[go: up one dir, main page]

Academia.eduAcademia.edu
Behav Ecol Sociobiol (2013) 67:311–320 DOI 10.1007/s00265-012-1451-z ORIGINAL PAPER Hierarchical patch choice by an insectivorous bat through prey availability components D. Almenar & J. Aihartza & U. Goiti & E. Salsamendi & I. Garin Received: 24 January 2012 / Revised: 7 November 2012 / Accepted: 7 November 2012 / Published online: 20 November 2012 # Springer-Verlag Berlin Heidelberg 2012 Abstract Food availability does not only refer to the abundance of edible items; accessibility and detectability of food are also essential components of the availability concept. Constraints imposed by a habitat’s physical structure on the accessibility and detectability of food have been seldom treated simultaneously to the abundance of prey at the foraging patch level in observational studies. We designed a research that allowed decoupling the effects of microhabitat structure and prey abundance on foraging patch selection of the trawling insectivorous long-fingered bat (Myotis capaccinii). The use of different patches of river was surveyed by radiotelemetry during three periods of the bat’s annual cycle, and prey abundance was accordingly measured in and out of the hunting grounds of the tracked bats by insect traps emulating the species’ foraging. Bats preferentially used river stretches characterised by an open course and smooth water surfaces, i.e. they used the most suitable patches in terms of prey accessibility and detectability, respectively. In addition, prey abundance in the selected river stretches was higher than in others where bat activity was not recorded, although the latter also offered good access and prey detection possibilities. Bats also shifted foraging stretches seasonally, likely following the spatiotemporal dynamics of prey production over the watershed. We suggest that the decisions of bats during the patch choice process fitted a hierarchical sequence driven first by the species’ morphological specialisations and ability to hunt in unobstructed spaces, then by the detectability of prey on water surfaces and, finally, by the relative abundance of prey. Communicated by C. Voigt D. Almenar : J. Aihartza : U. Goiti : E. Salsamendi : I. Garin (*) Zoologia eta Animali Zelulen Biologia Saila, UPV/EHU, Sarriena z/g, 48940 Leioa, The Basque Country e-mail: inazio.garin@ehu.es Keywords Patch selection . Foraging . Prey availability . Chiroptera . Myotis capaccinii Introduction Foraging patch selection has received much theoretical attention within the field of behavioural ecology, and its study was originally approached by models predicting the decisions that foraging animals should make in patches with variable food abundance (i.e. MacArthur and Pianka 1966; Fretwell and Lucas 1970; Fretwell 1972; Charnov 1976). Original models have been refined to incorporate the effect of prey’s demographic and behavioural response, predator learning or behavioural plasticity among others, so that more realistic spatial predator–prey relationships could be depicted (e.g. Abrams 2007; Okuyama 2009; Scharf et al. 2012). A common practice is to assume the availability of food in a given patch as solely dependent on its abundance; however, the accessibility and detectability of the food to the animal are also essential components of resources’ actual availability (Holmes and Schultz 1988; Hall et al. 1997). While abundance indicates the absolute number of food items in a defined space (density), accessibility and detectability relate to the animal’s capability to reach and handle food items and to its ability to discriminate food items from the non-edible background, respectively. The participation of the environmental physical attributes that impose some limitation to prey access and detection in the spatial decisions of predators has been already revealed in a number of species (Rice 1983; Whelan 2001; Butler and Gillings 2004; Hopcraft et al. 2005; Jones et al. 2006; Siemers and Güttinger 2006; Chan et al. 2009). Furthermore, where the prey abundance and the structural features of the environment have been investigated, the patch choice process seems to be driven by access and detection rather than abundance of prey (e.g. Kelsey and Hassall 1989; Getty and Pulliam 1993; 312 Uiblein et al. 1995; Bennetts et al. 2006; Andruskiw et al. 2008; Greenville and Dickman 2009; Rainho et al. 2010; Hagen and Sabo 2011; but see Olsson et al. 2001). Nevertheless, the effect of abundance, accessibility and detectability on foraging patch selection is seldom teased apart in observational studies (Holmes and Schultz 1988; BenoitBird et al. 2011), which to some degree may explain the failure to unveil the widely accepted relationship between predator patch choice and prey numbers. In fact, assessing the abundance of natural prey in addition to structural features of the environment is usually a formidable task and measuring it at the precise foraging patch of wild animals is rarely affordable. A feasible ecological model to study patch selection with consideration to the multilevel nature of prey availability may be offered by echolocating bats. The roles of prey detectability (e.g. Neuweiler 1989; Safi and Siemers 2010) and accessibility (e.g. Norberg and Rayner 1987; Rainho et al. 2010) are relatively well known for insectivorous bats that rely on echolocation to catch their prey. Besides, current radio-tracking techniques allow researchers to accurately define individual foraging grounds despite their nocturnal habits and flight and in turn to estimate the abundance of prey actually accessible to and detectable by bats (e.g. Almenar et al. 2008). Furthermore, trawling bats represent an excellent choice to study the influence of availability on patch selection by insectivorous bats, as their foraging behaviour is well defined. Bats in this group share several morphological