READING
READING
READING
You should spend about 20 minutes on Questions 1-13, which are based on Reading
Passage 1 on pages 2 and 3.
The Origin of Paper
The word paper derives from the Greek term for the ancient Egyptian writing material
called papyrus. In about 2400 BC, the Egyptians discovered how to make a writing surface
out of papyrus, a type of reed that grows along waterways in southern Europe and North
Africa. The Egyptians cut the plant into strips which they softened in water.
Papyrus was cross-woven into a mat and then pounded into a hard thin sheet.
As the papyrus plant requires subtropical conditions to grow, papyrus was not much used
in Europe at that time; instead, the main material used for writing was parchment.
This was made from animal skin and was extremely expensive. In fact, it has been
estimated that a single book written on parchment required the skins of 300 sheep. The
skins had to be specifically prepared by drying them and they were then stretched on a
special frame. It is not known when parchment was first introduced, but it was the main
writing material in Europe for hundreds of years.
Paper, which is made from pulp, rags, and fibers of plants, seems to have been invented in
China and is considered to be one of the Four Great Inventions of Ancient China. In
105 AD, under the Han Dynasty emperor Ho-Ti, a government official in China named Ts'ai
Lun was the first to start a papermaking industry. Ts'ai Lun seems to have made his paper
by mixing finely chopped mulberry bark and hemp rags with water, mashing the mixture
flat with a stone mortar, and then pressing out the water and letting it dry in the sun. He
may have based his idea on bark cloth, which was very common in China and also made
from mulberry.
Previously, during the Shang (1600-1050 BC) and Zhou (1050-256 BC dynasties of
Ancient China, documents were ordinarily written on bone or bamboo (on tablets or on
bamboo strips sewn and rolled together into scrolls), making them very heavy and
awkward to transport. The light material of silk was sometimes used, but was normally too
expensive to consider. When it was first invented, paper was used for purposes of
wrapping or padding protection for delicate bronze mirrors. Although paper used for
writing became popular by the 3rd century AD, it continued to be used for wrapping and
other purposes.
During the Tang Dynasty (618-907 AD) paper was folded and sewn into square bags to
preserve the flavor of tea. During the same period, according to a written account, tea was
served from baskets with multi-colored paper cups and paper napkins of different sizes
and shapes. During the Chinese Song Dynasty (960-1279 AD) not only did the government
produce the world's first known printed paper money, or banknote, but paper money
bestowed as gifts was wrapped in special paper envelopes.
Paper spread slowly outside of China; other Asian cultures, even after seeing paper, could
not make it themselves. Instruction in the manufacturing process was required, and the
Chinese were reluctant to share their secrets. It made its true push westward in
751 AD when the Tang Dynasty was at war with the Islamic world. During a battle on the
banks of the Tarus river, a Chinese caravan was captured which happened to include
several papermakers. They were taken away to Samarkand, which was a good place to
make paper because it had an abundant supply of hemp and flax.
Samarkanders changed the technology of manufacturing paper. They began to prepare it
in stone mills and Samarkand became an important papermaking center.
The rudimentary and laborious process of papermaking was refined and bulk
manufacturing of paper began in Iran, where they invented a method to make a thicker
sheet of paper, which helped transform papermaking from an art into an important
business.
Gradually papermakers made their way further west through the Arab world - to Baghdad,
Damascus and Cairo. Finally, when the Moors from North Africa invaded Spain and
Portugal they brought the technology with them and so it was that papermaking entered
Europe in the 12th century.
In Europe, the preferred medium for the artists and literati of the time was still the smooth
and lustrous parchment. The notion of paper being used as a practical everyday item did
not occur until the 15th century when Johannes Gutenberg perfected movable type
printing, which included the use of metal molds and alloys, a special press, and oil-based
inks, allowing the mass production of printed books. The birth of the modern paper and
printing industry is commonly marked from this date.
Printing technology rapidly developed and created an ever-increasing demand for paper.
The early European papers were made from recycled cotton and linen - and a huge trade
quickly developed around the trading of old rags. It is said that the disease known as 'the
Black Death' entered England from Europe on these old rags. Yet soon this source
became insufficient and some curious attempts were made to find new materials.
Experiments with fibers such as straw, cabbage, wasp nests and finally wood, resulted in
inexpensive - and replaceable - materials for papermaking. Today, the long soft fibers of
softwoods such as spruce have become the most suitable source of pulp for the mass
production of paper.
Questions 1 - 7
Do the following statements agree with the information given in Reading Passage 1?
