Intro To Linguistics
Intro To Linguistics
Intro To Linguistics
Area: ENGLISH
Competencies:
1. demonstrate familiarity with the theories of language and language learning and
their influence on language teaching
2. revisit the knowledge of linguistic theories and concepts and apply it to the
teaching of communication skills – listening, speaking, reading, writing, and
grammar
3. show understanding of how language rules are used in real conversations
3. Helping students to discover the meaning of words by parsing them into small
parts depends heavily on the teacher’s knowledge of morphology or word
formation rules. To exemplify, students may parse or segment the following words,
taking note of the morpheme {-ment} that recurs in embarrassment, government,
disillusionment, enhancement. As students discover the meaning of {-ment} as
‘state or condition’, they may be able to give the meaning of the cited examples as:
‘state of being embarrassed’, ‘state of governing’, ‘state of being disillusioned’,
and ‘state of enhancing’. Hence, the process of word formation such as derivation
may help learners interpret and remember meaning of words that follow certain
patterns in forming short words into longer words.
4. Teachers’ knowledge about larger units of language use – discourse structure –
may be relevant when teaching exchanges or conversations. The use of language
for social functions such as asking permission involves familiarity with modals that
express formality and a higher degree of politeness when speaking with someone
who is older, who occupies a higher position, or is an authority than the speaker. In
this context appropriacy has to be observed in selecting modals. For example, it is
appropriate to use may, not can when asking permission from someone who is
older, higher in position than the speaker. e.g. May I use the office computer?
That language is arbitrary means that the relationship between the words and the
‘things’ they denote is merely conventional, i.e. native speakers of English, in
some sense, agreed to use the sounds / kæt / ‘cat’ in English because native
speakers of English ‘want’ it to be.
Language is innate. The presence of the language acquisition device (LAD) in the
human brain predisposes all normal children to acquire their first language in an
amazingly short time, around five years since birth.
Language is universal. It is universal in the sense that all normal children the world
over acquire a mother tongue but it is also universal in the sense that, at a highly
abstract level, all languages must share key features of human languages, such as
all languages have sounds; all languages have rules that form sounds into words,
words into phrases and clauses; and all languages have transformation rules that
enable speakers to ask questions, negate sentences, issue orders, defocus the doer
of the action, etc.
2. The functionalists believe that language is a dynamic system through which
members of speech community exchange information. It is a vehicle for the
expression of functional meaning such as expressing one’s emotions, persuading
people, asking and giving information, making people do things for others.
This view of language emphasizes the meaning and functions rather than the
grammatical characteristics of language, and leads to a language teaching content
consisting of categories of meaning/notions and functions rather than of elements
of structure and grammar.
B. Acquisition of Language
It regards language learning as a behavior like other forms of human behavior, not
a mental phenomenon, learned by a process of habit formation. Since language is
viewed as mechanistic and as a human activity, it is believed that learning a
language is achieved by building up habits on the basis of stimulus-response
chains. Behaviorism emphasizes the consequences of the response and argues that
it is the behavior that follows a response which reinforces it and thus helps to
strengthen the association.
a. The child imitates the sounds and patterns which s/he hears around her/him.
b. People recognize the child’s attempts as being similar to the adult models and
reinforce (reward) the sounds by approval or some other desirable reaction.
c. In order to obtain more of these rewards, the child repeats the sounds and patterns
so that these become habits.
d. In this way the child’s verbal behavior is conditioned (‘shaped’) until the habits
coincide with the adult models.
The behaviorists claim that the three crucial elements of learning are: a stimulus,
which serves to elicit behavior; a response triggered by the stimulus, and
reinforcement, which serves to mark the response as being appropriate (or
inappropriate) and encourages the repetition (or suppression) of the response.
3. Krashen’s Monitor Model (1981). Probably this is the most often cited among
theories of second language acquisition; considered the most comprehensive, if not
the most ambitious, consisting of five central hypotheses:
a. The acquisition/ learning hypothesis. It claims that there are two ways of
developing competence in L2:
Acquisition - the subconscious process that results from informal, natural
communication between people where language is a means, not a focus nor an end,
in itself.
Learning - the conscious process of knowing about language and being able to talk
about it, that occurs in a more formal situation where the properties or rules of a
language are taught. Language learning has traditionally involved grammar and
vocabulary learning.