traits and hunting styles (Norberg and Rayner 1987; Fenton and Bogdanowicz 2002), taking their main prey directly from the water surface (or immediately above it) in a stereotyped manner known as ‘trawling’ behaviour (Jones and Rayner 1988; Kalko and Schnitzler 1989; Kalko 1990; Jones and Rayner 1991; Britton et al. 1997), by which they obtain water-related arthropods (emerging, floating or flying level with the surface; Vaughan 1997; Funakoshi and Takeda 1998; Limpens et al. 1999; Law and Urquhart 2000; Flavin et al. 2001). Production rates of these kinds of prey would unlikely show a demographic response to bat predation; hence, measurements of relative abundance can be plausibly correlated with the encounter rate. In addition, foraging in these bats is specialised with regard to techniques and microhabitats, limiting dietary shifts between alternative prey and, accordingly, promoting estimation of the effective profitability in the foraging patches. Detection of prey is facilitated by the ‘mirror effect’ of sound travelling over flat surfaces (Siemers et al. 2001, 2005; Greif and Siemers 2010). This acoustical effect explains why breaking or cluttering of the water surface is a major constraint on foraging by these bats (von Frenckell and Barclay 1987; Boonman et al. 1998; Rydell et al. 1999; Warren et al. 2000; Almenar et al. 2006). In addition, the group shows limited flight manoeuvrability in structurally complex environments, driving the bats to open spaces for commuting and hunting (Norberg and Rayner 1987). Behav Ecol Sociobiol (2013) 67:311–320 The long-fingered bat (Myotis capaccinii, Bonaparte 1837) is a trawling bat that dwells in caves (Spitzenberger and von Helversen 2001) distributed throughout the Mediterranean basin and South-western Asia (Hutson et al. 2001). It forages almost exclusively on aquatic habitats, showing a preference for rivers (Almenar et al. 2006, 2009). The present study aims to understand patch selection by foraging animals, under the assumption that patch use by insectivorous bats will fit the expectations of general patch selection models (Fretwell and Lucas 1970; Fretwell 1972). We specifically tested whether, within the limits imposed by prey detection and accessibility, the foraging activity of M. capaccinii will concentrate in patches with higher abundance of prey. To do so, we removed the effect of physical constraints on the patch choice process using a two-step approach: we first identified the environment’s physical features that locally constrained patch choice and then we compared prey abundance between patches with and without recorded hunting but similar in physical attributes of microhabitat structure. We thereby minimised the distorting effect of the physical features on the measures of prey abundance. Materials and methods Study area, bat population and study period The study area is located in the eastern Iberian Peninsula (39°04′ N–0°35′ W) and spreads over 3,850 km2 of the lower basin of the Xúquer River. This area includes several tributaries, among which the Albaida and Sellent rivers are the most important. The water flow shows Mediterranean seasonality, with lower levels during mid and late summer. Rivers are managed via a large dam (called Presa de Tous), many weirs (locally known as ‘assuts’) and a vast network of irrigation canals. There are also many ponds (about 3/km2). Aquatic habitats cover 1.2 % of the study area; of these, the calm smooth-surfaced waters that are the preferred foraging habitat of long-fingered bats account for 72 % (Almenar 2008). A large floodplain (20–50 ma.s.l.) mainly covered by orange groves comprises approximately two thirds of the study area. The remaining area is hilly (maximum altitude, 600 ma.s.l.) and mainly covered by Mediterranean post-fire scrublands and pinewoods. For a full description of the study area, refer to Almenar et al. (2009). Two roosts in the area are inhabited consistently every year by large groups of M. capaccinii during the breeding season. The main roost is a limestone cave with a variable colony size through the season: 81 M. capaccinii during pre-breeding, 219 during lactation and 113 during weaning of 2004. More than 100 individuals inhabit the second roost during lactation, located 8.4 km from the first. Both roosts are shared with other bat species (Rhinolophus euryale, Rhinolophus mehelyi, Behav Ecol Sociobiol (2013) 67:311–320 Myotis blythii, Myotis myotis, Myotis emarginatus, Myotis nattereri and Miniopterus schreibersii). Fieldwork was carried out in 2004 and performed during three periods corresponding to three phases in the bats’ annual cycle: pre-breeding (9 to 30 April), lactation (24 May to 11 June) and weaning (1 to 17 July). 313 Table 1 Capture date and radio-tracking effort of 45 long-fingered bats during the three periods of the present study Study period Capture date Characteristic Days Fixes Pre-breeding 4-9-04 AF AF AM AM AF AF AF AM AM AF AM AF AM 3 4 4 8 5 2 4 6 5 3 3 1 2 41 36 43 132 60 31 140 89 78 50 43 19 14 AM AM AF L AM L L L L L L AM P L L AF L JF 5 1 2 4 3 3 4 1 4 1 4 2 2 2 1 1 2 5 38 17 17 7 30 2 47 22 13 20 36 50 33 7 41 23 50 89 JM JF JM JF AF AF AF JM JF JF JF JM AF AF 4 7 5 3 5 3 3 2 5 3 2 4 3 3 64 64 53 23 74 56 69 6 5 36 38 7 40 27 Identification of foraging locations Foraging patches were identified by radio-tracking. We captured bats twice per period when they returned to the main roost after nightly foraging, using a harp trap (modified from Tuttle 1974). In total, 62 bats were radio-tagged, sexed, aged and weighed. After clipping the fur between their shoulder blades, we equipped the bats with a 0.45-g PIP II radio transmitter (Biotrack Ltd., Dorset, UK) using Skinbond surgical adhesive (Smith and Nephew, Largo, FL, USA). The transmitter represented a load of <5 % of the bat’s weight (Aldridge and Brigham 1988), except for 11 cases, mostly