How Did You Count
How Did You Count
How Did You Count
C H R I S TO P H E R D A N I E L S O N
Cover and select interior photography by Asha Belk
and by Routledge
4 Park Square, Milton Park, Abingdon, Oxon, OX14 4RN
The right of Christopher Danielson to be identified as author of this work has been
asserted in accordance with sections 77 and 78 of the Copyright, Designs and Patents
Act 1988.
All rights reserved. No part of this book may be reprinted or reproduced or utilised in
any form or by any electronic, mechanical, or other means, now known or hereafter
invented, including photocopying and recording, or in any information storage or
retrieval system, without permission in writing from the publishers.
Typeset in Gotham
by KnowledgeWorks Global Ltd.
Acknowledgments . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . viii
References . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 135
Acknowledgments
This book has spent years in incubation. As a result, many, many people have had a
chance to influence, nurture, and support the work along the way. I am grateful to all of
them, and I need to express that gratitude to some of them directly.
Thanks to Simon Gregg for offering frequent glimpses into his and his colleagues’
classrooms, and for sharing his students’ thinking via annotations—both publicly
on Twitter, and behind the scenes. Thank you, Lauren Baucom for your stories, the
photograph in Chapter 2, and your careful and thoughtful review of the mathematics in
that chapter. Thanks to Anand Thakker and his daughter for staging a penny-counting
photograph, to Chris and Chase Lusto for their story about pairs of things, to Faith and
Clem Moynihan for telling me about three (the magic number), and to Chris Nho for his
fabulous animation of the tetrahedron of tangerines, stills from which I have included in
Chapter 4.
Thank you, Kristin Thomas for being a kind and supportive friend and colleague, for
inviting me into your classroom to play with first graders, and for letting me use your
interactive whiteboard for the annotations in Chapter 5.
Thanks to Margaret Williams for organizing my classroom visits, and to classroom
teachers Chelsea Zerr, Leslie Witucki, Megan Beranek, Kate Schaffer, Janene Unke,
Nicole Egan, Hillary Schraut, Danielle Plumley, and Cassie Alexis. You all hosted me
graciously in your classrooms, and I am grateful. Please pass my thanks along to your
beautiful, diverse, and brilliant students for sharing their thinking.
Thank you to my former student Marissa Brown for taking such a perfect photograph
for noticing the associative property of multiplication. Thanks to Brynn DeVaan for her
diligence with the sphere-stacking frame designs which make cameo appearances in
Chapters 2 and 4, and to Lauren Siegel for getting us started with sphere-stacking play
in the first place.
Tracy Zager edited early versions of the Teacher’s Guide with her usual thoughtfulness,
grace, and critical questioning. I am deeply grateful. Kassia Wedekind picked up
where Tracy left off, and has been an equally worthy adversary… er… editor, colleague,
viii
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
and friend. Asha Belk took the beautiful photographs that fill the student book. Her
camera skills and artistic eye made this book gorgeous. Her patience, thoughtfulness,
and attention to detail made it better. Thank you, Asha! Thank you, too, to the entire
Stenhouse team, past and present, and especially to Dan Tobin and Toby Gordon who
first saw a place at Stenhouse for books that go into the hands of both children and
adults. Thank you to Helen Strain, Sarah Fish, Melanie Moy, and the entire production
team for making this book beautiful as you have the previous ones.
Online Materials
Please use this QR code to access the book’s digital content. Scroll down to the
“Support Materials” tab and enter the password when prompted.
https://www.routledge.com/9781625312938
ix
Taylor & Francis
Taylor & Francis Group
http://taylorandfrancis.com
CHAPTER 1
N
umbers are funny.
I mean, what even are they? Do they actually exist, or are they products
of our imagination? What does five mean? These are questions that
mathematicians, philosophers, and young children all consider at some point.
The rest of us spend most of our time unaware that we don’t actually know the answers.
Fittingly then, I’d like to open this book by telling you a story that a mathematician
passed along to me a while back.
Peter is a university mathematician, married to a mathematician colleague of mine at
the time, Peggy. Peggy introduced me to Peter at a wedding reception, and I came to
understand that Peter was a fan of my Talking Math with Your Kids work. He was eager
to relate to me an interaction he had recently witnessed.
A young child—perhaps three or four years old—was asked, “Do you have more
fingers on your left hand, or on your right hand?” Many of us—including Peter—would
have expected the child to know and use the fact that they had five fingers on each
hand to answer that question, or perhaps to count them.
Instead, the child responded without counting, but by matching the fingers thumb-to-
thumb, index-to-index, etc.
“The child invented one-to-one correspondence!” my mathematician friend exclaimed
with pleasure. That a young child could answer a question about numbers without
counting was delightful to him, in part because it challenges our ideas about numbers
and what they are for. It certainly challenges the perception most of us have about the
sophistication of young children’s minds.
1
HOW DID YOU COUNT?
Another friend and colleague of mine recently asked, “If I know that there are four
groups of five things, but I don’t know that’s twenty altogether, have I actually counted
those things?” A more extreme version of that question might ask, “If I know that there
are two groups of one hundred things, or two hundreds of them, but I don’t know that’s
two hundred, have I actually counted those things?”
Like I said, numbers are funny.
How Did You Count? proceeds from an assumption that we know enough about what
numbers are to function in the world. A good-enough definition of numbers is that they
are adjectives that describe how many things there are. I bought twenty avocados is a
sentence with the same structure as I bought green avocados. Twenty and green are
both adjectives. Unlike colors, there are lots of ways to know which number adjective to
use. There are lots of ways to know how many.
We’ll call all of these counting. Counting is how you know how many.
OK. That’s not quite true. If you know at a glance how many dots are on a die,
you have subitized rather than counted. And if a family member tells you there
are nine eggs remaining in the carton, then here too you know a quantity without
counting.
But by and large, the work you do to figure out how many of something there are—or
were, or could, might, or will be—are forms of counting.
Colloquially, counting means one at a time. Point to the first object, say one; point to
the next and say two, and so on, where the last number you say while pointing to the
final object is the size of the set—the count. Early in elementary school, children learn to
count by other convenient numbers. Twos are nice because they are easy to see and to
keep track of. Fives and tens require some preparation work, but that work pays off if
you’re counting to a number such as 85 or 152.
Teacher Lauren Carr has frequently documented her students’ beautiful and brilliant
counting work on social media. A while back, she shared a video in which students look
at each other’s groupings of large collections of objects and deduce the underlying
counting strategy. For the arrangement pictured in Figure 1.1, we hear a student count,
“five, ten, fifteen, twenty,” as they gesture at the larger groups of goldfish crackers
across the top of the image. They continue from there, counting by twos across the
2
W hat C ou n t s a s C ou n ti n g ?
FIGURE 1.1
Counting goldfish
middle row, “twenty-two, twenty-four…” and ending in the lower right-hand corner,
having counted fifty goldfish.
Ms. Carr’s students have both represented (by making groups) and interpreted
(by skip-counting the groups their classmates made) important and useful number
structures. These young mathematicians are learning important things about
counting.
Each of these—counting objects individually or in pairs or in groups of five or ten—
involves reciting a number sequence (one, two, three… or two, four, six… etc.). This
recitation is one meaning of the word counting.
This book is concerned with a broader sense of counting. Here, counting is an activity
whose purpose is knowing how many, and one that relies on structure. If I count a
collection of buttons individually, the structure I’m using is one-to-one correspondence.
3
HOW DID YOU COUNT?
FIGURE 1.2
Shoes
4
W hat C ou n t s a s C ou n ti n g ?
In my early work with How Many? I saw this conversation differently than I do now.
Initially, I took “I counted” to be a tactic for not stating a strategy in full—perhaps
because doing so is difficult, or perhaps because doing so was too risky in front of
their peers, or in front of me. Perhaps it was the only answer they could imagine to
the question, “How do you know there are that many?” I saw I counted as an evasion;
a way of engaging less fully. My follow-up question, “Did you say eighteen?," was a
strategic move to counter that perceived evasion. I suspected they were telling me
they did something (counting one-by-one) that they didn’t actually do; that they
were hiding—or unable to make explicit—the patterns and structures they were
using.
Now I understand that when I ask, “How do you know?," children have several
categories of ways of knowing, including these:
• I just know
• My friend told me
• I guessed
• I counted
When I look at it this way, I can see that “I counted” isn’t an evasion of the “How do
you know?” question—none of these responses is. Instead, it is a good faith effort at
categorizing among several different ways of knowing. This shift in thinking led me to
start asking a new follow-up question: “How did you count?”
I used to see “I counted” as something I needed to push back on; I wanted to
demonstrate that in fact this child didn’t count; they had instead done something more
interesting and sophisticated. Now I see “I counted” as an invitation from the child to
talk about how they counted. This book accepts that invitation, and then issues it to all. I
want us to invite everyone to the counting party.
5
HOW DID YOU COUNT?
Here are five common ways children know that there are twenty eyelets on the shoes
in Figure 1.2:
• one-by-one
• by twos
• by fives
Children have offered each of these while answering the question, “How did you count?”
They understand something that I did not: counting is a way of knowing how many
there are. In this Teacher’s Guide, I’ll explore relationships among these various counting
strategies.
This book is in part a result of my wanting to explore children’s ways of counting,
and of wanting to support children in developing new ways of counting for themselves
in conversation with their peers and caregivers. Also, this book is the product of many
years of my own learning to see the structure of numbers.
An important stage in this learning took place about ten years ago, when I was
teaching at a community college. I designed a series of small assignments for
the students in my math course for elementary and special education teachers in
order to help them to investigate math in their lives, and outside of textbooks. I
had been thinking about the frequent claim that “math is everywhere,” and about
relationships between children’s everyday math experiences and the math they
study in school. I had also been learning with online colleagues about capturing
students’ curiosity with imagery. Unlike at the beginning of my teaching career,
most of my college students had instant access to digital photography through their
cell phones (and those who did not still had less-than-instant access via friends and
family members).
6
W hat C ou n t s a s C ou n ti n g ?
FIGURE 1.3
Shoe rack
7
HOW DID YOU COUNT?
Did you see either four rows of six shoes or twelve pairs of shoes? Maybe you saw
something different, but these two are the most common due to the structure of the
rack (rows) and the structure of shoes (pairs). Seeing the shoes either way leads to
a total of twenty-four shoes, but the first way has us thinking about 4 × 6 while the
second way has us thinking about 12 × 2.
On one level, this was a lovely example of an array photograph. On another level, it
was an example of the associative property of multiplication (about which I will say
a great deal more in Chapter 2), something I had previously only considered to be an
abstract truth about numbers.
Formally stated, the associative property of multiplication is this:
But here was a photo that helped me and my students see the associative property as
being about groups.
Or in this case:
8
W hat C ou n t s a s C ou n ti n g ?
classrooms with these images, children noticed, described, and used relationships that I
had not noticed myself.
In Chapter 2 of this Teacher’s Guide, I’ll describe the math I embedded in these
images, and I’ll explain the importance of that math for a range of elementary school
learners, as well as for secondary students.
Chapter 3 is about implementation. How can you use these images in classrooms
and other settings? How can you support students in generalizing from the particular
example of a single image to other places and ways that the same ideas and structures
appear elsewhere?
In Chapter 4, I’ll shift to the mathematics that children see in these images, and how
they describe it. Finally, Chapter 5 is an answer key of sorts, where you can quickly
find correct totals, as well as
common counting strategies for
each image.
But first, I’ll end this chapter
with a short description of the
difference between the math I
intended and the math children
notice.
The big dice image (Figure
1.4) consists of five groups
of five groups of five dots.
There are 5 x 5 x 5, or 53 dots
on the tops of these dice.
This repeated multiplication
introduces exponents in the
same way that repeated
addition sometimes introduces
FIGURE 1.4
Many dots
9
HOW DID YOU COUNT?
FIGURE 1.5
So many dots
10
CHAPTER 2
M
y friend Lauren’s daughter Naomi happened across a tire gauge (Figure 2.1)
one day when she was four and a half years old. Perhaps the tire gauge was
in a toolbox, or a junk drawer, or just lying around in anticipation of some
upcoming automotive maintenance.
FIGURE 2.1
A tire gauge that goes all the way
to (and perhaps beyond?) tenty ten.
11
HOW DID YOU COUNT?
and use quantities, but the ways we write and say numbers are invented by people.
Children encounter those ways as they go about their everyday lives. They overhear
conversations; they read books and play games with their parents; they help out around
the house; they investigate the objects that surround them, and along the way they
notice, wonder, imagine, think, and experiment.
Naomi gives us a glimpse of this fact. She has heard the words ten, twenty, and so on
many times. She has probably not heard the word tenty many times, and possibly she
never has. She—like many children learning to count in English—probably invented it as
the logical extension of the patterns in the words. It’s not correct in the sense of being
conventional, but it is correct in the sense of being logical, and in that other people can
know what she means.
