual Differences 47 864–868 Silverthorne Z A, Quinsey V L, 2000 “Sexual partner age preferences of homosexual and heterosexual men and women” Archives of Sexual Behavior 29 67–76 Vanderlaan D, Vasey P, 2008 “Mate retention behavior of men and women in heterosexual and homosexual relationships” Archives of Sexual Behavior 37 572–585 Wells T J, Dunn A K, Sergeant M J T, Davies M N O, 2009 “Multiple signals in human mate selection: a review and framework for integrating facial and vocal signals” Journal of Evolutionary Psychology 7 111–139 Wu K, Childers D G, 1991 “Gender recognition from speech. Part I: Coarse analysis” Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 90 1828–1840 Yeo R A, Thornhill R, Gangestad S W, 1994 “Facial attractiveness, developmental stability, and luctuating asymmetry” Ethology and Sociobiology 15 73–85 Zuckermann M, Miyake K, Elkin C S, 1995 “Effects of attractiveness and maturity of face and voice on interpersonal impressions” Journal of Research in Personality 29 253–272