In boxes 1-7 on your answer sheet, write
TRUE if the statement agrees with the information
FALSE if the statement contradicts the information
NOT GIVEN if there is no information on this
1.In Ancient China, writing was occasionally done on silk.
2.Coloured paper was invented during the Tang Dynasty.
3.Papermakers from Samarkand were captured by the Chinese.
4.Papermaking as a large-scale industry originated in Iran.
5.Papermaking skills were brought to Europe via North Africa.
6.When Gutenberg invented movable type, paper materials were very expensive.
7.The end of the trade in rags was brought about by the difficulty of transporting them.
Questions 8 - 13
Complete the table below.
Choose ONE WORD ONLY from the passage for each answer.
Write your answers in boxes 8-13 on your answer sheet.
The development of paper
Place Date of introduction Materials used Method of production
Egypt about 2400 BC a plant called papyrus 8........... of the plant were
softened in water, woven
and pounded
Europe unknown parchment made from the skin was dried and
skin of animals such as 10.............
9............
China 105 AD 11............. of a mulberry ingredients were mixed
tree and hemp rags with water, pressed and
put outside in the sun
12........... 751 AD flax and hemp mills made of 13...........
used were
READING PASSAGE 2
You should spend about 20 minutes on Questions 14-26, which are based on Reading
Passage 2 on pages 7 and 8.
Questions 14 - 20
Reading Passage 2 has seven paragraphs, A-G.
Choose the correct heading for each paragraph from the list of headings below.
Write the correct number, i-x, in boxes 14-20 on your answer sheet.
List of Headings
i.Some of the problems that developed at Talbot Park
ii.Where the residents lived while the work was being completed
iii.The ethnic makeup of the new Talbot Park
iv.The unexpectedly high standard of the housing
v.Financial hardship in Talbot Park and a neighbouring community
vi.The experiences of one family living at Talbot Park today
vii.How to co-ordinate and assist the people who live at Talbot Park
viii.Raising the money to pay for the makeover
ix.A close community in the original Talbot Park development
x.Details of the style of buildings used in the makeover
14.Paragraph A
15.Paragraph B
16.Paragraph C
17.Paragraph D
18.Paragraph E
19.Paragraph F
20.Paragraph G
A The new Talbot Park is immediately eye-catching because the buildings look quite
different to other state housing* projects in Auckland. 'There is no reason why state
housing should look cheap in my view,' says architect Neil Cotton, one of the design team.
'In fact, I was anticipating a backlash by those who objected to the quality of what is
provided with government money.' The tidy brick and wood apartments and townhouses
would not look out of place in some of the city's most affluent suburbs and this is a central
theme of the Talbot Park philosophy.
B Talbot Park is a triangle of government-owned land, which in the early 1960s was
developed for state housing built around a linear garden that ran through the middle.
Initially, there was a strong sense of neighbourliness. Former residents recall how the
garden played a big part in their childhoods - a place where kids came together to play
softball, cricket and bullrush. 'We had respect for our neighbours and addressed them by
title - Mr and Mrs so-and-so,' recalls Georgie Thompson, who grew up there in the 1960s.
C Exactly what went wrong with Talbot Park is unclear. The community began to change
in the late 1970s as more immigrants moved in. The new arrivals didn't always integrate
with the community and a 'them and us' mentality developed. In the process, standards
dropped and the neighbourhood began to look shabbier. The buildings themselves
were also deteriorating and becoming run down, petty crime was on the rise and the
garden was considered unsafe. In 2002, Housing New Zealand decided the properties
needed upgrading. The question was, how to avoid repeating the mistakes of the past?
D One controversial aspect of the upgrade is that the new development has actually
made the density of housing in Talbot Park greater, putting 52 more homes on the same
site. Doing this required a fresh approach that can be summed up as
'mix and match'. The first priority was to mix up the housing by employing a variety of
plans by different architects: some of the accommodation is free-standing houses, some
semi-detached, some low level, multi-apartment blocks. By doing this, the development
avoids the uniform appearance of so many state housing projects, which residents
complain denies them any sense of individual identity. The next goal was to prevent
overspending by using efficient designs to maximise the sense of space from minimum
room sizes. There was also a no-frills, industrial approach to kitchens, bathrooms and
flooring, to optimise durability and ensure the project did not go over budget.
Architecturally, the buildings are relatively conservative: fairly plain houses standing in a
small garden.There's a slight reflection of the traditional Pacific beach house (a fale) but
it's not overplayed. 'It seems to us that low-cost housing is about getting as much amenity
as you can for the money,' says architect Michael Thompson. Another key aspect of the
'mix and match' approach is openness: one that not only lets residents see what is going
on but also lets them know they are seen. The plan ensures there are no cul-de-sacs or
properties hidden from view, that the gardens are not enclosed by trees and that most
boundary fences are see-through - a community contained but without walls.