I think of the primary audience for this book as teachers—it is a Teacher's Guide after
all. Nonetheless, I have tried to write in a way that can speak to anyone with an interest
in children’s mathematical development. This includes parents and other caregivers, and
also scholarly folks who may not have any regular, direct contact with children learning
mathematics, but who work with future or practicing teachers.
I also want to address the term mathematician at the outset of this chapter, as
it appears several times here. I think of mathematics as a discipline—one that has
practices, language, and customs that are useful for studying numbers, patterns, and
space. (Please forgive the vague and incomplete nature of this characterization; it is
intended to be good enough to sketch out a big picture, not to be a rigorous definition.
Writing a more rigorous definition of mathematics is a very interesting thing to attempt,
but not essential to the current project.)
Anyone engaging in these practices is acting—in that moment—as a mathematician.
We may be accustomed to mathematician applying only to people who work at a
university, perhaps with a chalkboard on their office wall, and who publish mathematical
papers. I used to think this way. I used to think doing mathematics meant expanding the
boundaries of human knowledge—developing new ideas that the world had never known
before. Now I understand that doing mathematics means expanding the boundaries of
your own knowledge using the tools of the discipline. Naomi, in noticing a pattern in
the numbers on the tire gauge and inventing language (tenty) to describe that pattern,
12
H o w C o u nting D e v el o ps
was very much behaving as a mathematician. When I use the term mathematician
in this chapter, I am referring to the larger community, and I will use professional
mathematician to refer to the subset of that community that consists of people spending
their professional lives discovering and inventing ideas that are new to the community.
You—the reader—and I are members of this community of mathematicians. Most of
us adults have little memory of our own process of learning to count, the challenges
and the triumphs. In this chapter, I’ll highlight some typical landmarks for young
mathematicians in this process, beginning with ways of understanding quantities
that we come by naturally, and moving forward through much of elementary school
arithmetic and far, far beyond.
Let’s begin with numbers that don’t require counting: one, two, and three.
SUBITIZING
As an exercise, look quickly at the sets of dots in Figure 2.2 and say how many dots are
in each set. If you were to this while hooked up to an elaborate machine that displays
such collections, with randomly chosen quantities and arrangements, for only a brief
period of time, and that measures in milliseconds the time it takes you to say how many
there are, then the results will probably be that you recognize one, two, or three objects
in about the same amount of time. The time you need to identify four, five, or six objects,
however, will grow at a roughly constant rate with each additional object. The standard
interpretation of these results is that there is something different going on in your
brain when you recognize three objects at a glance than when you count five objects.
The name for this number-
recognition-without-counting
process is subitizing, and from
experiments with infants (and
certain animals, including crows),
it appears to be preverbal and
FIGURE 2.2
Arbitrary arrangements of dots
13
HOW DID YOU COUNT?
FIGURE 2.3
Quincunx
14
H o w C o u nting D e v el o ps
FIGURE 2.4
How many dots (with a quick
glance)?
15
HOW DID YOU COUNT?
I will call the work children are doing counting in order to show how it provides
connections between counting objects one-by-one and the generalized ideas of
algebra. The story of counting itself begins at the beginning, with the whole number
counting sequence.
16
H o w C o u nting D e v el o ps
Each of these features is instructive for what it means to learn to count in English.
She counts one to twelve correctly and confidently. Counting one to twelve is not
a logical process. If you know the English words for one through seven, you cannot
guess the word for eight. You don’t figure out how to count from one to twelve; you
memorize it. You memorize it through repetition—reading counting books, counting
along to animated Sesame Street videos, watching and listening to parents, teachers,
older siblings, and other caregivers either deliberately demonstrating this counting or
naturally counting as they go about their daily lives.
These are all opportunities to absorb the language of counting, and young
children’s minds are uniquely optimized for learning language. Children learn initial
counting words in the same way they learn color words and the names of the letters
of the alphabet—through a combination of deliberate exposure and immersion in
culture.
She skips some teens. The numbers thirteen through nineteen have a structure—
something derived from the initial counting sequence, followed by the syllable teen.
Some of these number names are recognizable from earlier numbers: seventeen,
eighteen, nineteen, but others—thirteen and fifteen—are more disguised. Furthermore,
children encounter these numbers with lower frequency. You probably know lots of
counting books that stop at ten or twelve, relatively fewer that continue to twenty.
And anytime you count to twenty by ones, you get there by way of the numbers one
through twelve. The reverse is not true—counting to twelve doesn’t involve reciting any
teens, nor saying twenty. This combination of imperfect structure and lower frequency
means that young children frequently have a mental model of one, two, … eleven,
twelve, and then some teens.
She skips twenty. The twenties are well structured. Once you know twenty-one, you
can guess twenty-two, and keep guessing straight on to twenty-nine. (Why did Tabitha
skip twenty-seven? Who knows? She’s four.) But skipping twenty is extremely common.
This is because the sequence that gives the twenties their structure—the original one
through nine sequence—starts at one. If we started counting at zero, you might expect
Tabitha to say eighteen, nineteen, twenty-zero, twenty-one. While uncommon, twenty-
zero is a thing young children occasionally say. Another common follow-up to nineteen
17
HOW DID YOU COUNT?
is tenteen, and all of these errors are strong indications that children are working with
the patterns and structure of number language at this point, rather than memorizing as
they must one through twelve.
She doesn’t get past twenty-nine. Children rarely get stuck on twenty-eight or
twenty-five, but very commonly stop counting at twenty-nine, because the structure
has run out and requires a new word that you cannot guess: thirty. Here, children
learning to count in some other languages—such as Japanese—have an advantage over
children learning English, French, or Spanish. If you know that ten-nine is followed by
two-tens, two-tens-one, and so on, you might be able to guess that two-tens-nine is
followed by three-tens. But the transition from nineteen to twenty doesn’t really help
you know that twenty-nine is followed by thirty. You might guess twenty-ten, twenty-
eleven, and so on, and often children do count this way. At the moment captured in the
video, Tabitha did not. She seemed to know that numbers don’t end at twenty-nine,
so perhaps she threw a couple more twenties in for good measure before quitting. It
is unlikely that she would count twenty-nine, twenty-three, twenty-eight again if asked
to film the video again the next day, but it is very likely that she would again falter at
twenty-nine.
As a postscript, I am pleased to report that Tabitha is now a teenager, and she counts
fluently without needing remediation anywhere along the way. Her counting errors
documented one day more than ten years ago are a sign of her learning and growth as
a learner, not indications anyone should be concerned.
To summarize, learning the sequence of counting words in your home language
is part of what it means to learn to count. It involves a combination of rote
memorization through exposure and practice, and logical analysis of patterns. All
languages involve some of each; some languages support pattern seeking more
robustly than others.
Being able to recite the number sequence in your home language isn’t all there is
to counting, though. Just as you can memorize a nonsense poem without it having
meaning, so too can you memorize a sequence of counting words and not understand
its relationship to quantities. Another skill you need is recognizing one-to-one
correspondence.
18
H o w C o u nting D e v el o ps
ONE-TO-ONE CORRESPONDENCE
Before reading this next section, consider taking a moment for an exercise. Look at
Figure 2.5 and do two things. First, count the radishes in the left-hand image. Second,
think about what the photograph on the right might demonstrate. Then read on.
One-to-one correspondence means matching up things from two sets—one from set
A with one from set B. It is why socks come in pairs; why in addition to the one that
goes around your waist, underpants have two holes (and some have three), and why
gloves don’t keep your fingers as warm as mittens. Mittens do not have a one-to-one
correspondence between compartments and your fingers, so your fingers can keep
each other warm. One-to-one correspondence is how a child once claimed they knew
there were five radishes in each half of the radish circle. The child had five fingers, and
matched each finger to one radish. That’s five radishes per half, so ten radishes total.
FIGURE 2.5
How many radishes (left)? Demonstrating a set of five radishes (right). Sadly, the original image I
was sent is lost to time and bad electronic file organization, so I have reproduced the image with
my own much larger and much less adorable hand.
19
HOW DID YOU COUNT?
In each of these examples, the sets consist of physical objects such as fingers, legs,
and radishes. That doesn’t have to be the case. A set might consist of intangible things.
In particular, a set may consist of number words that you say while counting out loud.
In learning to count, you need to learn to say exactly one counting word for each
object; set A is the counting words you say; set B is a bunch of cookies on a plate.
Counting the cookies involves pointing to each cookie in turn, while saying the next
number in the sequence. This requires learning to coordinate multiple challenging
activities, and mistakes are common early on, including:
• Counting and pointing at different rates. One form of this is the smooth sweep where
the child moves their pointer finger at a steady speed while reciting numbers without
coordinating the motion and the recitation.
• Pointing to an object twice—perhaps at the beginning and again at the end of the count.
• Pointing to an object, but not saying a new number. (This is especially common
when working through numbers such as eleven or twenty-seven, which have multiple
syllables in English.)
A frequent cause of counting troubles is not having a robust system for keeping
track of which objects you’ve counted and which you have not. Putting objects in a
line, touching them, and moving them from one place to another as you count are all
common ways of ensuring a one-to-one correspondence between the counting words
you say and the things you are counting.
Note that a correct count does not mean a lack of one-to-one correspondence errors.
Many a kindergartner has double-counted an object, skipped twenty, and gotten a correct
total of twenty-three objects. The reverse is also true. Children sometimes count with perfect
one-to-one correspondence but skip some number words, and end up with an incorrect total.
Also, you can count correctly but not be able to say how many there are, which brings
us to the third major early counting skill: recognizing cardinality.
20
H o w C o u nting D e v el o ps
CARDINALITY
Imagine a preschool teacher and a four-year-old child looking at the photograph in
Figure 2.6.
It is entirely reasonable for young children to understand the question How many?
to be a request to count the objects in front of them. Learning that the last number
you say (in this example, five) is different from the others in that it tells you something
about the whole collection of
oranges (that there are five
of them), as well as about the
individual orange you’re pointing
to (it’s the fifth one you counted),
is an impressive achievement.
While the correct answer to the
question “How many oranges?”
is five, a common thing children
have seen and heard occurring
after the question “How many?” is
counting. Many children therefore
first intuit the question “How
many?” to be a social cue to act,
not a request for information.
FIGURE 2.6
How many oranges?
21
HOW DID YOU COUNT?
The fiveness of the set of oranges—together with the fact that this number does
not change when you rearrange them or count them in a different order—is the set’s
cardinality. Along with knowing the counting sequence and maintaining a one-to-one
correspondence as you count, recognizing a set’s cardinality is the third major skill to
make up early counting.
To be clear, it is possible to have any two of these skills and not the third.
• The counting sequence and cardinality. A child may confidently proclaim that there
are six oranges in Figure 2.6 while performing a smooth sweep and correctly reciting
the numbers one through six.
• The counting sequence and one-to-one correspondence. A child may correctly point
to each orange in Figure 2.6 in an organized way, correctly reciting one number each
time, yet not understand that there are five oranges. Each number (one, two, etc.) may
simply act as a name for each orange rather than making a contribution to the child’s
understanding of the set of oranges as a whole.
These skills are challenging and sophisticated. As with all mathematical ideas, they
form the basis of even more sophisticated ideas. For example, once you get good at
counting one-by-one, you become interested in ways to accomplish your goal more
quickly, and more reliably. Making groups—especially of two, five, or ten—is a great way
to achieve both of these ends.
22
H o w C o u nting D e v el o ps
a number from 0 to 23. We place 0 at an edge of the space and so the path serves to
draw children—and their families behind them—in to play at Math On-A-Stick.
There is a natural progression in the ways young children play on these stepping
stones. They begin at 0 and go one step per stone, counting aloud each number they
land on. Most commonly, on reaching 23, children turn around and go back the way
they came, counting back to 0. This sometimes goes a few rounds, and children are
sometimes pulled away by hungry parents eager to find the Food Building. Often
children persist, and when they do, their play evolves. They start to jump over every
other stone and count by twos. If they start on the 0 stone, the numbers they say are
two, four, six, and so on, ending at twenty-two. Perhaps children are eager to count by
twos, or perhaps they are eager to leap over stones. Either way, the sequence two, four,
six, eight… is a familiar one even to young children, and is an example of skip counting.
Skip counting is a common activity in early elementary classrooms, and a useful one
for building both a sense of number relationships and of connections between additive
and multiplicative thinking (more about those connections coming later in this chapter).
Counting by twos on the stepping stones is compelling because jumping is fun, and
also because counting by twos is the simplest form of skip counting (simple in the sense
of being least complex, which is not to say that it’s easy) as it skips the least possible
number of numbers. Further, our bodies are built with lots of convenient twos.
