[go: up one dir, main page]

Skip to main content
Log in

Combining tDCS and Working Memory Training to Down Regulate State Rumination: A Single-Session Double Blind Sham-Controlled Trial

  • Brief Report
  • Published:
Cognitive Therapy and Research Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Rumination has been associated with reduced working memory and dorsolateral prefrontal cortex activity. This study explored whether single session anodal transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS) applied to the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex and/or working memory training can transiently ameliorate working memory and down regulate state rumination. Sixty-six participants were randomly allocated to three conditions: (1) control training + tDCS, (2) working memory training + sham tDCS and (3) working memory training + tDCS. Before and after manipulation participants performed working memory tasks and state rumination was measured with self-report and heart rate variability. Participants who received real tDCS were significantly faster in switching between information in working memory than participants who received sham tDCS. No effects on self-reported state rumination were found. However, both groups receiving working memory training showed a higher increase in heart rate variability than the control training group, indicating more adaptive self-regulation.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Subscribe and save

Springer+ Basic
$34.99 /Month
  • Get 10 units per month
  • Download Article/Chapter or eBook
  • 1 Unit = 1 Article or 1 Chapter
  • Cancel anytime
Subscribe now

Buy Now

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4

Notes

  1. Analysis based on correct trials only did not alter the conclusions.

References

  • Appelhans, B. M., & Luecken, L. J. (2006). Heart rate variability as an index of regulated emotional responding. Review of General Psychology, 10(3), 229–240. doi:10.1037/1089-2680.10.3.229.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Baddeley, A. D., & Hitch, G. (1974). Working memory. In G. A. Bower (Ed.), The psychology of learning and motivation (Vol. 8, pp. 47–89). New York, NY: Academia Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bai, S. W., Dokos, S., Ho, K. A., & Loo, C. (2014). A computational modelling study of transcranial direct current stimulation montages used in depression. Neuroimage, 87, 332–344. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.11.015.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Berman, M. G., Peltier, S., Nee, D. E., Kross, E., Deldin, P. J., & Jonides, J. (2011). Depression, rumination and the default network. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 6(5), 548–555. doi:10.1093/scan/nsq080.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Berntson, G. G., & Cacioppo, J. T. (2004). Heart rate variability: Stress and psychiatric conditions. In M. Malik & A. J. Camm (Eds.), Dynamic electrocardiography (pp. 56–63). New York, NY: Blackwell Futura.