Krashen suggests that monitoring occurs when there is sufficient time, where there
is pressure to communicate correctly and not just convey meaning, and when the
appropriate rules are known.
d. The input hypothesis. Krashen proposes that when learners are exposed to
grammatical features a little beyond their current level (i.e., i + 1), those features
are ‘acquired’. Acquisition results from comprehensible input, which is made
understandable with the help provided by the context. If learners receive
understandable input, language structures will be naturally acquired. Ability to
communicate in a second language ‘emerges’ rather than indirectly put in place by
teaching.
c. The affective filter hypothesis. Filter consists of attitude to language, motivation,
self-confidence and anxiety. Thus learners with favorable attitude and self-
confidence may have a ‘low filter’ which promotes language learning. Learners
with a low affective filter seek and receive more input, interact with confidence,
and are more receptive to the input they are exposed to. On the other hand, anxious
learners have a high affective filter which prevents acquisition from taking place.
2. Phonetics. It studies language at the level of sounds: how sounds are articulated by
the human speech mechanism and received by the auditory mechanism, as well as
how sounds can be distinguished and characterized by the manner in which they
are produced.
Morphology is the study of word formation; it deals with the internal structure of
words. It also studies the changes that take place in the structure of words, e.g. the
morpheme ‘go’ changes to ‘went’ and ‘gone’ to signify changes in tense and
aspect.
4. Syntax. It deals with how words combine to form phrases, phrases combine to
form clauses, and clauses conjoin to make sentences. Syntax is the study of the
way phrases, clauses and sentences are constructed. It is the system of rules and
categories that underlies sentence formation. It also involves the description of
rules, of positioning of elements in the sentence such as noun phrases, verb
phrases, adverbial phrases, etc.
Syntax also attempts to describe how these elements function in the sentence, i.e.,
the function that they perform in the sentence. For example, the noun phrase “the
student” has different functions in the following sentences:
In sentence a), the student functions as the subject of the sentence while in
sentence b), it functions as indirect object.
7. Discourse. It is the study of chunks of language which are bigger than a single
sentence. At this level, inter-sentential links that form a connected or cohesive text
are analyzed. The unit of language studied in discourse and pragmatics may be an
utterance in an exchange or a text in written form.
Phonology:
The /t/ in top is aspirated [th]; the /t/ in stop is released [t]; the /t/ in pot is
unreleased [t7].
3. Sounds are categorized into two major classes: vowels and consonants.
4. Consonant sounds are produced with some restriction or closure in the vocal tract
as the air from the lungs is pushed through the glottis out the mouth. The airflow
is either blocked momentarily or restricted so much that noise is produced as air
flows past the constriction. Consonants are described in terms of physical
dimensions: place of articulation, manner of articulation, voicing, as shown in
Figure 1.
Labiodental
Interdental
Alveolar
Bilabial
Palatal
Glottal
Velar
Stops voiceless p t k
voiced b d g
Fricatives voiceless f θ s š h
voiced v ð z ž
Affricates voiceless č
voiced ǰ
Nasals voiceless
voiced m n ŋ
Liquids voiceless
voiced l r
Glides voiceless
voiced w y
Source: Parker, F. & K. Riley. (1994). Linguistics for Non-Linguists.Boston: Allyn and
Bacon.
FIGURE 1. Consonant Phonemes of English
(a) Bilabial (from bi ‘two’ + labial ‘lips’). The primary constriction is at the
lips (/p,b,m,w/).
(b) Labiodental (from labio ‘lip’ + dental ‘teeth’). The primary constriction
is between the lower lip and the upper teeth (/f,v/).
(c) Interdental (from inter ‘between’ + dental ‘teeth’). The primary
constriction is between the tongue and the upper teeth (/θ,ð/).
(d) Alveolar (from alveolar ridge). The primary constriction is between the
tongue and the alveolar ridge (/t,d,s,z,n,l/).
(e) Palatal (from palate). The primary constricton is between the tongue and the palate
(/š,ž,č,ǰ,r,y/).
(f) Velar (from velum). The primary constriction is between the tongue and
the velum (/k,g,ŋ/).
(g) Glottal (from glottis, which refers to the space between the vocal cords). The primary
constriction is at the glottis (/h/).
(a) Stops. Two articulators (lips, tongue, teeth, etc.) are brought together such that the
flow of air through the vocal tract is completely blocked (/p,b,t,d,k,g/).
(b) Fricatives. Two articulators are brought near each other such that the flow of air is
impeded but not completely blocked. The air flow through the narrow opening
creates friction, hence the term fricative (/f,v,θ,ð,s,z,š,ž,h/).
(c) Affricates. Articulations corresponding to affricates are those that begin like stops
(with a complete closure in the vocal tract) and end like fricatives (with a narrow
opening in the vocal tract) (/č,ǰ/). Because affricates can be described as a stop plus
a fricative, some phonemic alphabets transcribe / č/ as /tš/ and /ǰ/ as /dž/.