My friend Chris’s son, Chase, went through a period of being very interested in pairs
of things when he was about fifteen months old. Why pairs? One for each hand. Chase
seemed to attend to the similarity of features such as shape, texture, and function while
ignoring color. A typical morning would see Chase marching around the living room
with a stuffed bunny in one hand, and a stuffed alien in the other. These would soon be
replaced by two differently colored discarded Christmas card envelopes, and then two
metal canning jar rings. On rare occasions, Chase would come across the second stuffed
alien, which his parents had on hand as a backup in case of a toddler emergency, and
these mornings were the lad’s absolute ideal.
It is easy to imagine that Chase was building a strong sense of one-to-one
correspondence (here between hands and stuffed creatures), and also that counting by
twos will be a natural thing for him to think about in the near future.
23
HOW DID YOU COUNT?
FIGURE 2.7
Counting pennies by twos
If you read that sequence aloud, you’ll probably fall into a familiar rhythm as you
surpass twenty-five; something DA-da-da DA-da (and contrast this with counting by
sixes, where I can barely make it to thirty-six without having to slow down to work out
what comes next, and where I certainly don’t fall into a rhythm).
Finally, counting by tens takes full advantage of the structure of our number system
by running through each of the new words (ten, twenty, thirty, and so on) required
for the decades, but skipping over all the units in between. If you have a large number
of objects to count, making piles of ten is a great strategy for efficiency. Checking
that a pile of ten actually has ten objects (and not nine or eleven) is a bit tedious, so
24
H o w C o u nting D e v el o ps
sometimes you might choose to make piles of five and pair them—verifying that a pile
of five is actually five is at the edge of most people’s subitizing powers, and verifying
that each pair of piles is actually a pair (not a singleton or a triple) is certainly possible
at a glance. In short, groups of two, five, or ten are common and convenient extensions
of the ideas of number language, one-to-one correspondence, and cardinality, and for
many children, they are the among the first ideas they encounter about the various
ways that numbers are structured.
NUMBER STRUCTURE
Another favorite plaything at Math On-A-Stick is a Pattern Machine—a mechanical toy
consisting of nine rows of nine buttons; each of which works like the clicky end of a
ballpoint pen. Press once to pop it up; press again to pop it down (Figure 2.8). Pattern
Machines are adapted from a
commercial product designed for
memorizing multiplication facts,
but we modify them by covering
the facts to create a well-
structured plaything for children.
As an exercise, consider trying
to determine whether there are
more buttons popped up or
down in Figure 2.8. Remarkably,
children are often able to
determine this without knowing
how many buttons are up or
down—that is, without counting.
I’ll share a typical argument at
the end of this section.
FIGURE 2.8
A Pattern Machine
25
HOW DID YOU COUNT?
FIGURE 2.9
Scattered coins
26
H o w C o u nting D e v el o ps
FIGURE 2.10
Alternating rows on a Pattern
Machine
27
HOW DID YOU COUNT?
Even if a child doesn’t know (or care) that there are eighty-one buttons on a Pattern
Machine, they know that there are many buttons. They know that there is some number
that describes how many there are. Even if they don’t know (or care) how many rows
there are, or how many buttons are in each row, they have learned that the buttons are in
groups. The difference between a collection of individual things and a collection of groups
is an important part of the difference between additive and multiplicative thinking.
The idea that a group can be a single thing is an element of structure that runs
through place value (where we are concerned with groups of ten, groups of ten tens,
and so on), fractions (where we can think of half or three-quarters of a group), and they
are essential to the meaning of multiplication.
Coming back to the exercise I suggested for Figure 2.8, children often notice that
the top row has five up-buttons and four down-buttons, while the second row has four
up-buttons and five down-buttons. That is, the top row has an extra up while the second
row has an extra down. Together these two rows have the same number of ups and
downs. You can make four pairs of rows like that, and then the bottom row has an extra
up-button without a corresponding next row to have an extra down. Therefore, there is
one extra up-button on the whole machine.
28
H o w C o u nting D e v el o ps
this algorithm. There are algorithms with colorful names, such as the Russian Peasant
Algorithm, which is a multiplication algorithm. There is even an algorithm that used to
be standard in the United States, but has not been regularly included in curricula for a
long time—an algorithm for extracting square roots.
All of this is to say that the term “standard algorithm” is much more nebulous than
it might seem if the American standard algorithms are the only ones you’ve ever had a
chance to learn. In what follows, I’ll use the terms “standard algorithm” and “standard
American algorithm” interchangeably, on the assumption that these are algorithms
familiar to most readers. If anything about these algorithms is unfamiliar or long-
forgotten, you’ll find lots of resources with a quick internet search. The same is true if
all this talk of alternative algorithms has you interested in expanding your repertoire of
computation methods.
FIGURE 2.11
Eggs
29
HOW DID YOU COUNT?
adding five eggs for each column as you move from left to right. Each number you recite
is a number of eggs, and so you are using the additive structure of numbers. If instead
you count the columns “1, 2, 3, 4, 5, 6,” and think “six groups of five, which is thirty,” then
you are using the multiplicative structure of numbers. In this second count, the numbers
name groups of eggs, and there is no running tally of the number of eggs. (Sometimes,
you will hear children doing a kind of intermediate count, saying “one” for the row, then
“five” for the number of eggs so far. “two… 10, three… 15, four… 20, five… 25, six… 30.”)
At its heart, multiplication is about some number of same-sized groups. While
mathematicians have several of ways of talking about multiplication, this one is simple
enough for young children to make sense of, and robust enough to extend through
most of high school algebra and calculus.
In this sense, A x B means A groups of B. 3 x 5 is three groups of five; 6 x 9 is six groups
of nine. Alternatively, you can use the same idea a bit differently and say that A x B means
groups of A, B times. This would mean that 3 x 5 is five groups of three. The former
structure is more common in American school curriculum, so I’ll use it as the initial meaning
for multiplication. What’s important is that the two meanings share the structure of groups.
A groups of B captures one way in which multiplicative thinking is more sophisticated
than additive thinking. In the expression 3 x 6, three and six have different meanings.
There are three groups and six objects in each group. Let’s say we’re counting the eggs
in Figure 2.12.
Three rows of six eggs is
eighteen whats? Colloquially,
you cannot add apples to
oranges, but evidently you can
multiply rows by eggs. The
result is eggs; what happened to
the rows?
FIGURE 2.12
Eighteen eggs
30
H o w C o u nting D e v el o ps
A conversation a few years back helped me to notice something new. When you
count the rows and the eggs in each row, you might count across the bottom row
from left to right, and then up the left-hand side. To a novice (say, a kindergartner),
it may appear as though you are counting the lower-left egg twice. As an exercise
before reading on, consider answering that novice’s question, “Why did you count that
lower-left egg twice?”
To the extent that pointing and saying a number is counting, you sort of are counting
that egg twice, but really, you’re counting different things each time. On the first pass,
that egg is an egg, and it’s being counted as one of several in its row. On the second
pass, that egg stands in for a row, one of several in the carton. Now imagine a towering
stack of egg cartons. You might count that same egg a third time, as the representative
of its carton, one of several in
the stack (Figure 2.13).
What is going on here is
abstraction—an egg isn’t always
an egg. Sometimes an egg
is an egg—this is concrete.
Sometimes an egg stands for
something else: a row of eggs,
or a carton—this is abstract.
Making that act of abstraction
explicit by saying something
such as “Now we’ll count the
rows,” or by gesturing along
the rows as you count them is a
great way to support children in
learning to commit such acts of
abstraction for themselves.
FIGURE 2.13
A palette of eggs
31
HOW DID YOU COUNT?
As for what happens to the rows when you multiply three rows by six eggs, it’s not
really rows times eggs. You’re multiplying three rows and six eggs per row. “Eggs
per row” is a unit rate, like miles per hour or liters per flush. Unit rates serve as a
kind of intermediary between different units in multiplication situations. In effect, A
groups of B objects per group is C objects is a more detailed view of our meaning
of multiplication. Note that there are three units: groups, objects, and objects per
group.
Children bring lots of expertise about unit rates to the classroom. This expertise is
likely to be diverse in most classrooms because it is embedded in culture. Eggs per
row, points per basket, wheels per vehicle, stitches per inch, etc. Anywhere children
encounter unit rates, the multiplication structure of numbers is lurking. As educators, we
can draw these examples out of students—both to share their knowledge and to help
them look for more examples of this mathematics in their everyday lives. As parents
or other outside-of-school caregivers, we can talk and wonder about how/whether/
when unit rates scale. (I can walk a mile in 20 minutes; can I really walk 100 miles in
2,000 minutes?) or whether there are exceptions (do all egg cartons have six eggs per
row? Is a basket always worth two points?).
The idea of A groups of B extends to fractions with a little bit of work. 21 × 7 means
half of a group of seven, which is 3 21 . 4 × 43 means four groups of 43 . It feels a little strange
to talk about “groups” that are smaller than one. A useful generalization of “group” is
“unit”—both group and unit can be the thing you’re counting by. Four units, each is 43 , is
another way of interpreting four groups of 43 .
In this view, a unit is anything you consider to be one. I can get 1 21 cups of flour by
using my 43 cup measure twice (Figure 2.14). 2 × 43 = 1 21 means “two 43 -cup measures is
1 21 cups” or “two groups of 43 of a cup is 1 21 cups.” In this case, the 43 -cup measure is
serving as a unit. “I’ll use two of these,” you might say to your baking companion,
causing no confusion in referring the 43 -cup measure as one.
Besides the A groups of B meaning of multiplication, a different idea is that
multiplication is repeated addition. In this interpretation, 3 × 5 means 5 + 5 + 5, and
6 × 9 means 9 + 9 + 9 + 9 + 9 + 9. This isn’t a wrong idea, and indeed repeated addition
is often helpful in solving multiplication problems or noticing patterns. But defining
32
H o w C o u nting D e v el o ps
FIGURE 2.14
Measuring cup
multiplication in terms of
addition focuses on the
procedure for calculating
a product rather than on
the underlying structure.
Multiplication isn’t a shorthand
way of writing particular
kinds of addition problems; it
is instead a powerful way of
structuring numbers—a way
different from addition.
When children count the eggs
in Figure 2.11 by fives, we don’t
know for sure whether they are
thinking about multiplication per
se. We don’t know that they see
a special structure of six groups
of five. As teachers, we can support students making that connection in a number of
ways, such as using language like six groups of five, pairing that language with notation
such as 6 × 5, and by asking whether they remember from one day to the next how
many objects there are altogether in six groups of five.
As a quick illustration of the richness of elementary mathematics, I’ll tell you about
another meaning for multiplication. If you have three pairs of pants and two shirts as
in Figure 2.15, you can mix and match to end up with several outfits (perhaps not all
of them in good taste or matching, of course). As an exercise, consider counting how
many outfits you can make this way.
One way to count these outfits is to make a list of them. If you label the pants P, Q,
and R, and the shirts S and T, then you can write (P, S) to represent an outfit with the
33
HOW DID YOU COUNT?
FIGURE 2.15
Outfits
(P,S) (P,T)
(Q,S) (Q,T)
(R,S) (R,T)
Organizing the outfits in a 3-by-2 array like this highlights the connection to A
groups of B as a meaning for multiplication. In this case, the original structure of
the units works differently. Three is the number of pairs of pants, and two is the
number of shirts. Neither is a number of groups. From a practical perspective, you
cannot make all six outfits at once. You have to imagine them. You can dress yourself
six separate times, but you cannot dress six people at once with this collection of
clothing items.
Mathematicians call this view of multiplication the Cartesian product of two sets. The
Fundamental Counting Principle summarizes that the size of the Cartesian product is the
product of the sizes of the two sets. Three pairs of pants and two shirts make six outfits,
and more generally, A pairs of pants and B shirts make A × B outfits. This is a more
abstract notion of multiplication than counting eggs in a carton. When you’re counting
eggs, the eggs are there in front of you. When you’re counting outfits, you cannot
actually make all six outfits at once; you have to imagine outfits that do not currently
exist in your world.
Recall that counting means any work you do to figure out how many there are
(or could be!). We began with the underlying skills of knowing number language,
34
H o w C o u nting D e v el o ps
A Note on Notation
There are several ways of indicating multiplication. The standard in elementary school
mathematics is an x. 3 x 4 means “three times four” or “three multiplied by four” or “the
product of three and four."
In algebra, mathematicians tend to use the letter x as a variable, which would make
3 x x a confusing thing to look at. Whether for this reason or others, when possible,
mathematicians tend to omit the multiplication symbol altogether. 3x means “three times x”
or “three multiplied by x” or “the product of three and x." We usually read it as “three-ex.”
Finally, omitting multiplication symbols means we can’t tell the difference between
“three multiplied by four” and “thirty-four”—both would be 34. So, when you want to
show the multiplication of two numbers, you need a symbol. Ideally that symbol isn’t the
letter x (because that’s a variable now). That is what a raised dot is for: 3 • 4.
In the remainder of this Teacher’s Guide, these three ways of showing multiplication
will be interchangeable. Generally, I’ll use x more often for purely arithmetic ideas,
• more often when I want to emphasize the operation of multiplication, and omit
multiplication symbols altogether when the ideas are purely algebraic.