E The population today is cosmopolitan: 50% Pacific Islanders, 20% Maori, 15%
Asian, 10% New Zealand European and the rest composed of immigrants from Russia,
Ukraine and Iran. 'It was important that the buildings were sufficiently flexible to cater for
the needs of people from a wide variety of cultural backgrounds,' explains designer James
Lundy.
F Despite the quality of the buildings, however, there should be no doubt that Talbot Park
and its surrounding suburb of Tamaki are low socio-economic areas. Of the 5,000 houses
there, 55% are state houses, 28% privately owned (compared to about 65% nationally)
and 17% private rental. The area has a high density of households with incomes in the
$5,000 to $15,000 range and very few with an income over $70,000. That's in sharp
contrast to the more affluent suburbs in Auckland.
----------------------------------------------------------
* state housing: government subsidised accommodation for people who cannot pay
market rents
Questions 21 - 23
Look at the following people (Questions 21-23) and the list of ideas below.
Match each person with the correct idea, A-F.
Write the correct letter, A-F, in boxes 21-23 on your answer sheet.
21.James Lundy
22.Graham Bodman
23.Stuart Bracey
List of Ideas
A. Good tenant management involves supervision and regulation.
B.State housing must be built at minimum expense to the public.
C.Organising social events helps tenants to live close together.
D.Mixed-race communities require adaptable and responsive designs.
E.Complaints were expected about the high standard of the development.
F.Too many rules and regulations will cause resentment from tenants.
Questions 24 - 26
Complete the summary below.
Choose ONE WORD ONLY from the passage for each answer.
Write your answers in boxes 24-26 on your answer sheet.
The 'mix and match' strategy
One aspect of the Talbot Park project that some critics are concerned about is that the
higher 24.........of accommodation would lead to the old social problems returning. To
prevent this, a team of various 25 ............ worked on the project to ensure the buildings
were not uniform. Further, they created pleasant, functional interiors that could still be built
within their 26.............Finally, the absence of walls means Talbot Park is characterised by
openness, making it easier to regulate behaviour within the community.
READING PASSAGE 3
You should spend about 20 minutes on Questions 27-40, which are based on Reading
Passage 3 on pages 10 and 11.
The Analysis of Fear
Researchers are investigating the processes in the brain that give rise to fear in animals.
The results may lead to new ways to treat human anxiety
Over the years, the majority of people acquire a range of skills for coping with frightening
situations. They will attempt to placate a vexed teacher or boss and will shout and run
when chased by a hostile stranger. But some individuals become overwhelmed in
circumstances others would consider only minimally stressful: fear of ridicule might cause
them to shake uncontrollably when called on to speak in a group, or terror of strangers
might lead them to hide at home, unable to work or shop for groceries. Why do certain
people fall prey to excessive fear?
Ned H. Kalin and Steven E. Shelton at the University of Wisconsin-Madison are addressing
this problem by identifying specific brain processes that regulate fear and its associated
behaviors. Despite the availability of non-invasive computer imaging techniques, such
information is still extremely difficult to obtain in humans. Hence, they have turned their
attention to another primate, the rhesus monkey. These animals undergo many of the
same physiological and psychological developmental stages that humans do, but in a
more compressed time span. As we gain more insight into the nature and operation of
neural circuits that modulate fear in monkeys, it should be possible to pinpoint the brain
processes that cause inordinate anxiety in people, and to devise new therapies to
counteract it Effective interventions would be particularly valuable if they were applied at
an early age, as growing evidence suggests overly fearful youngsters are at high risk of
later emotional distress.
When they began their studies two decades ago, Kalin and Shelton knew that they would
first have to find cues that elicit fear and identify behaviors that reflect different types of
anxiety. With such information in hand, they could then proceed to determine the age at
which monkeys begin to match defensive behaviors selectively to specific cues. Finally, by
determining the parts of the brain that reach maturity during the same time span, they
could gain clues to the regions that underlie the regulation of fear and fear-related
behavior.
The experiments were carried out at the University of Wisconsin-Madison. Kalin and
Shelton discerned varied behaviors by exposing monkeys between six and 12 months old
to three related situations. In the alone condition, an animal was separated from its mother
and left by itself in a cage for ten minutes. In the no-eye-contact condition, a person stood
motionless outside the cage and avoided looking at the solitary infant. In the stare
condition, a person was again present and motionless but, assuming a neutral expression,
peered directly at the animal. These positions are no more frightening than those that
primates encounter frequently in the wild, or those that human infants encounter every
time they are left at a day-care center.