    Google Scholar 

  • Broadway, J. M., & Engle, R. W. (2010). Validating running memory span: Measurement of working memory capacity and links with fluid intelligence. Behavior Research Methods, 42(2), 563–570. doi:10.3758/brm.42.2.563.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Brunoni, A. R., Boggio, P. S., De Raedt, R., Bensenor, I. M., Lotufo, P. A., Namur, V., & Vanderhasselt, M. A. (2014). Cognitive control therapy and transcranial direct current stimulation for depression: A randomized, double-blinded, controlled trial. Journal of Affective Disorders, 162, 43–49. doi:10.1016/j.jad.2014.03.026.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Brunoni, A. R., Ferrucci, R., Bortolomasi, M., Vergari, M., Tadini, L., Boggio, P. S., & Priori, A. (2011). Transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS) in unipolar vs. bipolar depressive disorder. Progress in Neuro-Psychopharmacology and Biological Psychiatry, 35(1), 96–101. doi:10.1016/j.pnpbp.2010.09.010.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Brunoni, A. R., & Vanderhasselt, M. A. (2014). Working memory improvement with non-invasive brain stimulation of the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Brain and Cognition, 86, 1–9. doi:10.1016/j.bandc.2014.01.008.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cabeza, R., & Nyberg, L. (2000). Imaging cognition II: An empirical review of 275 PET and fMRI studies. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 12(1), 1–47. doi:10.1162/08989290051137585.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Chambers, R., Lo, B. C. Y., & Allen, N. B. (2008). The impact of intensive mindfulness training on attentional control, cognitive style, and affect. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 32(3), 303–322. doi:10.1007/s10608-007-9119-0.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Corbetta, M., & Shulman, G. L. (2002). Control of goal-directed and stimulus-driven attention in the brain. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 3(3), 201–215. doi:10.1038/nrn755.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Curtis, C. E., & D’Esposito, M. (2003). Persistent activity in the prefrontal cortex during working memory. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7(9), 415–423. doi:10.1016/S1364-6613(03)00197-9.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • De Lissnyder, E., Koster, E. H. W., & De Raedt, R. (2012a). Emotional interference in working memory is related to rumination. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 36(4), 348–357. doi:10.1007/s10608-011-9352-4.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • De Lissnyder, E., Koster, E. H. W., Everaert, J., Schacht, R., Van den Abeele, D., & De Raedt, R. (2012b). Internal cognitive control in clinical depression: General but no emotion-specific impairments. Psychiatry Research, 199(2), 124–130. doi:10.1016/j.psychres.2012.04.019.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • De Raedt, R., & Koster, E. H. W. (2010). Understanding vulnerability for depression from a cognitive neuroscience perspective: A reappraisal of attentional factors and a new conceptual framework. Cognitive Affective and Behavioral Neuroscience, 10(1), 50–70. doi:10.3758/cabn.10.1.50.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dockery, C. A., Hueckel-Weng, R., Birbaumer, N., & Plewnia, C. (2009). Enhancement of planning ability by transcranial direct current stimulation. The Journal of Neuroscience, 29(22), 7271–7277. doi:10.1523/jneurosci.0065-09.2009.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Draine, S. (2011). Inquisit (Version 3.0.5.0) [Computer software]. Seattle, WA: Millisecond Software.

  • Elliott, P., & Hawthorne, G. (2005). Imputing missing repeated measures data: How should we proceed? Australian and New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry, 39(7), 575–582. doi:10.1111/j.1440-1614.2005.01629.x.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Eysenck, M. W., Derakshan, N., Santos, R., & Calvo, M. G. (2007). Anxiety and cognitive performance: Attentional control theory. Emotion, 7(2), 336–353. doi:10.1037/1528-3542.7.2.336.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fregni, F., Boggio, P. S., Nitsche, M., Bermpohl, F., Antal, A., Feredoes, E., & Pascual-Leone, A. (2005). Anodal transcranial direct current stimulation of prefrontal cortex enhances working memory. Experimental Brain Research, 166(1), 23–30. doi:10.1007/s00221-005-2334-6.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Goeleven, E., De Raedt, R., Leyman, L., & Verschuere, B. (2008). The Karolinska directed emotional faces: A validation study. Cognition and Emotion, 22(6), 1094–1118. doi:10.1080/02699930701626582.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hansen, A. L., Johnsen, B. H., & Thayer, J. F. (2003). Vagal influence on working memory and attention. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 48(3), 263–274. doi:10.1016/S0167-8760(03)00073-4.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hoskinson, P., & Toomim, J. (2008). Brain Workshop (Version 4.8.1) [Computer Software]. Retrieved from http://brainworkshop.sourceforge.net.

  • IBM Corp. (2011). IBM SPSS statistics for windows, version 20.0. Armonk, NY: IBM Corp.

  • Jaeggi, S. M., Buschkuehl, M., Jonides, J., & Perrig, W. J. (2008). Improving fluid intelligence with training on working memory. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 105(19), 6829–6833. doi:10.1073/pnas.0801268105.