35
HOW DID YOU COUNT?
The idea of commutativity is important for young children, but the name is
not. Some classrooms refer to the “rearrangement principle,” others use invented
terminology such as “turnaround facts,” and some use “commutative." However
you refer to it, helping students notice that you can rearrange the terms of a sum,
or the factors of a product, ensures that they have a powerful problem-solving
strategy.
Here are a few sums and products that may become easier for many of us to do in
our heads if we remember that addition and multiplication are commutative. As an
exercise, consider changing the order of the numbers in each row and thinking about
how that might change the way somebody might perform the calculations. I’ll suggest
possibilities at the end of this section.
4 + 56
68 + 117 + (−68)
5 × 14
1
4 × 17 ×
4
36
H o w C o u nting D e v el o ps
If you think of 3 + 4 as combining a set of three and a set of four, then a diagram for
3 + 4 might look like this:
XXX XXXX
XXXX XXX
This second diagram is 4 + 3. The order that the two sets come in doesn’t matter. There
is nothing special about 4 and 3, or even about these being whole numbers. Instead, it’s
the fact that I’m combining the sets that matters. There are reasonable questions about
whether this argument extends to negative numbers, or to irrational numbers (such as
the square root of two, or pi), where representing a number with Xs is non-intuitive (or
maybe even impossible). But those are questions we can save for later. A diagram such
as the one above provides a basis for thinking about the structure of the operation of
addition, which is a big step forward for lots of young mathematicians.
By contrast, a diagram for 7 − 4 might look like this:
OOOXXXX
One interpretation is that there were seven circles, but we crossed out four of them.
With that interpretation, 4 − 7 would mean there were four circles, but we crossed out
seven of them. This is a very different situation from the previous one—how do you
cross out circles that don’t exist? If you spin the 7 − 4 diagram 180°, you end up this:
XXXXOOO
This new diagram is still seven circles with four crossed out. It still represents 7 − 4,
not 4 – 7. Subtraction behaves differently from addition in this way. I’ll say more about
subtraction and the commutative property a bit later on, when discussing division,
which is similarly non-commutative. But first, let’s consider multiplication.
37
HOW DID YOU COUNT?
FIGURE 2.16
An array. Four rows of three.
38
H o w C o u nting D e v el o ps
FIGURE 2.17
The same array, rotated to become
three rows of four.
FIGURE 2.18
Area models for twelve showing three rows of four (left) and one-and-a-half rows of eight (right).
39
HOW DID YOU COUNT?
FIGURE 2.19
Area shows up in many places! Fractions (left), the Pythagorean Theorem (center), and calculus
(right).
We saw a little ways back that not all operations are commutative; 3 – 2 does not equal
2 – 3. Similarly, division is not commutative. 12 ÷ 4 = 3, but 4 ÷ 12 = 31 . To summarize, addition
and multiplication are each commutative operations, which is a formal way of saying what
many children notice on their own—that it doesn’t matter in which order you put two
numbers you’re going to add (or multiply); the sum (or product) will be the same either way.
In order to notice the importance of a property such as the commutative property,
it is often helpful to see that not everything has that property. Subtraction and division
have in common that they are not commutative, just as addition and multiplication have
in common that they are.
It is inconvenient that subtraction and division are not commutative operations. There
is a workaround, however. If you think of 7 − 4 not as a subtraction problem, but an
addition problem involving a negative number, you’ll have 7 + −4. You can rewrite that
as −4 + 7 because addition is commutative. The same thing is true for division, but you
need to rewrite as the reciprocal rather than the opposite. 12 ÷ 4 becomes 12 • ( 41 ). You
can rewrite that as ( 41 ) • 12 because multiplication is commutative.
As teachers, we don’t have to rely on children noticing the commutative property on
their own, though. We can help them to notice it, to investigate it, and to use it in their
own mathematical work. If children see five groups of six in Figure 2.11, you can ask if
40
H o w C o u nting D e v el o ps
anyone sees six groups of five, and you can even wonder aloud whether you will always
be able to find B groups of A whenever you see A groups of B. While the commutative
property of multiplication is about the order of two factors, another property is about
the order of two operations. If I’m multiplying twice, say 3 • 4 • 5, does it matter whether
I do the first multiplication first or the second one? The answer to that question is
summarized in the associative property, which is the next topic.
But first, I promised earlier in this section to share some ways of thinking that might
emerge by rearranging numbers in some sums and products. Here goes…
4 + 56 can become 56 + 4, and now I go a short distance from fifty-six rather than a
long distance from four (or add a few things to a large collection, rather than adding a
whole bunch of things to a small collection).
68 + 117 + (−68) can become 68 + (−68) + 117. If I know that a number and its opposite
add to zero, then I only have to add zero to 117.
5 × 14 can become 14 × 5. If I’m thinking about five groups of fourteen, I might try to
count by fourteens. Thinking about fourteen groups of five allows me to count by fives,
or even (to preview the next section on the associative property) to notice that fourteen
groups of five is the same as seven groups of ten.
Finally, 4 × 17 × ( 41 ) can become 4 × ( 41 ) × 17, which is just like the trick for 68 up above,
only now we’re multiplying by one instead of adding zero.
(A + B) + C = A + (B + C)
In the case of my 9 + 8 example, that looks like this:
(1 + 8) + 8 = 1 + (8 + 8)
41
HOW DID YOU COUNT?
Adding 10 to 25, and then adding 5, gives the same result as adding 15 straightaway.
As a final example, consider 37 + 45. As an exercise, perform this sum with pencil and
paper, keeping track of the steps you take along the way, even if they don’t result in
marks on the paper.
In the standard (American) addition algorithm, you would add the ones digits of the
two numbers first. Here, that means doing 7 + 5 and getting 12. This works because of
the associative property:
(30 + 7) + 5 + 40 = 30 + (7 + 5) + 40
30 + 40 + (7 + 5)
In the standard algorithm, you would add 7 + 5 to get 12, write the 2 and “carry the one.”
That is a very cleverly disguised instance of the associative property of addition.
Finally, you may just know that 30 + 40 is 70, or you may think about 3 tens + 4 tens =
7 tens, which is an example of the distributive property (which I’ll discuss shortly),
and then one more ten is 8 tens, or 80. But if you’re following the steps of the
42
H o w C o u nting D e v el o ps
standard algorithm more tightly, you’re rewriting that last expression above using the
commutative property of addition.
= 40 + 40 + 2
= 80 + 2
= 82
FIGURE 2.20
Adding 37 and 45 with the standard
American algorithm.
43
HOW DID YOU COUNT?
(A × B) × C = A × (B × C)
Recall the shoe rack from Chapter 1 (Figure 1.3). In that example, you might count the
24 shoes as 4 groups of (3 pairs), or (4 groups of 3) pairs, so
4 × (3 × 2) = (4 × 3) × 2
In this example the unit “pairs” disguises the third factor in the multiplication. If I want to
count individual shoes, I can count the pairs and then double, or I can count the shoes
directly. X pairs of shoes will always be the same number as 2X shoes.
In that sense, the associative property of multiplication is a formal way of saying that you
can count things individually or bundled into groups; you’ll get the same total either way.
4 groups of (3 pairs) suggests 4 groups of 6 shoes—the shoes are unbundled while I
count them. (4 groups of 3) pairs suggests 12 pairs—the shoes are bundled until I finish
up the count; it’s 24 shoes either way.
More importantly, the associative property means that this bundling and unbundling
idea is true of any size groups, not just for pairs of shoes.
One of the top ten most important groups in our number system is a group of ten.
Why does twenty have a two in it? Because twenty consists of two groups of ten, and
this leads us to an answer to the question, “When I’m multiplying by a multiple of ten—
such as 3 × 40—why can I ignore the zero, and then stick it back on at the end?”
Three groups of forty is three groups of (4 tens). Because we’ve studied pairs of
shoes in great depth, we know we can write this as (3 groups of 4) tens, which is 12 tens,
and 12 tens is 120.
Let’s linger a moment to ensure that there’s no magic here—just various ways of
thinking about the numbers.
This shift in thinking is when you “ignore the zero.” Don’t think of it as 40 ones for right
now, think of it as 4 tens—same number, but differently grouped.
44
H o w C o u nting D e v el o ps
I expressed my surprise at the length of this list of doublings, but he wasn’t quite done.
Again, I expressed my surprise, and he replied, “Yeah. That’s all I can do, though. I can’t
think of what comes next.” We had confirmed that the idea of doubling was one that
Griffin had well in hand before knowing anything about exponents. The point of this
story here is not anything to do with exponents, but instead that being able to double
numbers is a common skill that children develop as they explore arithmetic.
At first glance, this may seem like a skill with limited utility. After all, a relatively small
percentage of multiplication problems we meet in our lifetimes have two as one of their
two factors. But if you also develop an intuition for the kinds of unit switching that the
associative property captures, doubling numbers becomes powerful for lots of mental
arithmetic. Consider a product such as 35 × 16. As an exercise, you might compute this
product with paper and pencil before reading further. If you do, pay attention to the
steps you take along the way, even if those steps don’t result in marks on the paper.
The standard algorithm for multiplication (Figure 2.21) breaks both numbers up by
place value; regardless of what the tens digit is for either number, you would multiply
5 × 6 as the first step.
45
HOW DID YOU COUNT?
FIGURE 2.21
Multiplying 35 and 16 using the
standard algorithm.
If instead of seeing 16 as 10 +
6, you see 16 as two eights, you
can write:
35 × (2 × 8) = (35 × 2) × 8 = 70 × 8
70 × (2 × 4) = (70 × 2) × 4
140 × (2 × 2) = (140 × 2) × 2
= 280 × 2
= 560
Many children invent such strategies, and they have no idea that these strategies
are examples of the associative property of multiplication. As teachers supporting our
students’ learning, we can be more prepared by knowing that these strategies always
work (because the associative property of multiplication is true for all numbers), and
by supporting students in finding connections among groupings, working with tens,
doubling, and beyond.
46
H o w C o u nting D e v el o ps
Before we turn our attention away from the associative property of multiplication,
here are a few more products with useful ways of re-associating the underlying factors.
14 × 35 = (7 × 2) × (5 × 7)
= 7 × (2 × 5) × 7
29 × 40 = 29 × (2 × 2 × 10)
= (29 × 2) × (2 × 10)
= (58) × (2 × 10)
= (58 × 2) × 10
= (379 × 2) tenths
47
HOW DID YOU COUNT?
and she knew there were fifteen, probably by counting by fives. I asked, “What if it had
been three rows of six dots instead?” She thought about each row having one additional
dot, so there are three more dots than before—eighteen altogether.
That relationship is what the distributive property formalizes. 3 x 6 is the same as 3 x
5 + 3 x 1. In algebraic symbols, that looks like this:
a • (b + c) = a • b + a • c(1)
(b + c) • a = b • a + c • a(2)
If we’re thinking about A groups of B, then equation (1) is how Tabitha was thinking:
three groups of six is the same as three groups of five, plus three groups of one. If
instead she and I had started with two rows of six, then she would likely have thought
in the style of equation (2): three groups of six is the same as two groups of six plus one
group of six (Figure 2.22).
The distributive property makes multiplication algorithms work. Consider 43 x 12. As
an exercise, compute this product with paper and pencil before reading on. Figure 2.23
shows what my paper looks like after finding this product with the standard algorithm.
FIGURE 2.22
Three groups of six is the same as two groups of six, plus one more.
48
H o w C o u nting D e v el o ps
FIGURE 2.23
Multiplying 43 and 12 with the standard
algorithm.
43 • (10 + 2) = 43 • 10 + 43 • 2
43 • (10 + 2) = 43 • 10 + 43 • 2
Now each of the expressions on the right-hand side is a product that we can rewrite.
43 • 10 + 43 • 2 = (40 + 3) • 10 + (40 + 3) • 2
= 400 + 30 + 80 + 6
49
HOW DID YOU COUNT?
FIGURE 2.24
Multiplying 43 and 12 with the
partial products algorithm.
6 • 4 + 6 • 6 = 6 • (4 + 6)
50
H o w C o u nting D e v el o ps
FIGURE 2.25
The distributive property of grapes.
(4 + −4) • (−5)
−20 + (−4)(−5) = 0
51
HOW DID YOU COUNT?
And so the product of −4 and −5 must be positive 20. The two negative numbers
(−4 and −5) aren’t special in any way, so the same argument applies to all negative
numbers.
The commutative, associative, and distributive properties are ideas that children
first encounter in the context of specific computations or relationships, such as that
five rows of six eggs is the same as six columns of five eggs. As children move toward
claiming that changing the order of two factors always gives the same product, and as
they begin to justify why this must be so, they are moving from arithmetic to algebra.