juveniles, in which it reached 6.3 %; no abnormal behaviour was observed in these individuals. Capture, manipulation and tagging were performed with permission from the Valencian Government. Bats were (as much as possible) continuously tracked from emergence to return. Up to six teams searched for bats simultaneously, using either 1000-XRS radio receivers (Wildlife Materials, Carbondale, IL, USA) or FT-290RII units (A. Wagener Telemetrieanlagen, Köln, Germany) and three-element Yagi antennae. Mobile teams tracked bats by car or on foot and were guided to foraging sites by up to three observers at stationary vantage points. The stationary observers received radio signals from a wider spatial range than mobile trackers, and their searching scope encompassed both aquatic and terrestrial habitats. Mobile teams focused on the tracking of up to three bats each night. Only those fixes obtained through homing-in (White and Garrot 1990) by the mobile teams were analysed. We failed to gather any fix of nine individuals (three per period). Each period, the 15 bats with the most foraging locations were selected for analyses among tracked individuals (Table 1). When bats flew directly and quickly between the roost and a foraging site or between two separate foraging sites, we considered them to be commuting. Continuous movements to and fro over a location were considered as foraging. Because bats were able to commute to distant foraging sites in 10 min (DA, personal observation), we used a time interval of 10 min between consecutive fixes to avoid spatial autocorrelation. Fixes were mapped in the field on orthoimages or 1:10,000 topographic maps and then transferred into a Geographical Information System (ArcView 3.2, ESRI, Redlands, CA, USA). 4-17-04 Lactation 5-24-04 6-4-04 Weaning 7-1-04 7-10-04 AF adult female, AM adult male, L lactating female, P pregnant female, JF juvenile female, JM juvenile male, Days number of days with radiolocations, Fixes total number of fixes gathered on rivers 314 Classification of patches Patch use was investigated along the rivers, which were the preferred and most used habitat unit by the studied population (individual foraging activity was 92 % on average; Almenar et al. 2009). A colonial minimum convex polygon (MCP; White and Garrot 1990) encompassing all the radiolocations limited the area of foraging activity. To define patches, we first divided the river network into physically homogeneous sectors, distinguishing between sectors with open smooth waters vs. sectors with open rippled surfaces and/or covered by dense reed vegetation. These features were known to affect the preferences of foraging M. capaccinii (Almenar et al. 2006, 2009; Biscardi et al. 2007). Then, we arbitrarily subdivided the homogeneous stretches into 100-m-long stretches. The 100-m measure was chosen on the basis of the approximate distance of the bats’ characteristic to and fro repetitive flight pattern during foraging (Ahlén 1990; Kalko 1990; DA, personal observation). Because the length of homogeneous stretches was not an exact multiple of 100 m, 11 % of them did not reach 100 m. These shorter patches had low impact on the outcome of the analysis, as only 4.7 % of them were used by bats and the test results did not change with their removal from the analysis. A patch was classified as used during any of the three study periods if at least 1 h of foraging activity was recorded there during that period. We set this limit to avoid the inclusion of patches used for exploration. Among the patches without foraging activity of radio-tracked individuals, we chose those used during any of the other two periods—that is, with confirmed suitable structural features for foraging—and classified them as potential patches. Since the rivers’ physical features remained unaltered during the three periods, we reduced the likelihood of any seasonal change of use to occur due to a response of the physical attributes of the patch. Measures of arthropod abundance Although some M. capaccinii populations are known to eat fish in the wild (Aihartza et al. 2003; Levin et al. 2006; Biscardi et al. 2007), the diet of the surveyed individuals appeared to be exclusively arthropods (Almenar et al. 2008). Arthropod abundance was measured by two different prey capture methods during the nights of radio-tracking. One method emulated the bats’ trawling behaviour by dragging the water surface with a hand net (75×25 cm, 1 mm mesh) for 15 min 1–3 h after sunset. The second method sampled airborne arthropods (adults with aquatic and terrestrial larvae) during the whole night by using a malaise interception trap (height 2 m; Entomopraxis, Madrid, Spain) mounted in a structure with floaters for set-up on the water surface. The arthropods caught by both methods were stored in 70 % Behav Ecol Sociobiol (2013) 67:311–320 ethanol and subsequently counted and identified to the ordinal level. Arthropod abundance was sampled simultaneously in 11 used and 13 potential patches during pre-breeding, 12 used and 6 potential patches during lactation and 16 used and 11 potential patches during weaning. Statistical analyses We compared the frequency of patches with contrasting physical features (open smooth waters vs. open rippled surfaces and/or vegetation coverage) between the used patches (observed frequency) and the patches present at the rivers of the study area as a whole (expected frequency). Disproportion between expected and observed frequencies would indicate that physical features were not used at random. A goodnessof-fit G test was used to compare the frequencies (Sokal and Rohlf 1994). Two variables were analysed for differences in arthropod abundance between used and potential patches: aerial prey (captured by malaise traps) and surface prey (trapped by handnetting). Two factorial analyses of variance (ANOVAs) were performed, with period, usage