    Article  PubMed Central  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Joormann, J. (2005). Inhibition, rumination, and mood regulation in depression. In R. W. Engle, G. Sedek, U. von Hecker, & D. N. McIntosh (Eds.), Cognitive limitations in aging and psychopathology: Attention, working memory, and executive functions (pp. 275–312). New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Joormann, J., & Gotlib, I. H. (2008). Updating the contents of working memory in depression: Interference from irrelevant negative material. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 117(1), 182–192. doi:10.1037/0021-843x.117.1.182.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kaufmann, T., Sütterlin, S., Schulz, S., & Vögele, C. (2011). ARTiiFACT: A tool for heart rate artifact processing and heart rate variability analysis. Behavior Research Methods, 43(4), 1161–1170. doi:10.3758/s13428-011-0107-7.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Koster, E. H. W., De Lissnyder, E., & De Raedt, R. (2013). Rumination is characterized by valence-specific impairments in switching of attention. Acta Psychologica, 144(3), 563–570. doi:10.1016/j.actpsy.2013.09.008.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lundqvist, D., Flykt, A., & Öhman, A. (1998). The Karolinska directed emotional faces (KDEF). Stockholm: Department of Neurosciences, Karolinska Hospital.

    Google Scholar 

  • Malik, M. (1996). Heart rate variability. Annals of Noninvasive Electrocardiology, 1(2), 151–181. doi:10.1111/j.1542-474X.1996.tb00275.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Marchetti, I., Koster, E. H. W., & De Raedt, R. (2013). Rest-related dynamics of risk and protective factors for depression: A behavioral study. Clinical Psychological Science. doi:10.1177/2167702613489668.

    Google Scholar 

  • Martin, D. M., Liu, R., Alonzo, A., Green, M., Player, M. J., Sachdev, P., & Loo, C. K. (2013). Can transcranial direct current stimulation enhance outcomes from cognitive training? A randomized controlled trial in healthy participants. International Journal of Neuropsychopharmacology, 16(9), 1927–1936. doi:10.1017/s1461145713000539.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • McNair, D. M., Lorr, M., & Droppelman, L. F. (1971). Manual for the profile of mood states. San Diego, CA: Educational and Industrial Testing Services.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mor, N., Marchetti, I., & Koster, E. H. W. (2013). The Momentary Ruminative Self-focus Inventory (MRSI): Validation and psychometric evaluation. Manuscript submitted for publication.

  • Nitsche, M. A., Cohen, L. G., Wassermann, E. M., Priori, A., Lang, N., Antal, A., & Pascual-Leone, A. (2008). Transcranial direct current stimulation: State of the art 2008. Brain Stimulation, 1(3), 206–223. doi:10.1016/j.brs.2008.06.004.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Nitz, D. (2010). Running memory span task. Retrieved from http://www.millisecond.com/download/library/RunningSpan/.

  • Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (1991). Responses to depression and their effects on the duration of depressive episodes. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 100(4), 569–582. doi:10.1037/0021-843x.100.4.569.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Nolen-Hoeksema, S., & Jackson, B. (2001). Mediators of the gender difference in rumination. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 25(1), 37–47. doi:10.1111/1471-6402.00005.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nolen-Hoeksema, S., & Morrow, J. (1991). A prospective study of depression and posttraumatic stress symptoms after a natural disaster. The 1989 Loma-Prieta earthquake. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 61(1), 115–121. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.61.1.115.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Nolen-Hoeksema, S., Wisco, B. E., & Lyubomirsky, S. (2008). Rethinking rumination. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 3(5), 400–424. doi:10.1111/j.1745-6924.2008.00088.x.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ottaviani, C., Shapiro, D., Davydov, D. M., Goldstein, I. B., & Mills, P. J. (2009). The autonomic phenotype of rumination. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 72(3), 267–275. doi:10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2008.12.014.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Psychology Software Tools Inc. (2007). E-Prime (Version 2.0). Pittsburgh, PA: Psychology Software Tools Inc.

  • Raes, F., Schoofs, H., Hoes, D., Hermans, D., Van Den Eede, F., & Franck, E. (2009). ‘Reflection’ en ‘brooding’ als subtypes van rumineren: Een herziening van de Ruminative Response Scale. Gedragstherapie, 42, 205–214.