ALGEBRA
There are several ways to think about what algebra is. You can think of algebra as a
set of procedures for figuring out unknown values. Another way is to think of algebra
as generalized arithmetic. 1 + 2 = 2 + 1 is a truth about the relationship between the
numbers 1 and 2, whereas a + b = b + a is a statement about the relationship between all
pairs of numbers. This idea of making statements about all cases is what mathematicians
mean by generality or generalizing.
In this view, algebra is the result of studying the relationships among numbers
without worrying about any specific numbers in particular. For example, is an odd
number times an odd number always odd? Arithmetic can offer lots of examples, such
as those below.
3 × 5 = 15
101 × 7 = 707
If I check enough examples, I might be pretty sure that two odd factors will always
give an odd product. Algebra allows us to consider all odd numbers at once. Let’s
see what that looks like, and how the algebra depends on the number and operation
properties of arithmetic we’ve discussed in this chapter.
Start with even numbers. An even number is a product of two and some other whole
number. Six is either two groups of three (2 · 3) or three groups of two (3 · 2). One
hundred is 2 · 50, which because of the commutative property of multiplication you
52
H o w C o u nting D e v el o ps
can think of as either two groups of fifty or fifty groups of two. If you want to think
about even numbers in general, you can write 2m, where m stands for a whole number.
Think of m as some number of pairs of shoes; then 2m is the number of shoes. When
mathematicians write something like 2m, we’re thinking about 2m as an even number,
and also about 2m as all even numbers. This is a lot like the abstraction that children
have to make in order to think about ten as simultaneously one group and ten individual
things.
Whole numbers that are not even are odd, and those are the numbers that come in
between the even numbers. If you have an even number, you can get an odd number
by adding one. Therefore, we can refer to all odd numbers with the expression 2m + 1,
where m is still a whole number. In the language of shoes, you have m pairs of shoes,
plus an extra shoe. Seven is 2 · 3 + 1, 1041 is 2 · 520 + 1. If you want to think about two
odd numbers without worrying about what those odd numbers are specifically, you can
write the second one as 2n + 1. Maybe we have an odd number of rows of shoes. You
can count (2n + 1) rows of (2m + 1) shoes in each row with the product
(2n + 1) • (2m + 1)
FIGURE 2.26
Area model
53
HOW DID YOU COUNT?
FIGURE 2.27
Area model, relabeled
Because n and m are whole numbers, and because they are being added and
multiplied, that complicated thing in parentheses— ((m · 2n) + (m · 1) + (n · 1))—is a
whole number. I’ll call it M. Now the product looks like this:
Looking at a specific example often helps make sense of the generalities. Let’s look at
7 · 9.
7 is 2 · 3 + 1, while 9 is 2 · 4 + 1. As an area model, that looks like the left-hand side of
Figure 2.28.
FIGURE 2.28
Area models for 7 • 9
54
H o w C o u nting D e v el o ps
(2 • 3 • 2 • 4) + (2 • 4) + (2 • 3) + (1 • 1)
which is 2 · (31) + 1, and that is 63, which is both odd and the correct product.
That turns out to be an extremely complicated way to multiply any two particular
odd numbers. There are much more efficient ways to calculate 7 · 9. The power
instead is in the argument about all products of two odd numbers, which the algebraic
representation captures.
Algebra lets us use the properties of numbers and operations to make and check
claims about all numbers. Algebra moves us from “7 · 9 is odd” to “any two odd
numbers have an odd product.”
Transitioning from arithmetic (these odd numbers) to algebra (all odd numbers)
requires an understanding of structures, not just a familiarity with certain facts.
How Did You Count? is a book about structures. You can count everything in
the book one-by-one. But you can also count by twos or fives, or by pairs, rows,
columns, triangles, or squares. The fun is less in knowing how many there are, and
much more in making and sharing new ways to know how many there are. How
Did You Count? supports a virtuous cycle where the more ways you know how
to count, the more new ways you can think of. All of this is in service of a rich
understanding of number and operation relationships in arithmetic, which is not
only a worthy goal on its own, but it also builds intuitions that support later math
learning beyond arithmetic.
In the next chapter, I’ll share some fun ways to read this book at home, at the library,
at school, or anywhere else. But first, one last set of structures—a connection between
geometry and arithmetic.
55
HOW DID YOU COUNT?
FIGURATE NUMBERS
Odd numbers as “even numbers, plus one” is an example of a number structure, and
algebra captures that by writing 2n + 1. Geometry provides another source of number
structure. How Did You Count? uses several number structures related to geometry.
We have already seen rectangular arrays; now let’s consider squares, triangles, and
pyramids.
The blue and red plastic balls on the back cover of the book are arranged in a square
(Figure 2.29). Six rows of six plastic balls, so this is a special case of a rectangular array.
Because the number of rows and the number of columns is the same, you can write 6 · 6
or 62. The expression 62 is “six to the second power” or “six squared." This connection to
geometric squares is the reason mathematicians read an exponent of 2 as “squared."
But there is more structure here. In the upper-right corner, there is one blue plastic
ball. Three red plastic balls surround it, then five blue plastic balls surround those, and
so on. The 36 plastic balls are
in groups, represented in this
expression: 1 + 3 + 5 + 7 + 9 + 11.
Thirty-six is the sum of the first
six odd numbers! Is thirty-six a
special square number in this
way? (I invite you to consider
that question for a moment
before continuing.)
In order to make the next
square number (72 or 49), you
could add six balls in a new
column on the left-hand side
of the existing square, and six
more balls along the bottom,
FIGURE 2.29
Thirty-six is six squared.
56
H o w C o u nting D e v el o ps
plus one more in the corner. In this way, you can build any (whole) square number
from the previous one. A square with side length n has n2 objects in the array. Add a
new column of n on the left, a new row of n on the bottom, and one more in the corner.
Geometric squares can convince us that:
(n + 1)2 = n2 + 2n + 1
The bowling pins in the student book are arranged in a triangle, as are the pudding
cups, and each of two groups of eggs in the egg photograph (Figure 2.30). These
triangles are special in that each row is greater than the previous by one, so triangular
numbers are sums of consecutive numbers: 1 + 2 + 3 + 4 = 10 bowling pins. Sometimes—
like the bowling pins—triangular
numbers are arranged as an
equilateral triangle. Sometimes—
like the eggs—triangular
numbers are arranged as a
right triangle. Either way, the
numbers 1, 3, 6, 10, 15, 21, etc. are
all triangular numbers.
The thirty-six plastic balls on
the front and back covers of
How Did You Count? also make
a triangle (as seen on the inside
back cover of the student book,
and in Figure 2.31). Indeed, this
is the reason I chose thirty-six
for the number of plastic balls in
these images. Thirty-six is both
FIGURE 2.30
Two triangular numbers of eggs
57
HOW DID YOU COUNT?
FIGURE 2.31
A triangle of spheres
a square number and a triangular number. This is a property that is uncommon, but not
unique to thirty-six. The next number that is both square and triangular is 1,225, which is
352, and also the forty-ninth triangular number.
Triangular numbers appear several times in the book in order to give children a
chance to see connections, and to feel the excitement of noticing that something they’ve
done before is useful in a new challenge. For example, you can save yourself some work
counting the pudding cups in Figure 2.32 if you know that the first four rows total ten
because they’re just like the bowling pins. Now 1 + 2 + 3 + 4 + 5 + 6 becomes 10 + 5 + 6
directly—only three things to add instead of six.
58
H o w C o u nting D e v el o ps
FIGURE 2.32
A triangle of pudding cups
The first dice picture is a sneaky version of triangular numbers, and offers an
opportunity for abstraction. There are lots of ways to count the dots on the dice in
Figure 2.33, but one of those ways is adding 1 + 2 + 3 + 4 + 5, which is five more dots
than there were bowling pins, and also is the same as the number of eggs in the
right-hand, upside-down triangle in Figure 2.30.
The algebra of triangular numbers is just a bit more complicated than the algebra of
square numbers. Imagine two of the same triangular number, each arranged in a right
triangle—oriented in opposite directions, as in Figure 2.34.
You can push them together to make a rectangle, and that rectangle’s dimensions are
different by 1. The fourth triangular number yields a 4 by 5 rectangle; the nth triangular
59
HOW DID YOU COUNT?
FIGURE 2.33
A triangular number in the abstract
number yields an n by (n + 1)
rectangle. Each of the original
triangles is half of that, so the
nth triangular number is:
(n + 1)
n•
2
FIGURE 2.34
Two triangular numbers make a rectangle.
60
H o w C o u nting D e v el o ps
FIGURE 2.35
A hexagon of tangerines
(n + 2)
n • ( n + 1) •
6
61
HOW DID YOU COUNT?
FIGURE 2.36
Tetrahedron of basketballs
62
H o w C o u nting D e v el o ps
FIGURE 2.37
There are ten dots altogether. How
many are under the splat?
63
HOW DID YOU COUNT?
FIGURE 2.38
Stacking spheres in frames
CONNECTING IMAGES
Students often experience math as a series of disconnected facts. Textbooks are
common culprits in fostering this experience, with skills instead of ideas, and these
skills broken down into large numbers of tiny, precise steps. Some textbooks even have
warnings to tell students not to commit particular errors at particular moments, a sort of
discouragement from connecting ideas together.
To many students, math ends up feeling like a long list of rules. Things need not be
this way, and there are many projects aimed at supporting students in understanding
math as connected and deep instead of arbitrary and shallow.
64
H o w C o u nting D e v el o ps
In that spirit, I designed the images in How Did You Count? with connections in
mind. Triangles appear in a number of images, for example. Connecting the triangle of
pudding cups to the triangle of bowling pins saves you work counting the pudding cups.
Connections can save us time.
But not all triangular arrangements are built the same. As an exercise before reading
on, consider Figure 2.39. Do you see triangles in this image? If so, how are they like or
unlike the triangles in Figures 2.30, 2.31, and 2.32?
The paint pots in Figure 2.39 have triangles using only odd numbers in the rows: 1 +
3, and 1 + 3 + 5. Noticing that both the paint pots and the bowling pins have triangular
arrangements opens space for new questions (e.g. Are there different kinds of triangular
numbers?). Context provides connections as well. Is it really the same number of
blue and red plastic balls on
the covers and endpapers of
the book? In what ways is a
tetrahedron like a triangle?
Encouraging the search for
connections supports richer
math learning. Pudding cups,
after all, aren’t really about
pudding cups—they are about
triangular numbers, addition,
and algebra. But only if you’re
looking for connections.
Many mathematicians
conceive of “counting” broadly.
Counting incorporates one-to-
one correspondence between
FIGURE 2.39
Pots of purple paint make a new
sort of triangle
65
HOW DID YOU COUNT?
spoken number words and physical objects (or objects in pictures), as well as multiplying
to determine the number of ways something could happen, even if none of those ways
ever does happen. Part of learning mathematics is learning new ways to count, and new
ways to represent that counting.
How Did You Count? offers lots of opportunities for each of these. This chapter has
focused on various number structures, which in turn lead to new ways of counting. The
next chapter offers ideas and resources for using How Did You Count? in classrooms
and beyond.
66
CHAPTER 3
67
HOW DID YOU COUNT?
Many of the teaching strategies developed or discussed by these other creators are
appropriate for using with How Did You Count? And also, How Did You Count? is written
first as a book that a child can read and enjoy on their own, or with a caregiver, sibling,
or friend.
68
USING HOW DID YOU COUNT?
FIGURE 3.1
How many tangerines? How did you
count them?
Equal Footing
In a How Did You Count? conversation, all participants have equal access to the
mathematics. It is not a quiz where the answers are predetermined and held secret
by the teacher. It is not a game of I Spy in which a particular response is the target,
69
HOW DID YOU COUNT?
and—once uttered—ends the game. In a genuine How Did You Count? conversation,
the mathematics exists in the interactions among the participants, provoked by the
image. As a teacher (and as a reader of this book!) you have expertise that informs
your judgment about what mathematics your students are likely to see. You should use
that expertise to make sure important mathematics happens in the group. At the same
time, you need to make sure that everyone understands that the math comes from the
participants, not from an answer key, a standards document, or some other place of
authority.
70
USING HOW DID YOU COUNT?
Focus on Relationships
In the spirit of asking questions whose answers we do not know, a How Did You Count?
conversation is less focused on a particular number, and more focused on relationships
among numbers. Focusing on relationships might include noticing that several counting
strategies involve groups. While the number and size of the groups may be different,
the basic strategy is the same, and that strategy is fundamentally different from
counting one-by-one. Focusing on relationships might mean inviting students to say
whether a counting-by-twos strategy feels to them more like counting one-by-one, or
more like noticing three groups of four.
Follow-up questions and tasks help focus on relationships. In the next section, I’ll
describe how intentional use of classroom media such as whiteboards and computer
projectors can also serve this purpose, and I’ll also address other pedagogical questions
related to this routine.