and interaction term as factors. As the variables approached a log-normal distribution, they were log-transformed for the analyses. The GLM procedure of the software SAS 9.0 was used to complete the hypothesis tests, with 0.05 as the designated significance level. Results Among the 851 patches of the study area, 373 showed open smooth water surfaces and thus were classed as structurally suitable for foraging, whereas the other 478 patches showed water surfaces with entangled vegetation and/or disturbance. A total of 1,880 foraging fixes were gathered at rivers by radio-tracking (95 % of all foraging fixes), averaging 42 per bat (range, 2–140), allowing identification of 49 used patches, with 4 being used during more than one period (Fig. 1). The frequency of patches with open smooth water surfaces was significantly higher in the group of used patches than in the whole study area (73 and 44 %, respectively; G020.33, df01, p<0.0001). Accordingly, 27 % of used patches were classified as cluttered. The patches used by the radio-tracked bats contained more aerial and surface prey than did the potential patches (those physically suitable for hunting but not used) (Table 2; Fig. 2). Only the abundance of aerial prey varied significantly between periods (Table 2; Fig. 2). The significance of the interaction term indicates that the magnitude of difference between used and potential patches during the three periods was the same for surface prey but not for aerial prey. Specifically, the difference in overall aerial prey abundance Behav Ecol Sociobiol (2013) 67:311–320 315 Fig. 1 Maps of foraging radiolocations on rivers of 45 M. capaccinii. The study area is delimited by an MCP (black line) encompassing all the radiolocations (both over and outside rivers). River network is drawn in grey. Foraging fixes on rivers are drawn as black dots. The main roost is represented by a black asterisk and the secondary roost is represented by a grey asterisk. a Pre-breeding, b lactation, c weaning Table 2 ANOVA tables for two prey abundance variables in study area: aerial prey and surface prey Source Df SS MS F Variable: aerial prey abundance (log-transformed) Period 2 10.98 5.49 4.59 Use 1 10.90 10.90 9.11 Period×use 2 8.54 4.27 3.57 Residual 65 77.83 1.19 Variable: surface prey abundance (log-transformed) Period Use Period×use Residual 2 1 2 65 10.09 10.61 5.52 168.16 5.04 10.61 2.76 2.59 1.95 4.10 1.07 p 0.0137* 0.0036* 0.0338* 0.1506 0.0470* 0.3499 Three factors were considered: period (three levels), use (used patch/ potential patch) and interaction term. Asterisk denotes significant difference (p<0.05) between used and potential patches was small during the lactation period (Fig. 2). Regardless of usage by bats, the simulated catch rate by aerial traps was higher during weaning (312.0 prey/trap; SD0290.4), but that by surface traps was higher during lactation (188.5 prey/trap; SD0385.1). Nematoceran dipterans, especially chironomids (including pupae in the surface trap), dominated the overall captures obtained by both trap types during all periods. Aerial catches of nematocerans ranged from 81 % during weaning to 91 % during lactation. The proportion of nematocerans over the surface was lower, ranging from 54 % during weaning to 78 % during lactation. Other arthropod groups were present in lower proportions. Only trichopterans during weaning exceeded 5 % of aerial catches, and only heteropterans (mainly aquatic) and hymenopterans accounted for more than 5 % of surface catches in some period. Prey size composition was dominated by small arthropods. The proportion of large prey was higher for 316 Behav Ecol Sociobiol (2013) 67:311–320 a Geometrical mean of prey abundance 1750 1500 1250 1000 750 500 250 0 Potential Use Pre-breeding Potential Use Lactation Potential Use Weaning b Geometrical mean of prey abundance 800 600 400 200 0 Potential Use Pre-breeding Potential Use Lactation Potential Use Weaning Fig. 2 Variation in prey abundance, estimated by means of two arthropod capture devices in the rivers of study area. Bars represent the geometrical mean, and lines indicate the 95 % confidence interval: a aerial prey, b surface prey. Geometrical mean is used because data approaches lognormal distribution. Note the different scale between a and b surface traps (32 %) than aerial traps (9 %). For both aerial and surface traps, the proportion of larger prey increased from pre-breeding to weaning. Discussion Foraging long-fingered bats (M. capaccinii) showed a disproportionate use of river stretches with increased accessibility and detectability, i.e. open smooth water surfaces. Similarly to other trawling bat species, their hunting is constrained by disturbance of the water surface (Boonman et al. 1998; Rydell et al. 1999; Warren et al. 2000; Siemers et al. 2001). Particularly, entangled weed vegetation in some stretches of our study area hampers the bats’ access to patches that otherwise would likely offer suitable prey production (Almenar et al. 2006), and patches with turbulent flow are likely to reduce search efficiency for prey compared to smooth surfaces (Rydell et al. 1999; Siemers et al. 2005). This pattern of patch selection has also been observed in other populations of M. capaccinii (Biscardi et al. 2007). Nonetheless, overcoming the physical constraints alone does not fully explain foraging patch choice in the long- fingered bat. We found that most selected patches were used by foraging bats only during precise periods and their spatial arrangement changed noticeably through seasons (Fig. 1). Thus, we believe that another factor, which was seasonally variable, could be acting upon patch choice. Prey abundance is the most likely candidate, since our results revealed an association between the usage of patches and the amount of catchable arthropods there. The bulk of the species’ diet is comprised by insects