    Google Scholar 

  • Richmond, L. L., Wolk, D., Chein, J., & Olson, I. R. (2014). Transcranial direct current stimulation enhances verbal working memory training performance over time and near transfer outcomes. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 26(11), 2443–2454. doi:10.1162/jocn_a_00657.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Segrave, R. A., Arnold, S., Hoy, K., & Fitzgerald, P. B. (2014). Concurrent cognitive control training augments the antidepressant efficacy of tDCS: A pilot study. Brain Stimulation, 7(2), 325–331. doi:10.1016/j.brs.2013.12.008.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sheehan, D. V., Lecrubier, Y., Sheehan, K. H., Amorim, P., Janavs, J., Weiller, E., & Dunbar, G. C. (1998). The mini-international neuropsychiatric interview (MINI): The development and validation of a structured diagnostic psychiatric interview for DSM-IV and ICD-10. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 59, 22–33.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Smith, T. W., Gallo, L. C., Goble, L., Ngu, L. Q., & Stark, K. A. (1998). Agency, communion, and cardiovascular reactivity during marital interaction. Health Psychology, 17(6), 537–545. doi:10.1037//0278-6133.17.6.537.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Thayer, J. F., Åhs, F., Fredrikson, M., Sollers Iii, J. J., & Wager, T. D. (2012). A meta-analysis of heart rate variability and neuroimaging studies: Implications for heart rate variability as a marker of stress and health. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 36(2), 747–756. doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2011.11.009.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Treynor, W., Gonzalez, R., & Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (2003). Rumination reconsidered: A psychometric analysis. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 27(3), 247–259. doi:10.1023/A:1023910315561.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Verkuil, B., Brosschot, J. F., de Beurs, D. P., & Thayer, J. F. (2009). Effects of explicit and implicit perseverative cognition on cardiac recovery after cognitive stress. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 74(3), 220–228. doi:10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2009.09.003.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wald, F. D. M., & Mellenbergh, G. J. (1990). De verkorte versie van de Nederlandse vertaling van de Profile of Mood States (POMS). Nederlands Tijdschrift voor de Psychologie, 45, 86–90.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wicherts, J. M., & Vorst, H. C. M. (2004). Modelpassing van de Verkorte Profile of Mood States en meetinvariantie over mannen en vrouwen. Nederlands Tijdschrift voor de Psychologie en haar Grensgebieden, 59(1), 12–21.

    Google Scholar 

  • Woody, M. L., McGeary, J. E., & Gibb, B. E. (2014). Brooding rumination and heart rate variability in women at high and low risk for depression: Group differences and moderation by COMT genotype. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 123(1), 61–67. doi:10.1037/a0035450.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Zaehle, T., Sandmann, P., Thorne, J. D., Jancke, L., & Herrmann, C. S. (2011). Transcranial direct current stimulation of the prefrontal cortex modulates working memory performance: Combined behavioural and electrophysiological evidence. BMC Neuroscience, 12, 11. doi:10.1186/1471-2202-12-2.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

This research was supported by a Grant of the Special Research Fund (BOF) of Ghent University (B/13811/01). Marie-Anne Vanderhasselt is a postdoctoral fellow of the Research Foundation Flanders (FWO) (FWO08/PDO/168). This research was also supported by the Ghent University Multidisciplinary Research Partnership “The integrative neuroscience of behavioural control”. The authors would like to thank Yentl Van den Broeck for her assistance with data collection.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Laura M. S. De Putter.

Ethics declarations

Conflict of Interest

Laura M. S. De Putter, Marie-Anne Vanderhasselt, Chris Baeken, Rudi De Raedt and Ernst H. W. Koster declare that they have no conflict of interest.

Ethical Standard

The present study was approved by the Medical Ethical Committee of Ghent University Hospital. All procedures followed were in accordance with the ethical standards of this responsible committee on experiments involving human participants.

Informed Consent

Informed consent was obtained from all individual participants included in the study.

Animal Rights

This article does not contain any studies with animals performed by any of the authors.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

De Putter, L.M.S., Vanderhasselt, MA., Baeken, C. et al. Combining tDCS and Working Memory Training to Down Regulate State Rumination: A Single-Session Double Blind Sham-Controlled Trial. Cogn Ther Res 39, 754–765 (2015). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10608-015-9710-8

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10608-015-9710-8

Keywords