Recording
Annotation can go a long way in focusing students’ thinking on relationships. Writing
both “3 groups of 4” and “3 × 4” highlights the relationship between the operation and
its meaning. Writing both 3 × 4 and 4 × 3 emphasizes how these two expressions have
the same result—the commutative property of multiplication. Writing “4 groups of 3”
and “4 × 3” together with a diagram such as Figure 3.2 builds a bridge of relationships
from concrete to abstract.
A fascinating result of the TIMSS (Trends in International Mathematics and Science
Study) video study, summarized in The Teaching Gap (Stigler and Hiebert 2009), is that
American math teachers use instructional media such as whiteboards and projectors
differently from how Japanese math teachers use them. American math teachers tend
to use media to focus student attention on one thing at a time, and consequently new
71
HOW DID YOU COUNT?
FIGURE 3.2
Annotated tangerines
72
USING HOW DID YOU COUNT?
FIGURE 3.3
Simon Gregg’s annotated avocados
In the upper left, we see that student E says there is 1 stone (or pit) in the image.
Student Al disagrees, pointing out that the uncut avocado must also have a pit. The
annotations capture a similar debate in the purple and red ideas near the bottom of the
image. Are there three dots on the knife handle (that we can see) or six dots (including
three we cannot see, but that must exist)? Capturing both of these exchanges allows
a teacher to point out the commonality between them; without the annotations, these
two discussions are more likely to happen independently and without connection. This
question of “should we count things that are not in the image?” is embedded much
more explicitly in the basketball image (Figure 3.4) from How Did You Count? “How
many basketballs must there be?” is a different question—with a different answer—from
“How many basketballs do you see?”
73
HOW DID YOU COUNT?
FIGURE 3.4
“How many basketballs can
you see?” versus “How many
basketballs must there be?”
74
USING HOW DID YOU COUNT?
FIGURE 3.5
Two strategies for the same image
Show two or more representations of the same strategy, as in Figure 3.6. Ask
questions that move between the representations, such as these: Can you find each
number (3, 8, and 12) in the
picture? I see two bunches of
red grapes and two bunches of
green grapes in each group; do
those bunches show up in the
numbers? How?
Put two related tasks side-
by-side, as in Figure 3.7. Ask
questions about relationships
FIGURE 3.6
One strategy; two representations.
Diagram (left) and symbols (right).
75
HOW DID YOU COUNT?
FIGURE 3.7
Juxtaposing two related tasks.
between the two tasks, such as these: What do these two pictures have in common? How
are they different from each other? How could you use your thinking or total from one of
these to help count the other one?
Tell Stories
When my children were very young, we read picture books with rich and interesting
characters taking on complicated and challenging situations. We were especially fond
of Russell Hoban’s classic Bread and Jam for Frances, in which Frances—an early-
elementary-aged anthropomorphic badger—declares that the only food she likes is bread
with jam. Her exasperated family indulges this demand, and (spoiler alert!) over the
course of several days, Frances tires of the monotony, relents, and begins to delight in a
richly varied diet. My children and I could relate to Frances and her disgust with a jiggly
soft-boiled egg at the book’s outset. Indeed, I can vividly recall a time when one of my
own children declared an entire slice of watermelon inedible because of a supposedly
76
USING HOW DID YOU COUNT?
mushy bit where a seed had been. (For the record, there was no mushy bit; there could
have been no mushy bit because there is no watermelon in an empty space! But I digress.)
The point is that we understood our lives a bit differently through this book, and we
understood the book a bit differently for having lived our lives together. After reading a
favorite story book, we would pick up a shapes book or a numbers book and have a much
less engrossing experience. Triangles, then squares, then rectangles on the next page
(but never a square on the rectangle page!). One of this, two of that, three of another.
These math-focused books were much less likely to resonate with us after the book was
finished, and offered many fewer opportunities for us to bring our experience to bear on
the books’ content.
How Did You Count? (like its sibling books, Which One Doesn’t Belong? and How
Many?) aims to break this cycle, for math has stories too. I hope that you and your
students will recognize their life experiences here, and recognize the ideas of the book
in their lives. I hope that some students will recognize the city recreation center where
we shot these photographs. I hope that some students will have stories about being the
one responsible for collecting all the soccer balls or basketballs after practice. I hope
they’ll know and share with others how difficult is to stack or organize these in any way.
I hope the pink pudding cups will produce a reaction of delight in some and disgust in
others.
In the classroom, these hopes mean leaving a bit of space for storytelling. Tell your
own stories, invite the children in your classroom to share their experiences with
bowling pins or eggs in bulk. Let your students be the experts for a while, and share
your expertise as well. Laugh together. Empathize. Here is how this sometimes sounds
when I visit classrooms.
“In a moment, we’re going to look at a picture together. I’ll ask you to count the
things in the picture, and we’ll talk about how many there are. More importantly, we’ll
talk about how you counted them. But before I show you the picture, I’ll tell you that it
is a picture of a kind of fruit. Do you have a favorite kind of fruit?”
Children will share their love of apples, or watermelon, or grapes. Some might express
their dislike of bananas or pineapple. Children might mention fruits unfamiliar to their
classmates, and we’ll celebrate this learning opportunity. Somebody might start an
77
HOW DID YOU COUNT?
argument about whether a tomato is a fruit or a vegetable. (If we define a fruit as the
ripened ovary of a flowering plant, then a tomato is definitely a fruit. If we define a fruit
as something sweet I would enjoy on a bowl of ice cream or baked into a pie, then we’ll
likely come to a different conclusion.) We’ll spend maybe ninety seconds on this before
I develop the mathematics of the conversation a little bit.
“Are there any of these fruits we have just discussed that you could eat more than
one of at a time?”
We’ll agree on grapes (yes!) and watermelons (no!). We might disagree on apples or
bananas. Now we’re ready to count tangerines together.
78
USING HOW DID YOU COUNT?
She agreed, so I continued. “But then once I roll it—even if I don’t look at it, and it’s
hidden from my view—there’s no longer a one-in-six chance that it’s a five because now
it either is or it isn’t.”
“Yeah,” she said. “But you can be 61 sure that it’s a five.”
When students study probability, this relationship between how sure I am that
something is the way it is, and the probability that it is that way is a big thing to learn.
Yet, statisticians swap those two things back and forth for each other all the time—
usually without even noticing it because the relationship between the two is so familiar
to people who work with these ideas every day.
As with many topics though, it does no service to learners to dismiss a novice
perspective with the expectation that they’ll adopt the expert one. A square and a
diamond might be congruent to each other, but ignoring that the child cares about
orientation means we might never actually talk about the thing that matters to the child.
I know that two rows of three is the same as three columns of two, but pretending they
are obviously the same means children have less opportunity to wrestle with whether
they are always the same, or in what circumstances they might be different. In each of
these cases, we can invite more learners into mathematics by seeking to understand
their ideas, rather than ignoring distinctions they may see as important. Let’s seek to
understand.
In practice this means listening carefully, asking follow-up questions, inviting students
to the board to point or annotate, and encouraging conversation about the ideas our
students express.
79
HOW DID YOU COUNT?
around in a circle,” is too much detail and nuance for a child’s first encounter with the
word vehicle. (For detailed discussion of definitions of vehicles, what that has to do with
math, and related resources, visit https://bit.ly/vehicle-definition.)
Mathematics is a field that thrives on precision. Even so, the needed level of
precision is contextual. There are countless stories of ideas that mathematicians
thought were precise until a strange example came along that didn’t seem to fit.
Imre Lakatos documented the evolving definition of polyhedron among eighteenth-
century mathematicians in his book, Proofs and Refutations. Bertrand Russell’s
Principia Mathematica used a great deal of space to prove that 1 + 1 = 2 because a
special project required a high level of precision.
In a How Did You Count? conversation, the context for precision is usually
communicating with each other. If a student says about the basketball tetrahedron,
“Well, the bottom layer is ten,” the rest of the class probably understands that the unit
is basketballs. In that case, insisting on stating units is not justified by a call for precision.
(There may be other valid reasons though, such as setting a habit in a classroom where
students often omit—and lose track of—the units they are working with.)
By contrast, when a student tells us how they counted those same basketballs
by saying, “Well, there’s four, four, four, and four, and then one in the middle,” we
genuinely do not know what those “fours” refer to. More precision is necessary. Such a
child might use language, gesture, annotation, or a combination of these to make their
communication more precise.
Develop Expertise
When children first play with tiling turtles at Math On-A-Stick (a large-scale playful,
creative math event that takes place annually at the Minnesota State Fair), they spend
a lot of time spinning each turtle around in order to figure out how to fit it into place.
The initial search for patterns and the attention required to visualize the tiling mean that
a design such as the one on the left in Figure 3.8 is what children usually make in their
first few minutes. As they develop expertise with their turtles, they’ll often start using
the colors of the turtles to make interesting patterns and designs.
80
USING HOW DID YOU COUNT?
FIGURE 3.8
Introductory turtle play (left), and play by a child with more expertise (right).
81
HOW DID YOU COUNT?
FIGURE 3.9
A large triangle of pudding cups (left). Two smaller triangles of 10 pink pudding cups, and one left
over (right).
I sometimes test and develop my own expertise with the ideas of How Did You
Count? by looking for new ways of counting familiar arrangements. I know there are
twenty-one pudding cups, because I see rows of 1, 2, 3, 4, 5, and 6. But how else could
someone see twenty-one in that triangle? I know from the bowling pins that ten is a
triangle with longest row of four. Can I find two of those triangles, with one left over? By
rearranging them a little bit I can, and my way of seeing it is in Figure 3.9.
Most of my expertise, though, comes from talking math with children. From them,
I have learned that you can always count a triangular number by spiraling in from the
longest row (Figure 3.10; I’ll describe the moment I learned this in Chapter 4).
In contrast to popular notions of school mathematics there is richness here, and there
are subtleties to explore. If we understand learning as developing expertise, then How
Did You Count? tasks—as well as other well-designed counting contexts—give us and
our students plenty of opportunity to be always learning.
82
USING HOW DID YOU COUNT?
FIGURE 3.10
A triangular number counted as a spiral, from the outside in.
83
HOW DID YOU COUNT?
Noticing Structure
One of the most productive mathematics questions is “How do you know?” Asking
“How do you know?” is different from asking “Why?” because “How do you know?” is
personal and the teacher cannot know its answer in advance. “Why?” is also a great
question, but is more likely to be tied to a predetermined explanation.
“Why?” sometimes even requires explaining a phenomenon unknowable to the child.
Why is 20 two tens? It just is! Compare that to “How do you know 20 is two tens?” The
possibilities expand here. Sure, you can say that you just know, but also “There is a 2 in
the tens place” or “Nineteen is ten plus nine, and twenty is one more than that, so now
you have a second ten.”
In the book How Many?, the question in the title sometimes requires the follow-up
question “How do you know?” To that question, children often reply, “I counted!” How
Did You Count? goes straight for justification, as this is where we have things to talk
about. In How Many?, we talk a lot about relationships between different units (e.g. eggs
and dozens, avocados and half-avocados, or slices and pizzas). In How Did You Count?,
we talk about the relationships between numbers (e.g. ten as the sum of the first four
counting numbers, or twenty-one as four groups of four, and five more).
Identifying Groups
Counting groups is the basis for both place value and multiplication. How Did You Count?
is a chance to play with counting groups, and to support learning both of these topics.
For example, one way to think about twenty-one is two groups of ten, and one more.
But also the pots of purple paint (Figure 3.5) can be seen as four groups of four, and
five more, or as five groups of four and one more. Only one of these is captured directly
when we write “21,” but it is all the same set of ideas. Place value is all about groups and
leftovers.
Similarly, any time we have four groups of five, we also make five groups of four.
If you practice looking for both of these you’ll get good at it pretty quickly, although
the two may not be equally clear in many situations. In any case, as this visual practice
becomes a habit of mind, it can also support a habit to manipulate abstract numbers in
the same way.
84
USING HOW DID YOU COUNT?
FIGURE 3.11
An array of avocados
Questioning Assumptions
Earlier, I wrote about the one-
to-one correspondence between
pits and avocados. It is such
a safe assumption that each
avocado has one pit that it
doesn’t seem worth questioning.
But you could. And if you do,
the direct evidence in the
avocado array (Figure 3.11) leads
you to the same conclusion—
there are still 7 21 avocados.
This can be a second role of
the question “How do you know?” To avoid annoying children with repetition, here are
some variants.
• Do we know that?
Every assumption can be tested by this sort of questioning. Early in the process, you
may need to ask the questions. Over time, your students will learn to do it too. The goal
is to set a habit of keeping mathematical claims close to the evidence at hand.
85
HOW DID YOU COUNT?
FIGURE 3.12
Identical arrangements of apples and half-apples
In How Did You Count? there are lots of places where we can make assumptions, and
where it is worth questioning them. The two apples photos, for example, are staged
with the same arrangement (Figure 3.12). This is to support noticing that half of twice
something is the thing you started with: 21 • (2 • 18) = 18.