In the alone condition, most monkeys became very active and emitted frequent gentle
'coo' calls made with pursed lips. More than 40 years ago it was deduced that when an
infant monkey is separated from its mother, it yearns to regain the closeness and security
provided by nearness to the parent. These responses help to draw the mother's attention.
In contrast, in the more frightening no-eye-contact situation, the monkeys reduced their
activity greatly and sometimes froze for extended periods of time. When an infant spots a
potential predator, its goal shifts from attracting the mother to becoming inconspicuous.
Inhibiting motion and freezing are common attempts to achieve this in many species. If the
infant perceives that it has been detected, its aim shifts to warding off an attack. So the
stare condition evoked a third set of responses. The monkeys made several hostile
gestures: barking (forcing air from the abdomen through the vocal cords to emit a harsh,
growl-like sound) and staring back. Sometimes the animals mixed the threatening displays
with submissive ones, such as fear grimaces, which look something like wary grins, or
grinding of teeth.
Having identified three categories of defensive behaviors, Kalin and Shelton set about
determining when infant monkeys first begin to apply them effectively. Several lines of
work had led them to surmise that the ability to make such choices emerges when an
infant is around two months old. To establish the critical period of development, they
examined four groups of infant monkeys ranging in age up to 12 weeks old. The babies
were separated from their mothers, left to acclimatize to a cage, and then exposed to the
alone, no-eye-contact and stare conditions. All sessions were videotaped for analysis.
They found that the infants in the youngest group (no more than two weeks old) engaged
in defensive behaviors.But they lacked some motor control and seemed to act randomly,
as if they had not noticed the human beings that were present. Babies in the two
intermediate-age groups had good motor control, but their actions seemed unrelated to
the test condition. Only animals in the oldest group (nine- to 12-week olds) conducted
themselves differently in each situation, and their reactions were both appropriate and
identical to those of mature monkeys. This finding meant motor control was not the prime
determinant of selective responding and that nine to 12 weeks is the critical age for the
appearance of a monkeys ability to adaptively modulate its defensive activity to meet
changing demands.
Questions 27 - 30
Choose the correct letter, A, B, C or D.
Write the correct letter in boxes 27-30 on your answer sheet.
27.In the first paragraph, the writer points out that
A. fear and stress are different feelings
B.most humans develop strategies for dealing with fear.
C.business situations cause more fear than others.
D.some people never experience fear.
28.When discussing the use of rhesus monkeys as experimental subjects, the writer
notes that
A.they react more quickly to fear than humans.
B. they are more influenced by fear than humans.
C.their mental growth resembles that of humans.
D.their brains work more slowly than those of humans.
29.Which of the following did Kalin and Shelton outline as the second stage in their
research project?
A.the identification of expressions of anxiety in monkeys
B.the identification of situations that arouse stress in monkeys
C. an analysis of brain development in monkeys
D.the study of reactions to fear in monkeys of different ages
30.In the fourth paragraph, the writer notes that the three related situations
A.reflect common experiences for infant humans and monkeys.
B.highlight the similarities between monkey and human infant care.
C.were predicted to cause monkeys more distress than human infants.
D.were graded in terms of their potential effect on young monkeys.
Questions 31 - 35
Look at the following responses of monkeys (Questions 31-35) and the list of conditions
below.
Match each response with the correct condition, A, B or C.
Write the correct letter, A, B or C, in boxes 31-35 on your answer sheet.
NB You may use any letter more than once.
31.aggressive facial expressions
32.prolonged stillness
33.a combination of contradictory signals
34.appeals for maternal protection
35.the production of soft sounds
List of Conditions
A.the alone condition
B.the no-eye-contact condition
C.the stare condition
Questions 36 - 40
Complete the summary below.
Choose NO MORE THAN THREE WORDS AND/OR A NUMBER from the passage for
each answer.
Write your answers in boxes 36-40 on your answer sheet.
Once they had identified three types of defensive behaviour, Kalin and Shelton grouped
the monkeys according to their 36............, in order to discover precisely when they were
able to respond appropriately to different fear-related cues. They videotaped their results
and found that monkeys as young as 37 ............. reacted to the cues but in a haphazard
fashion. The researchers noted that they seemed to be unaware of the 38...........
who were around them. Despite demonstrating 39 .............., the monkeys in the middle
groups failed to react in ways corresponding to the experimental situation. The oldest
group, however, reacted in the same way as 40............ and the researchers concluded
that monkeys are capable of selective responding between nine and 12 weeks old.