(d) Nasals. A nasal articulation is one in which the airflow through the mouth is
completely blocked but the velum is lowered, forcing the air through the nose
(/m,n,ŋ/).
(e) Liquids and Glides. Both of these terms describe articulations that are mid-way
between true consonants (i.e., stops, fricatives, affricates, and nasals) and vowels,
although they are both generally classified as consonants. Liquid is a cover term
for all l-like and r-like articulations (/l,r/).
+bilabial +bilabial
/p/ = +stop /b/ = +stop
−voice +voice
5. Vowels are produced with little obstruction in the vocal tract and are generally
voiced. They are described in terms of the following physical dimensions: tongue
height, frontness, lip rounding, tenseness. Different parts of the tongue may be
raised or lowered. The lips may be spread or pursed. The passage through which
the air travels, however, is never narrow as to obstruct the free flow of the
airstream.
Vowel sounds carry pitch and loudness; one can sing vowels. They may be long or
short.
Front Back
i u
ɪ ℧
Hig
h Tense
e o
Lax
ε Λ (ә)
Mid
Ɔ
æ a
Source: Parker, F. & K. Riley. (1994). Linguistics for Non-Linguists.Boston: Allyn and
Bacon.
6. Suprasegmentals are prosodic properties that form part of the makeup of sounds
no matter what their place or manner of articulation is. These properties are pitch,
intonation, stress, and juncture. They are variations in intensity, pitch, and
timing.
e.g.
2 1 2 1 1 2
fundamental introductory secondary
8. Pitch is the auditory property of a sound that enables us to place it on a scale that
ranges from low to high.
9. Intonation is the rise and fall of pitch which may contrast meanings of sentences.
The pitch movement in spoken utterances is not only related to differences in the
word meaning, but serves to convey information of a broadly meaningful nature
such as completeness or incompleteness of an utterance. Intonation refers to the
pitch contours as they occur in phrases and sentences.
In English, the statement ‘Marian is a linguist’ ends with a fall in pitch while as a
question, ‘Marian is a linguist?’ the pitch goes up.
10. Juncture refers to the pauses or breaks between syllables. It refers to the
transition between sounds. The lack of any real break between syllables of words is
referred to as close juncture; plus juncture or open juncture is used to describe a
break or pause between syllables in the same word or adjacent word; e.g. nitrate
vs. night rate; why try vs. white rye; black bird vs. blackbird
Morphology:
The word unhappiness has 3 morphemes: {un-}, {happy}, {-ness} while the
word salamander is a single morpheme.
2. Allomorphs are morphs which belong to the same morpheme. For example, /s/, /z/
and /əz/ in /kæts/ ‘cats’, /bægz/ ‘bags’ and / bΛsəz/ ‘buses’ are allomorphs of the
plural morphemes {(e)s}. Allomorphs are variants of a morpheme that may be
phonologically or morphologically conditioned; e.g. {-en} as in oxen and children
are allomorphs of {plural} morpheme.
3. Free morphemes are those that can stand on their own as independent words, e.g.
{happy} in unhappily, {like} in dislike, {boy} in boyhood. They can also occur in
isolation; e.g. {happy}, {like}
4. Bound morphemes are those that cannot stand on their own as independent
words. They are always attached to a free morpheme or a free form, e.g. {un-}, {-
ly}, {dis-} {-hood}. Such morphemes are also called affixes.
Bound morphemes are those that cannot stand alone as words; they need to be
attached to another morpheme; e.g. {con-}; {de-}, {per-} to be attached to {-ceive}
as in conceive, deceive, perceive.
5. Inflectional morphemes are those that never change the form class of the words
or morphemes to which they are attached. They are always attached to complete
words. They cap the word; they are a closed-ended set of morphemes - English has
only 8 inflectional morphemes.
6. Derivational morphemes are those that are added to root morphemes or stems to
derive new words. They usually change the form class of the words to which they
are attached; they are open-ended, that is, there are potentially infinite number of
them; e.g. actual + {-ize} actualize; help + {-ful} helpful; {un-} + lucky
unlucky.
Derivation. This involves the addition of a derivational affix, changing the syntactic
category of the item to which it is attached (e.g., discern (V) discernment (N);
woman (N) womanly (Adj)).
Category Extension. This involves the extension of a morpheme from one syntactic
category to another (e.g., house (N) house (V); fast (Adj) fast (Adv))
Blend. It is a combination of parts of two pre-existing forms (e.g., smog < smoke +
fog; motel < motor + hotel).
Acronym. It is a word formed from the first letter(s) of each word in a phrase (e.g.,
NASA < National Aeronautics and Space Administration; SARS < Severe Acute
Respiratory Syndrome).