of aquatic larvae, a typically ephemeral prey with spatially and temporarily aggregated emergence from water (e.g. Corbet 1964; Gathmann and Williams 2006). The resulting variability of prey abundance between patches could stem from changes in water temperature (Armitage et al. 1995). Since the predator showed also an irregular spatiotemporal pattern of foraging, presumably the foraging patches could be selected according to their profitability in terms of prey abundance. The diversity of hydromorphological features in the study area probably sustained a continuous, though spatially variable, supply of productive patches throughout the entire season of activity. Goiti et al. (2006) also proposed the importance of internal diversity in foraging habitats to ensure an uninterrupted source of prey for R. euryale. Therefore, both prey abundance and physical structure of the foraging habitat seem to be the leading factors in patch choice of the long-fingered bat. This may be generally applicable to many aerial insectivorous bats, as some authors have already anticipated (Grindal and Brigham 1999; Warren et al. 2000; Kusch et al. 2004; Fukui et al. 2006; Goiti et al. 2008; Akasaka et al. 2009). Conversely, studies that have simultaneously addressed the effect of access, detection and abundance on the patch selection process have usually pointed out a negligible influence of prey abundance. For instance, lions ambush prey at greater numbers in areas where prey catchability is higher rather than where prey is most abundant (Hopcraft et al. 2005); and foraging patch choice of dunlins that use tactile stimuli to locate prey in mud has been claimed to be constrained exclusively by the penetrability of the substrate (a surrogate for accessibility; Kelsey and Hassall 1989). Similarly, bats may be forced to forage in patches that grant unobstructed access to prey despite their lower insect density and similar detectability (Rainho et al. 2010; Hagen and Sabo 2011). However, it must be noted that there is a vacuum of studies that measure prey availability at the precise foraging patch, an unavoidable prerequisite to evaluate fairly the effects of the three components of availability on the patch selection process. This is seldom carried out, but when so, the depicted interplay between the components is similar to that found in our study (Holmes and Schultz 1988; Benoit-Bird et al. 2011), e.g. in birds that glean amidst foliage, the abundance of lepidopteran larvae determines the foraging choice at the leaf level only when the structural features of the foliage and the behaviour of the prey suit the ability of the bird to capture and Behav Ecol Sociobiol (2013) 67:311–320 perceive that kind of prey (Holmes and Schultz 1988). As a corollary, we suggest that accessibility, detectability and abundance of prey represent an effective conceptual framework to dissect and understand the factors contributing to patch selection by animals. Nevertheless, it has to be noticed that competitive interference and risk of predation are additional factors acting upon foraging patch selection (e.g. Brown 1988; Butler et al. 2005; Encarnaçao et al. 2010) and thus they may adjust animal preference to a subset of structural features within the physical attributes that broadly shape access to, detection of and abundance of prey. Specifically, we suggest that a hierarchical process rules patch choice by M. capaccinii, with different foraging decisions at different levels. First, the species’ morphological specialisations tie it to water habitats. Second, bats tend to forage in open spaces where their manoeuvrability is not limited. Third, their foraging occurs preferentially on smooth water surfaces, where prey detection is improved. Finally, bats spread over those patches where prey profitability is higher at any given moment. These kind of hierarchical or multilevel processes, where the results of these decisions are not consistent between levels or scales, are useful to understand the successive foraging decisions. For instance, selection of a given food item is envisaged as the consequence of a multiple scale decision process that starts with habitat selection and ends with the chosen item (Sallabanks 1993; Brown and Morgan 1995). Overall, patch selection seems to be determined by predatory risk at larger scales and by energy intake maximisation at the final steps of the selection process (Senft et al. 1987; Holbrook and Schmitt 1988; Bowers and Dooley 1993; Heithaus and Dill 2006). Our study shows that access to and detection of food may also play a role in a multi-scale decision context for foraging. Precise knowledge on the constraints on perception and locomotion may help to elucidate whether the obtained results apply to other organisms with presumably low predation hazard, such as open air flying bats. Due to our research design, we could have misclassified some patches. Indeed, we cannot be completely certain all the patches classified as potential were not used, as untagged bats might have hunted in them. This fact could have masked any true difference in prey abundance between the used and suitable but actually unused (potential) patches; however, the general difference in prey abundance observed between used and potential patches indicates that either the proportion of misclassified potential patches was very low or the actual differences between these patches were even larger. The larger colony size, the rising spread of individuals across the study area and the low overlap of the hunting sectors of the radio-tracked individuals during lactation (Almenar et al. 2011) may have enhanced the risk of misclassifying patches and, therefore, the chances of overlooking any difference between used and potential patches may have been larger during that period. This flaw could have 317 contributed to the similarity in prey production