Some students may notice the overall sameness of the setups: same pans, relatively full,
same-sized apples—and conclude that the two arrangements are the same. Other students
may question that assumption, perhaps owing to a defensiveness about trick questions.
Still others may not notice the similarity, or not notice that there is an assumption to
question. All students can benefit from pausing to notice that there is an assumption we
could make, and also to discuss how we might investigate the validity of the assumption.
Digging deeper, you might have made an assumption about the half-apples. Are
they thirty-six single halves of the apples from the previous photo? Or are they paired
halves of eighteen apples? Are they even halves at all, or could they be horizontal slices,
perhaps more than two per apple?
86
USING HOW DID YOU COUNT?
Did you make one of those assumptions in looking at the second photograph? Did
you assume something else? How might you investigate any of these assumptions? In
what sense does it matter what assumption you make?
As a wrapping-up note about assumptions, I’ll say that it was something of a mantra
in composing these images that if something looks true, it should be true. Those
spheres on the covers and endpapers of the book look like the same spheres in the
same numbers of the same colors because they are. The tetrahedron of basketballs
(Figure 3.4) looks like it is made entirely of basketballs that are about the same size, and
it is. There are no hidden tiny basketballs to make your count “wrong” when you assume
there are no tricks. (Although I will admit that some small cardboard rings and a tiny bit
of duct tape were necessary to hold the whole thing together, and you might be able to
spot the cardboard rings on the gym floor if you look very closely.)
87
HOW DID YOU COUNT?
FIGURE 3.13
Comparing Cheez-Its (left) and representations of Cheez-Its (right).
88
USING HOW DID YOU COUNT?
So yes, the older child got a more accurate result by manipulating an abstraction
rather than the crackers themselves. The precise truth of the claim was not the goal of
the activity, though. The process of noticing a claim, gathering evidence, and accepting,
rejecting, or amending the claim was the goal. Far from a distracting feature, context
can be a launching point for lots of mathematical exploration.
Of course, there is also a place for activities that begin with abstractions, and in
certain circumstances, the abstractions can become their own context. Go ahead and
have those Number Talks with arrangements of dots! Study place value using Unifix
cubes! Just don’t be misled that abstract manipulatives need to precede contextual
investigations.
So if a bowlful of tangerines—ready for you to peel and eat them—is reality,
then a photograph of that bowl is an abstraction. The photo has stripped away the
reality of the smells and flavors, and you cannot eat the photograph. A diagram
is another level of abstraction; it might represent those tangerines, or it might
represent children or teddy bears or any appropriately arranged group of twelve
things.
A numeric expression is a further abstraction. The expression 4 • 3 could be four
groups of three tangerines, or the number of legs on three dogs, or the number of feet
in four yards. 4 • 3 might be an example you use to investigate a claim that the product
of an even number and an odd number is always even.
Each of these rungs on the ladder of abstraction is important for math learning.
Moving between these rungs is even more important. This is one of the goals of
annotations, such as those in Figure 3.14. That example offers the opportunity to ask
questions such as the following:
• There are two threes in this expression. What does each represent?
• If instead of 3 • 6 + 3, someone wrote 7 • 3, what do you think the diagram would look
like?
89
HOW DID YOU COUNT?
FIGURE 3.14
Three groups of six, plus three more.
Whole Group
A favorite mode of interaction with the picture book—and my primary way when I visit
classrooms due to limited time—is displaying images from the book using classroom
90
USING HOW DID YOU COUNT?
media (an interactive whiteboard, computer projector, etc.). Before having students turn
and talk in pairs or small groups, I like to do some counting together in a large group. This
way I can model my expectations. Here are a few things I always include in my instructions:
• Wait time. Students need time to think. This means no waving hands in the air, and
no talking until a given amount of time has passed. You know your students and their
needs, but about a minute is a good amount of thinking time for getting a How Did
You Count? conversation started.
• Clear descriptions of strategies. If you counted by twos, tell us that! And also help us
to see the sets of two that you saw. This is an opportunity to practice spatial language
such as above, at the bottom, to the right, going around, etc. Gestures are important
for supporting this communication, so encourage gesturing together with language.
It is helpful to familiarize students with the routines of pausing for thought and
stating both a count and a technique. I tend to have them talk in small groups or pairs
or whatever structures the teacher already has in place for breaking conversations into
manageable-sized pieces so everyone can have a turn in a small amount of time.
Regardless of the age of participants or the sophistication of the mathematics I’m hoping
we’ll do together, I like to start with the first image in the picture book—the tangerines.
The structure of the tangerines is simple enough that everyone can be successful; yet the
arrangement has enough richness that there are several ways to count them.
To illustrate the utility of choosing a good starting place that is between simple and
rich, imagine a simple but not rich image with three tangerines. “How did you count
them?” could be rightly answered by most people by saying “I just know” because you
can subitize three tangerines. This leaves us with nothing to talk about. At the opposite
ends of these two scales, the basketball photograph is rich but not at all simple. We
need to build up to the complexity of the basketballs—often over several counting
sessions together. As a first exposure to How Did You Count?, the basketballs are
too complex. If we cannot agree how many there are, we are likely focused on that
disagreement rather than on seeing new structures.
91
HOW DID YOU COUNT?
Here are a couple of fun variations you might consider after you and your students
have a bit of experience with How Did You Count? conversations:
• Have students write the expressions for a counting strategy—perhaps their own
strategies, or maybe one that a classmate has talked about, or one you suggest.
• Have a student write an expression on the board, and ask the rest of the class to guess
how they counted.
• Tell students a total number—say 24—and have them write an expression for that
number (maybe (7 • 3) + 3). Then show them that many arranged objects. Can they
find their expression?
Small Group
You may choose to have students work through some How Did You Count? images in pairs
or small groups—especially after the routines have been established through whole-class
discussions. You could select about three prompts and have students move at their own
pace through the set. If you allow enough time for everyone to get through two of them,
you’ll likely give enough challenge to your faster-moving students while letting your slower-
moving students have enough time to engage. Remember that this isn’t about speed. Deep
thinking about a small number of prompts is preferable to rushing through a bunch of
them under time pressure. Noticing interesting details and relationships takes time.
Not everyone needs to do all of the same prompts. Depending on your classroom
environment and traditions, you may want to have students record some product of
their small-group work. They can record on paper, or mini whiteboards, or tablets quite
naturally. Typing responses on laptops or Chromebooks is likely to impede the kinds
of work students will want to do, which often includes drawing diagrams and arrows
pointing to parts of shapes.
92
USING HOW DID YOU COUNT?
graders to more than 100 parents and children at a family math night. But my absolute
favorite context is sitting down with one reading partner, or maybe two. At home, this
is the standard; at school it’s a luxury. But if it turns out to be a luxury you can afford—
ever—do it. Perhaps you have a few moments with a student whose bus is running late,
or a couple of students who eat lunch in your classroom on a regular basis. A small
number of math minds paying close, careful attention and delighting in each other’s
ideas—this is the best. Make time for it if you can.
Classroom Library
Consider putting a couple of copies of How Did You Count? in your classroom library.
File them with the other nonfiction books and give students an opportunity to think
quietly, to look deeply, and to create their own individual counting experiences.
Bulletin Board
A How Did You Count? bulletin board might have several images and a collection of
sticky notes and pens. Students can write their expressions or draw their diagrams on
a sticky note and affix it next to the image. Over the course of several days, these can
accumulate and become deeper and more complicated.
93
HOW DID YOU COUNT?
beyond the confines of the book, and that parents who don’t already do much math
talk at home find the open nature of the prompts liberating. Once this latter group
understands that literally any way of seeing the arrangement is correct, parents and
other caregivers often describe a feeling of freedom—freedom from the answer key, and
freedom from doing it wrong.
FIGURE 3.15
Many things to count in the bowling pins picture.
94
USING HOW DID YOU COUNT?
FIGURE 3.16
Many things to count in the milk photograph.
In these images, Gregg used annotations to attribute ideas to students (by way of
initials such as S, M, and An), and to tell the story of the conversations.
95
HOW DID YOU COUNT?
FIGURE 3.17
A visual pattern with two, five, ten,
and seventeen tiles.
FIGURE 3.18
Several ways to count the tiles in
stage 3.
96
USING HOW DID YOU COUNT?
FIGURE 3.19
If this is stage 4, what might stages 3 and 5 look like?
same, what changes, and can we see the same structures in this stage
that we saw in stage 3? In this case, we end up with 42 + 1, 3 * 4 + 5,
and 4 * 5 − 4. With more experience, showing stage 4 on its own was
less necessary. In either case, opening up to the first four stages and
asking students to write an expression for the nth stage was the final
step, not the starting place. Now all of my students had access to
ideas such as “It’s always a square, plus one—and the square’s side length is the same as
the stage number,” or “an n − 1 by n rectangle, and then a stack of n + 1 on the right-hand
side.” Students could then capture these ideas with expressions such as the following:
• n2 + 1
• (n – 1) • n + (n + 1)
• n • (n + 1) – (n – 1)
An additional way to mash up How Did You Count? and Visual Patterns, which I
learned from my math teaching friend and former colleague Michael Fenton, is to show
a single image to students, and ask, “If this is stage 4, what might stages 3 and 5 look
like?” (Figure 3.19).
97
HOW DID YOU COUNT?
• Tidy arrangements with missing objects, such as an egg carton with three missing eggs.
• Tidy arrangements with extra objects, such as a full egg carton with another three
eggs next to it on a countertop.
• Things arranged in a way that makes a larger shape, such as a square, triangle, or
hexagon.
• Some combination of these (e.g. a hexagon with spaces for two missing objects).
If you make an interesting arrangement the focal point of your image, you’ll have at
least one extra layer of mathematical depth (beyond simple counting) that will take a
moment for students to notice and to talk about. Look for examples in your life, and in
your students’ lives.
Make It Clear
It’s fun to have a variety of things to count, but having too many things distracts from
the conversation. Keep background clutter to a minimum and help students focus on a
smaller, related variety of counting targets.
98
USING HOW DID YOU COUNT?
or easy—even in my own home. It’s like I thought the marshmallows would be shy
about having their photo taken, so I needed to do it quickly while they weren’t paying
attention. These images turned out not to work as well by the time I projected them
in front of a crowded classroom. Now I understand that I need to take my time, get
right up in the marshmallows’ faces (figuratively speaking, of course), and fill the frame
with the thing I want my students to see. This isn’t Where’s Waldo? where the fun is
in hunting for a small thing hidden in a sea of noise. This is about drawing learners’
attention to bold images of mathematical relationships in the world. Make it easy to see
something that might otherwise be easy to overlook.
Attend to Lighting
Shadows can be trouble, and can make it harder to distinguish the subject of your
photograph. I have a favorite spot near the large front windows of my workshop that
gets lots of natural light—especially in the afternoon, when the light is indirect through
these east-facing windows. I can be spotted from the street very carefully arranging all
sorts of odd objects.
Keep an eye out for similar spots in your own home and school—well lighted, lightly
trafficked areas where you can take a minute or two to compose your photo. Of course
there are lots of great How Did You Count? images that are at too large a scale to be
able to choose your location. Objects on the scale of trains, parking lots, buildings, large
crowds of people, and trees may provide some great conversations, but you can’t select
where to photograph them. You can usually pick the angle, though, and sometimes
even the time of day or the weather. In any case, developing a habit of considering the
options for lighting before taking a picture will—over time—increase the quality of the
math you and your students are able to discuss.
99
CHAPTER 4
Mathematical Encounters
A
n important test I apply to most of my ideas is whether they provoke
interesting thinking in people with a wide range of expertise. The pudding
cups (Figure 3.9) are arranged as a triangle. If you know about triangular
numbers, you can use that knowledge in this task. But you don’t have
to know about them to participate. Furthermore, later images will likely stretch the
boundaries of your thinking about triangular numbers—by combining two triangular
numbers or moving into the third dimension.
In that spirit, this chapter shares counting strategies shared by mathematical thinkers
of diverse ages in response to How Did You Count? images that demonstrate many
levels of mathematical sophistication. Ideally most readers will recognize their own
thinking somewhere in this chapter, and also take away something new.
100
M at h e mati c a l En c o u nt e r s
and I moved on to other tasks. About fifteen minutes later, after some downtime and
cotton candy, I noticed Gwen back on the job. She flagged me down to let me know
there were thirty-six apples, which she demonstrated by counting by threes in each pan
of apples.
The grapes she counted by twelves (three groups of four green grapes makes twelve,
then another set like that for twenty-four, etc.) until she ran out of known multiples of
twelve. At that point, Gwen finished counting by ones. For the large arrangement of
dice, she announced, “This one I just have to count by fives!”
I have to admit that I was nervous through the first part of this conversation. Was
Gwen going to count everything one-by-one? Perhaps it was the bathroom break;
perhaps it was the cotton candy; perhaps she was just ready to try something new.