Abbreviation. It is a word formed from the names of the first letters of the
prominent syllables of a word (e.g., TV < television) or of words in a phrase (e.g.,
FBI < Federal Bureau of Investigation).
Proper Name. This process forms a word from a proper name (e.g., hamburger <
Hamburg (Germany); sandwich < Earl of Sandwich).
Folk Etymology. This process forms a word by substituting a common native form
for an exotic (often foreign) form (e.g., cockroach < Spanish cucuracha ‘wood
louse’).
Back Formation. This process forms a word by removing what is mistaken for an
affix (e.g. edit < editor; beg < beggar).
8. Morphophonemic Processes
There are processes that produce a great deal of linguistic variability: assimilation,
dissimilation, deletion, epenthesis, metathesis.
Assimilation is a process that results from a sound becoming more like another
nearby sound in terms of one or more of its phonetic characteristics; a process in
which segments take on the characteristics of neighboring sounds; e.g. probable –
improbable; potent -impotent; separable – inseparable; sensitive – insensitive
Syntactic Structures
1. Structure of Predication has two components: a subject and a predicate; e.g. the
seagull flies, the water level rose abruptly, the trial has begun
Semantics
1. Lexical ambiguity refers to a characteristic of a word that has more than one
sense, e. g. the English word fly is ambiguous because it has more than one
meaning: ‘an insect,’ ‘a zipper on a pair of pants,’ or ‘a baseball hit into the air
with a bat.’
2. Syntactic ambiguity refers to the characteristic of a phrase that has more than one
meaning, e.g. English literature teacher can mean ‘a teacher of English literature’
or ‘a literature teacher who is from England.’
3. Synonymy refers to words having the same sense; that is, they have the same
values for all of their semantic features. happy and glad; reply and respond; hastily
and hurriedly are synonymous words in English.
5. Antonymy refers to the characteristic of two words which are different both in form
as well as meaning. An antonym conveys the opposite sense (binary antonyms),
e.g. rich - poor; good – bad. They are also words whose meanings differ only in
the value for a single semantic feature; e.g. rich – poor; rich is marked [+wealth]
and poor is marked [- wealth]; dead – alive; dead is marked [-life] and alive is
marked [+life]. Gradable antonyms are words that describe opposite ends of a
continuous dimension, e.g. hot and cold. Not everything that can be hot or cold is,
in fact, either hot or cold. Liquid, for example, may be warm or cool.
6. Homonymy refers to sense relation in words with the same phonetic form but
different in meaning, e.g. bat meaning ‘a nocturnal animal’ and bat meaning ‘an
equipment used in baseball or softball.’
7. Coreference refers to the sense relation of two expressions that have the same
extralinguistic referent. In the sentence “Mercury is the nearest planet from the
sun,” Mercury and the nearest planet from the sun are coreferential because they
both refer to the same extralinguistic object – the planet Mercury in the solar
system.
9. Deixis refers to the characteristic of an expression that has one meaning but can
refer to different entities within the same context of utterance. Deictic expressions
have a ‘pointing function.’ Examples of deixis are you, I, she (personal pronouns);
here, there, right, left, (expressions of place); this, that, those, these
(demonstratives); now, yesterday, today, last year (time expressions).
Pragmatics
1. Speech act theory. Every utterance of speech constitutes some sort of act
(promising, apologizing, threatening, warning, etc.). Every speech act consists of
three separate acts:
Locutionary force an act of saying something; it is a description of what a speaker
says, e.g., I promise to return your book tomorrow.
Illocutionary act/force is the act of doing something; it is what the speaker intends
to do by uttering a sentence, e.g., by saying “I promise to return your book
tomorrow,” the speaker has made an act of promising.
Perlocutionary act is an act of affecting someone (i.e., the listener); it is the effect
on the hearer of what a speaker says, e.g., by saying “I will return your book
tomorrow,” the hearer may feel happy or relieved that s/he will get the book back
3. Conversational Maxims are rules that are observed when communication takes
place in a situation where people are co-operative. When people communicate,
they assume that the other person will be cooperative and they themselves wish to
cooperate.
Maxim of quality – a participant should not say that which is false or that which
the participant lacks evidence - “Make your contribution such that it is true; do not
say what you know is false or for which you do not have adequate evidence.”
e.g. A: Who did you see enter the room last?
B: The janitor
4. Implicatures refer to statements that imply a proposition that is not part of the
utterance and does not follow as a necessary consequence of the utterance.
For example: Dan says to his wife Nitz,“Uncle Ernie is driving us to Tagaytay”
to which Nitz responds, “I guess I’d better take tranquilizers.” Nitz’s utterance
raises the implicature that Uncle Ernie must be a fast, reckless driver.