of used and potential patches during lactation. The selection of foraging patches is intimately related to the information that animals have on their profitability (Giraldeau 1997; Dall et al. 2005). However, the emergence of aquatic insects can hardly be foreseen with precision in space and time (Corbet 1964; Gathmann and Williams 2006), and bats’ past experience alone may not guide their choice of profitable patches at the right moment. In our study, some patches attracted many individuals at the same time, principally during pre-breeding and weaning, which introduces a tantalizing question: Was there any information transfer between individuals? Or did each bat evaluate the profitability of a patch independently? Wilkinson (1992) found some evidence suggesting information transfer about profitable patches between foraging individuals of Nycticeius humeralis. According to this author, some bats with little foraging success could follow other bats to their foraging patches, since prey abundance for this species appears to be spatially heterogeneous and ephemeral. This scenario may also suit our studied population because the prey supply was also heterogeneous in space and time. Eavesdropping on echolocation sounds of other foraging bats has been proposed as a means of information transfer (Barclay 1982; Hickey and Fenton 1990; Jones and Siemers 2011), and Noctilio albiventris, also a trawling bat, apparently uses it to locate sources of prey (Dechmann et al. 2009). The role of eavesdropping in finding profitable foraging grounds is plausible in M. capaccinii. Nevertheless, the probability to spot high densities of prey by eavesdropping seems higher during commuting than during foraging, during seasons when dispersion of prey production is more contagious and in those populations that hunt often in scattered and isolated water bodies like ponds, irrigation pools, reservoirs or lakes (Némoz and Brisorgueil 2008). This study has shown that prey availability may largely explain the patch choice of insectivorous bats. However, this availability must be understood not only as prey abundance but also as detectability and accessibility. The ability of individuals to detect and hunt prey at a given river stretch, which is in turn related to the microhabitat features, affects the patch selection process. Given that the physical structure of the river stretches remained roughly constant throughout the activity period, the fluctuations of prey abundance within and between patches were the cause of the notable variations in the spatiotemporal pattern of the bats’ use of foraging areas (Almenar et al. 2011). Our understanding of the relationship between foraging animals and their surrounding landscape should account for the interaction between microhabitat features and prey abundance. By selecting patches with suitable microhabitats, foraging animals maximise prey detection and accessibility, and by selecting richer patches, they maximise their capture rate as well. 318 Acknowledgments We thank all those who kindly helped us in the field. Kate Johnson proofread the English of the manuscript before submission. We also wish to thank the four anonymous reviewers. This study was part of a LIFE project (LIFE00NAT/E/7337) coordinated by C. Ibáñez, Estación Biológica de Doñana (Consejo Superior de Investigaciones Científicas [CSIC]) and co-funded by the Conselleria de Territori i Habitatge of the Generalitat Valenciana and the European Commission. Support was also provided by the Centro de Protección y Estudio del Medio Natural ‘La Granja de El Saler’, the University of the Basque Country (UPV/EHU, research grant no. 9/UPV 0076.31015849/2004) and the Basque Government. D.A. and E.S. were sponsored by the CSIC and UPV/EHU, and U.G. was sponsored by the Basque Government and UPV/EHU. The funders had no role in the study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish or preparation of the manuscript. Ethical standards This study was performed according to the Environmental Laws and with the authorisation of the Valencian Government. References Abrams PA (2007) Habitat choice in predator–prey systems: spatial instability due to interacting adaptive movement. Am Nat 5:581– 594 Ahlén I (1990) Identification of bats in flight. Swedish Society for Conservation of Nature, Stockholm Aihartza JR, Goiti U, Almenar D, Garin I (2003) Evidences of piscivory by Myotis capaccinii (Bonaparte, 1837) in Southern Iberian Peninsula. Acta Chiropterol 5:193–198 Akasaka T, Nakano D, Nakamura F (2009) Influence of prey variables, food supply, and river restoration on the foraging activity of Daubenton’s bat (Myotis daubentonii) in the Shibetsu River, a large lowland river in Japan. Biol Conserv 142:1302–1310 Aldridge HDJN, Brigham RM (1988) Load carrying and manoeuvrability in an insectivorous bat: a test of the 5 % “rule” of radio-telemetry. J Mammal 69:379–382 Almenar D (2008) Uso y selección de recursos por el murciélago patudo (Myotis capaccinii): presas, hábitats y áreas de caza. Dissertation, University of the Basque Country Almenar D, Aihartza J, Goiti U, Salsamendi E, Garin I (2006) Habitat selection and spatial use by the trawling bat Myotis capaccinii (Bonaparte, 1837). Acta Chiropterol 8:157–167 Almenar D, Aihartza J, Goiti U, Salsamendi E, Garin I (2008) Diet and prey selection in the trawling long-fingered bat. J Zool 274:340– 348 Almenar D, Aihartza J, Goiti U, Salsamendi E, Garin I (2009) Foraging behaviour of the long-fingered bat Myotis capaccinii: implications for conservation and management. Endanger Species Res 8:69–78 Almenar D, Aihartza J, Goiti U, Salsamendi E, Garin I (2011) Reproductive and age classes do not change spatial dynamics of foraging long-fingered bats (Myotis capaccinii). Eur J Wildl Res 57:929–937 Andruskiw M, Fryxell JM, Thompson IA, Baker JA (2008) Habitatmediated variation in predation risk by the American marten. Ecology 89:2273–2280 Armitage PD, Cranston PS, Pinder LCV (1995) The Chironomidae. Biology and ecology of non-biting midges. Chapman and Hall, London Barclay RMR (1982) Interindividual use of echolocation calls: eavesdropping by bats. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 10:271–275 Bennetts RE, Darby PC, Karunaratne LB (2006) Foraging patch selection by snail kites in response to vegetation structure and prey abundance and availability. Waterbirds 29:88–94 Behav Ecol Sociobiol (2013) 67:311–320 Benoit-Bird KJ, Kuletz K, Heppell S, Jones N, Hoover B (2011) Active acoustic examination of the diving behavior of murres foraging on patchy prey. Mar Ecol Prog Ser 443:217–235 Biscardi S, Russo D, Casciani V, Cesarini D, Mei M, Boitani L (2007) Foraging requirements of the endangered long-fingered bat: the influence of micro-habitat structure, water quality and prey type. J Zool 273:372–381 Boonman AM, Boonman M, Bretschneider F, van de Grind WA (1998) Prey detection in trawling insectivorous bats: duckweed affects hunting behaviour in Daubenton’s bat, Myotis daubentonii. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 44:99–107 Bowers MA, Dooley JL (1993) Predation hazard and seed removal by small mammals: microhabitat versus patch scale effects. Oecologia 94:247–254 Britton ARC, Jones G, Rayner JMV, Boonman AM, Verboom B (1997) Flight performance, echolocation and foraging behaviour in pond bats, Myotis dasycneme (Chiroptera: Vespertilionidae). J Zool 241:503–522 Brown JS (1988) Patch use as an indicator of habitat preference, predation risk, and competition. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 22:37–47 Brown JS, Morgan RA (1995) Effects of foraging behavior and spatial scale on diet selectivity: a test with fox squirrels. Oikos 74:122– 136 Butler SJ, Gillings S (2004) Quantifying the effects of habitat structure on prey detectability and accessibility to farmland birds. Ibis 146:123–130 Butler SJ, Whittingham MJ, Quinn JL, Cresswell W (2005) Quantifying the interaction between food density and habitat structure in determining patch selection. Anim Behav 69:337–343 Chan EKW, Zhang Y, Dudgeon D (2009) Substrate availability may be more important than aquatic insect abundance in the distribution of riparian orb-web spiders in the tropics. Biotropica 41:196–201 Charnov EL (1976) Optimal foraging: the marginal value theorem. Theor Popul Biol 9:129–136 Corbet PS (1964) Temporal patterns of emergence in aquatic insects. Can Entomol 96:264–279 Dall SRX, Giraldeau LA, Olsson O, McNamara JM, Stephens DW (2005) Information and its use by animals in evolutionary ecology. Trends Ecol Evol 20:187–193 Dechmann DKN, Heucke SL, Giuggioli L, Safi K, Voigt CT, Wikelski M (2009) Experimental evidence for group hunting via eavesdropping in echolocating bats. Proc R Soc Lond B 276:2721–2728 Encarnaçao JA, Becker NI, Ekschmitt K (2010) When do Daubenton’s bats (Myotis daubentonii) fly far for dinner? Can J Zool 88:1192– 1201 Fenton MB, Bogdanowicz W (2002) Relationships between external morphology and foraging behaviour: bats in the genus Myotis. Can J Zool 80:1004–1013 Flavin DA, Biggane SS, Shiel CB, Smiddy P, Fairley JS (2001) Analysis of the diet of Daubenton’s bat Myotis daubentonii in Ireland. Acta Theriol 46:43–52 Fretwell SD (1972) Populations in a seasonal environment. Princeton University Press, Princeton Fretwell SD, Lucas HLJ (1970) On territorial behavior and other factors influencing habitat distribution in birds. I. Theoretical development. Acta Biotheor 19:16–36 Fukui D, Murakami M, Nakano S, Aoi T (2006) Effect of emergent aquatic insects on bat foraging in a riparian forest. J Anim Ecol 75:1252–1258 Funakoshi K, Takeda Y (1998) Food habits of sympatric insectivorous bats in southern Kyushu, Japan. Mammal Stud 23:49–62 Gathmann FO, Williams DD (2006) Insect emergence in Canadian coldwater springs: spatial and temporal patterns, and species–environment relationships. Int J Limnol 42:143– 156 Behav Ecol Sociobiol (2013) 67:311–320 Getty T, Pulliam HR (1993) Search and prey detection by foraging sparrows. Ecology 74:734–742 Giraldeau LA (1997) The ecology of information use. In: Krebs JR, Davies NB (eds) Behavioural ecology: an evolutionary approach. Blackwell Science, Oxford, pp 42–68 Goiti U, Aihartza J, Almenar D, Salsamendi E, Garin I (2006) Seasonal foraging by Rhinolophus euryale (Rhinolophidae) in an Atlantic rural landscape in northern Iberian Peninsula. Acta Chiropterol 8:141–155 Goiti U, Garin I, Almenar D, Salsamendi E, Aihartza J (2008) Foraging by Mediterranean horseshoe bats (Rhinolophus euryale) in relation to prey distribution and edge habitat. J Mammal 89:493–502 Greenville AC, Dickman CR (2009) Factors affecting habitat selection in a specialist fossorial skink. Biol J Linn Soc 97:531–544 Greif S, Siemers BM (2010) Innate recognition of water bodies in echolocating bats. Nat Commun 1:107 Grindal SD, Brigham RM (1999) Impacts of forest harvesting on habitat use by foraging insectivorous bats at different spatial scales. Ecoscience 6:25–34 Hagen EM, Sabo JL (2011) A landscape perspective on bat ecology along rivers: does channel confinement and insect availability influence the response of bats to aquatic resources in riverine landscapes? Oecologia 166:751–760 Hall LS, Krausman PR, Morrison ML (1997) The habitat concept and a plea for standard terminology. Wildlife Soc Bull 25:173–182 Heithaus MR, Dill LM (2006) Does tiger shark predation risk influence foraging habitat use by bottlenose dolphins at multiple spatial scales? Oikos 114:257–264 Hickey MBC, Fenton MB (1990) Foraging by red bats (Lasiurus borealis): do intraspecific chases mean territoriality? Can J Zool 68:2477–2482 Holbrook SJ, Schmitt RJ (1988) The combined effects of predation risk and food reward on patch selection. Ecology 69:125–134 Holmes RC, Schultz JC (1988) Food