Whatever triggered it, I was delighted (and admittedly a bit relieved) by her new
strategies in the second half of the book.
Gwen’s counting story is an example of the power of persistence—hers and mine—for
if either of us had given up, she would never have applied more sophisticated tools to
these tasks. It is also a reminder to me about assumptions of prerequisites. If we only ever
give students simple tasks that they can complete with simple methods, they’ll never
need to develop more complicated methods. We all need tasks that make us stretch.
101
HOW DID YOU COUNT?
One of my fears with teaching things like triangular numbers is that they’ll be seen
as the goal themselves. If I have a list of triangular numbers memorized—or square
numbers, or even a formula for either one of these—I might rely on that list so much
that I can’t work with things that aren’t on the list. I worry about training students to
associate this list of numbers with triangles rather than equipping students with tools
for analysis.
I need not have worried about that with Asa. He met something he didn’t know and
used the things he did to make sense of the new situation.
If I’m being perfectly honest, though, the part of How Did You Count? that Asa
found most compelling was a two-page spread that had been created by a missing
page in my prototype (Figure 4.1). The left-hand page read “How many soccer balls?
FIGURE 4.1
A missing page generates an unexpected answer.
102
M at h e mati c a l En c o u nt e r s
How did you count them?” and the right-hand page had the first apple photo. Asa
delightedly pointed out to me that—strictly speaking—there were no soccer balls at all.
FIGURE 4.2
A triangular pyramid of plastic
spheres as layers of triangular
numbers.
103
HOW DID YOU COUNT?
other direction, seeing things arranged as a triangle (such as the layers of the sphere
pyramid) means they’ll be easy to count because she knows lots of triangular numbers
by heart. There are twenty-one pudding cups because the triangle has a base of six
pudding cups.
FIGURATE NUMBERS
At a surface level, neatly arranged everyday objects give us something pretty to look
at while we practice counting, and How Did You Count? certainly has an aesthetic. At
a deeper level, these images connect big mathematical ideas to our everyday lives.
Bowling pins are not just arbitrarily arranged in groups of ten; they are in triangles. Eggs
are in rectangles. You can make and see groupings and shapes with just about anything.
Beyond the four operations of arithmetic (addition, subtraction, multiplication,
and division), algebra has many geometry structures built in. A student has a much
better chance of learning to work fluently with something like x 2 + 3x + 2 if they’re not
mystified about why x2 is pronounced “x-squared.” In fact drawing x 2 as a square is a
great first step in rewriting x 2 + 3x + 2 as (x + 2) (x + 1), as in Figure 4.3.
FIGURE 4.3
Two expressions for the area of the same rectangle.
104
M at h e mati c a l En c o u nt e r s
Numbers of things arranged to look like squares, triangles, and hexagons are not
standards in elementary school. Nevertheless, they do support grade-level learning
about patterns, structures, and number relationships, and they offer students a chance
to become familiar with these ideas so that they aren’t mysterious when meeting them
later on.
A square number is a number you get by multiplying a whole number by itself.
3 • 3 = 9, so nine is a square number. The process of multiplying a number by
itself is called squaring, and is often indicated with an exponent of 2, so we can
write 32 = 9, and read this as “three squared equals nine.” The same notation and
language go with algebraic symbols, so that x • x = x 2, and we can read x 2 as
“x-squared.”
But why “squared” for
multiplying a number by itself?
The answer is because there
is a relationship between
square numbers and squares.
In particular, a 3-inch by
3-inch square has an area of
9-square inches (Figure 4.4),
and similarly if you have nine
things, you can arrange them
in a square-ish sort of way as
three rows of three (Figure 4.5).
The same is true for any square
number of objects—you can
make an arrangement that has
the same number of rows as
columns.
FIGURE 4.4
Squaring three
105
HOW DID YOU COUNT?
FIGURE 4.5
A square of wooden circles
106
M at h e mati c a l En c o u nt e r s
FIGURE 4.6
A triangle of bowling pins
107
HOW DID YOU COUNT?
FIGURE 4.7
A tetrahedron (or triangular pyramid) of basketballs
The hip hop band De La Soul has a song about three being the magic number,
which is itself a reference to an old Schoolhouse Rock video dating back to my own
childhood, entitled “Three Is the Magic Number.” My friend Faith’s son Clem watched
this Schoolhouse Rock video many times one spring, and began to see the world in
terms of threes.
Faith and I were talking about an early draft of an image from How Did You Count? (at
left in Figure 4.8), and she found herself counting these oranges by threes, thinking of
the video, and she posited that Clem might also do so. She shared the image with Clem
early the next morning, and sure enough, three was the magic number for Clem as well.
Faith sent me the annotated version of Clem’s counting that is at right in Figure 4.8.
108
Mathematical Encounters
FIGURE 4.8
A hexagon of tangerines (left) and how Clem counted them (right).
109
HOW DID YOU COUNT?
FIGURE 4.9
Groups of grapes
110
M at h e mati c a l En c o u nt e r s
FIGURE 4.10
Juice boxes, not bowling pins
111
HOW DID YOU COUNT?
are” are now both reasonable answers to the question “How did you count?” Ideally, this
would set us up for opportunities to notice this arrangement embedded in later images,
such as the pudding cups, eggs, basketballs, or the first dice picture.
To do that though, we need to make the bowling pin structure familiar. “How could
we check that there are ten?” and “Tell us more about how you know these pins are
properly arranged, with no extras or missing pins” are examples of teacher moves that
elicit the 1 + 2 + 3 + 4 structure, which repeats in those other images.
FIGURE 4.11
Megadice—how many dots on all
the dice combined?
112
M at h e mati c a l En c o u nt e r s
FIGURE 4.12
Super-Megadice—how many dots
on the tops of all the dice?
113
HOW DID YOU COUNT?
“Well, each five is half of a ten. So you could have ten groups of (ten groups of ten),
which would be 1,000,” he began.
Somehow the rest of his thinking was difficult to articulate, but together we worked
to make explicit that ( 21 of 10) • ( 21 of 10) • ( 21 of 10) is the same as ( 21 • 21 • 21 ) of (10 • 10 •
10), or 81 of 1,000, which agrees with the elementary children’s count of 125.
If you continue Barry’s logic, then the Super-Megadice photo (Figure 4.12) should
have 161 of 10,000 dots. Does it?
FIGURE 4.13
Tetrahedron of tangerines
114
Mathematical Encounters
FIGURE 4.14
Tangerine tetrahedron, decomposed three ways.
115
HOW DID YOU COUNT?
Other people dissect the larger tetrahedron into smaller ones, as in the lower left in
Figure 4.14. Doing that results in five smaller tetrahedra, each with four tangerines. This
one seems to require different visualization skills than the layers strategy. Often, people
count the four groups of four at the corners, and then imagine more tangerines in
between than there actually are, resulting in frequent counts of twenty-one.
Finally, some people slice the tetrahedron into rectangular layers, starting along one
edge, as in the lower right of Figure 4.14. That gives us four rectangles: 1 by 4, 2 by 3, 3
by 2, and 4 by 1.
116
M at h e mati c a l En c o u nt e r s
FIGURE 4.15
Thirty-six is both square (left) and triangular (right).
no reason to squash this quest. I feel the same way about mathematical missions, except
that there is even less danger and the child truly cannot wreck anything.
In teaching there is an implicit social contract, and as teachers, we should be mindful
of that. Announcing “Your homework tonight is to find the next number after thirty-six
that is both square and triangular” is a violation of that contract in most classrooms.
Students have a reasonable expectation not to be required to complete a task at which
the teacher knows they will fail. At the other end of things though, telling students that
the next such number is beyond their grasp precludes the potential for some excellent
mathematical investigation.
Thirty-six is the eighth triangular number. To get the ninth one, you add nine (36 + 9 =
45). In general, if you know the nth triangular number, you add n + 1 to get the next
one. But thirty-six is the sixth square number. To get the seventh one, you add thirteen,
which is 2 • 6 + 1. In general if you know the nth square number, you add 2n + 1 to get
the next one. These are beautiful patterns, and useful for lots of mathematical work.
117
HOW DID YOU COUNT?
Each pattern relies on the simple concept of next. If encouraging a search for the next
number that is both square and triangular elicits some of these ideas, I say Let’s do it!
The dream here is a child who reports back later on, saying something like “I don’t
think there is another one—I tried the next ten square numbers and none of them was
triangular.” Such a claim on its own represents a mathematical achievement and is
worthy of celebration!
In case it comes up in your classroom, zero and one are also both square and
triangular, and the next such number after thirty-six is 1,225, which is 352, and also the
forty-ninth triangular number.
FIGURE 4.16
Spice jar shaker top. How many holes?
How did you count them?
118
M at h e mati c a l En c o u nt e r s
I hope that you and your students will notice number structures and geometry
everywhere. I hope that you’ll generate new answers to old textbook questions, new
ways of grouping the objects in your classroom, and perhaps new ways of grouping
yourselves! I hope you will find joy, like my mathematician friend does, in finding
simplicity (such as counting fives in the Megadice photo) in complex things, and
complexity (such as many ways to count tangerines) in simple things. And I hope that—
through that joy—you will all build new mathematical strengths that will serve you well
in your future endeavors, both inside and outside of math classrooms.
119
CHAPTER 5
Answers Key
W
hen I wrote Which One Doesn’t Belong? I was hesitant to include an
answer key. My experience has repeatedly demonstrated to me that
“the back of the book” holds too much power in the mathematical
relationship among students, teachers, and content. Additionally,
Which One Doesn’t Belong?—like How Many? and How Did You Count?—was written
deliberately to have multiple right answers, not all of which I could know in advance.
At the same time, I understood that I had something useful to share with teachers as
a consequence of talking with hundreds of learners about each of the tasks in the book.
I knew some answers that I hoped for in designing these images, but I also had learned
about answers I could not have imagined on my own. I knew a lot about how likely each
of these answers was to arise in a variety of classrooms.
I shared this dilemma with my friend and colleague Michael Fenton, who had the
brilliant idea of making it plural: “Answers Key." Where Answer Key carries some baggage
about mathematical authority, Answers Key signals a questioning of that authority.
Please consult this Answers Key in that spirit. I want to support you and your students
in noticing a wide range of possible structures. I want the Answers Key to model and
validate divergent thinking. I hope you will annotate your copy of this Answers Key so
that your own version becomes richer and more interesting than mine.
One final word of clarification about the following pages. Each image shows one
way of seeing the structure of the objects—bowling pins, basketballs, or whatever—and
at least one corresponding way of using symbols to represent that structure. These
images do not represent an ideal of what your own board should look like at any stage
of the conversation. You and your students will use colors and arrows and gestures and
language to make communication about their ideas more clear.
120
ANSWERS KEY
BOWLING PINS
121
HOW DID YOU COUNT?
PUDDING CUPS
122
ANSWERS KEY
EGGS
123
HOW DID YOU COUNT?
PAINT POTS
124
ANSWERS KEY
BASKETBALLS
The top two answers here depict ways to count all of the basketballs. The bottom one
shows a common way of counting the visible basketballs.
125
HOW DID YOU COUNT?
SOCCER BALLS
126
ANSWERS KEY
APPLES
127
HOW DID YOU COUNT?
HALF APPLES
128
ANSWERS KEY
SHOES
129
HOW DID YOU COUNT?
FLOWERS
130
ANSWERS KEY
GRAPES
131
HOW DID YOU COUNT?
DICE
132
ANSWERS KEY
MEGADICE
133
HOW DID YOU COUNT?
FINALE
134
References
Cameron, Antonia. 2020. Early Childhood Math Routines: Empowering Young Minds to
Think. Portsmouth, NH: Stenhouse.
Clements, Douglas H. 1999. “Subitizing. What is it? Why teach it?” Teaching Children
Mathematics 5 (7): 400–5.
Deans for Impact. 2019. The Science of Early Learning. Austin, TX: Deans for Impact.
Dehaene, Stanislas. 2011. The Number Sense: How the Mind Creates Mathematics.
Revised ed. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.
Fletcher, Graham, and Tracy Johnston Zager. 2020. Building Fact Fluency. Portsmouth,
NH: Stenhouse.
Gutiérrez, Rochelle. 2007. “Context matters: Equity, success, and the future of
mathematics education.” In T. Lamberg and L.R. Wiest (Eds.), Proceedings of the
29th Annual Meeting of the North American Chapter of the International Group for
the Psychology of Mathematics Education. Stateline (Lake Tahoe), NV: University of
Nevada, Reno.
Humphreys, Cathy, and Ruth Parker. 2015. Making Number Talks Matter: Developing
Mathematical Practices and Deepening Understanding, Grades 4–10. Portsmouth, NH:
Stenhouse.
Stigler, James W., and James Hiebert. 2009. The Teaching Gap: Best Ideas from the
World’s Teachers for Improving Education in the Classroom. New York: Free Press.