availability for forest birds: effects of prey distribution and abundance on bird foraging. Can J Zool 66:720–728 Hopcraft JGC, Sinclair ARE, Packer C (2005) Planning for success: Serengeti lions seek prey accessibility rather than abundance. J Anim Ecol 74:559–566 Hutson AM, Mickleburgh SP, Racey PA (2001) Microchiropteran bats. Global status survey and conservation action plan. IUCN, Gland Jones G, Rayner JMV (1988) Flight performance, foraging tactics and echolocation in free-living Daubenton’s bats Myotis daubentonii (Chiroptera: Vespertilionidae). J Zool 215:113–132 Jones G, Rayner JMV (1991) Flight performance, foraging tactics and echolocation in the trawling insectivorous bat Myotis adversus (Chiroptera: Vespertilionidae). J Zool 225:393–412 Jones G, Siemers BM (2011) The communicative potential of bat echolocation pulses. J Comp Physiol A 197:447–457 Jones KA, Krebs JR, Whittingham MJ (2006) Interaction between seed crypsis and habitat structure influence patch choice in a granivorous bird, the chaffinch Fringilla coelebs. J Avian Biol 37:413– 418 Kalko E (1990) Field study on the echolocation and hunting behaviour of the long-fingered bat, Myotis capaccinii. Bat Res News 31:42– 43 Kalko EKV, Schnitzler HU (1989) The echolocation and hunting behavior of Daubenton’s bat Myotis daubentonii. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 24:225–238 Kelsey MG, Hassall M (1989) Patch selection by dunlin on a heterogeneous mudflat. Ornis Scand 20:250–254 Kusch J, Weber C, Idelberger S, Koob T (2004) Foraging habitat preferences of bats in relation to food supply and spatial vegetation structures in a western European low mountain range forest. Folia Zool 53:113–128 319 Law B, Urquhart CA (2000) Diet of the large-footed myotis Myotis macropus at a forest stream roost in northern New South Wales. Aust Mammal 22:121–124 Levin E, Barnea A, Yovel Y, Yom Tov Y (2006) Have introduced fish initiated piscivory among the long-fingered bat? Mammal Biol 71:139–143 Limpens HJGA, Lina PHC, Hutson AM (1999) Action plan for the conservation of the pond bat (Myotis dasycneme) in Europe. Council of Europe, Strasbourg MacArthur RH, Pianka ER (1966) On the optimal use of a patchy environment. Am Nat 100:603–609 Némoz M, Brisorgueil A (2008) Connaissance et conservation des gîtes et habitats de chasse de 3 Chiroptères cavernicoles. Société Française pour l’Etude et la Protection des Mammifères, Toulouse Neuweiler G (1989) Foraging ecology and audition in echolocating bats. Trends Ecol Evol 4:160–166 Norberg UM, Rayner JMV (1987) Ecological morphology and flight in bats (Mammalia: Chiroptera): wing adaptations, flight performance, foraging strategy and echolocation. Phil Trans R Soc B 316:335–427 Okuyama T (2009) Local interactions between predators and prey call into question commonly used functional responses. Ecol Model 220:1182–1188 Olsson O, Wiktander U, Malmqvist A, Nilsson SG (2001) Variability of patch type preferences in relation to resource availability and breeding success in a bird. Oecologia 127:435–443 Rainho A, Augusto AM, Palmeirim JM (2010) Influence of vegetation clutter on the capacity of ground foraging bats to capture prey. J Appl Ecol 47:850–858 Rice WR (1983) Sensory modality: an example of its effect on optimal foraging behavior. Ecology 64:403–406 Rydell J, Miller LA, Jensen ME (1999) Echolocation constraints of Daubenton’s bat foraging over water. Funct Ecol 13:247–255 Safi K, Siemers B (2010) Implications of sensory ecology for species coexistence: biased perception links predator diversity to prey size distribution. Evol Ecol 24:703–713 Sallabanks R (1993) Hierarchical mechanisms of fruit selection by an avian frugivore. Ecology 74:1326–1336 Scharf I, Ovadia O, Foitzik S (2012) The advantage of alternative tactics of prey and predators depends on the spatial pattern of prey and social interactions among predators. Popul Ecol 54:187– 196 Senft RL, Coughenour MB, Bailey DW, Rittenhouse LR, Sala OE, Swift DM (1987) Large herbivore foraging and ecological hierarchies. Bioscience 37:789–799 Siemers BM, Güttinger R (2006) Prey conspicuousness can explain apparent prey selectivity. Curr Biol 16:157–159 Siemers BM, Stilz P, Schnitzler HU (2001) The acoustic advantage of hunting at low heights above water: behavioural experiments on the European “trawling” bats Myotis capaccinii, M. dasycneme and M. daubentonii. J Exp Biol 204:3843–3854 Siemers BM, Baur E, Schnitzler HU (2005) Acoustic mirror effect increases prey detection distance in trawling bats. Naturwissenschaften 92:272– 276 Sokal RR, Rohlf FJ (1994) Biometry: the principles and practice of statistics in biological research, 3rd edn. Freeman, New York Spitzenberger F, von Helversen O (2001) M. capaccinii (Bonaparte, 1837)—Langfußfledermaus. In: Krapp F (ed) Handbuch der Säugetiere Europas. AULA-Verlag, Wiebelsheim, pp 281–302 Tuttle MD (1974) An improved trap for bats. J Mammal 55:475–477 Uiblein F, Engelke S, Parzefall J (1995) Trade-off between visual detectability and nutrient content in the patch choice of the Pyrenean salamander Euproctus asper. Ethology 101:39–45 Vaughan N (1997) The diets of British bats (Chiroptera). Mammal Rev 27:77–94 320 von Frenckell B, Barclay RMR (1987) Bat activity over calm and turbulent water. Can J Zool 65:219–222 Warren RD, Waters DA, Altringham JD, Bullock DJ (2000) The distribution of Daubenton’s bats (Myotis daubentonii) and pipistrelle bats (Pipistrellus pipistrellus) (Vespertilionidae) in relation to small-scale variation in riverine habitat. Biol Conserv 92:85–91 Behav Ecol Sociobiol (2013) 67:311–320 Whelan C (2001) Foliage structure influences foraging of insectivorous forest birds: an experimental study. Ecology 82:219–231 White GC, Garrot RA (1990) Analysis of wildlife radio-tracking data. Academic, London Wilkinson GS (1992) Information transfer at evening bat colonies. Anim Behav 44:501–518