198x - Sowder - Mental Computation and Number Comparison
198x - Sowder - Mental Computation and Number Comparison
,, which I~ our th ird co n~trucl. Number cornparLJon can be defi ned opera lion-
~ lilly. It IS mea~urable by the ability 10 order real num be rs on the ba~is of
,I (ae, such U~ in ~c:IC:ClllIg the hHger o( two or more num ber ~, or by the abili ty
to compare different mllgnitudes, such aM ~electlnl! which o( 1'1010 nu mbe n
i, do. cr 1\1 u limo number.
Nl4l11ba H'nH~, the: hUi! con"ruct, I~ lc:,w c:a~lly 1.lc:lillcl.l bUI relerb to a well
organized conceptuill network thai ellables a per~on to relate number and
operation properties . It can be recognized in the ability to use number
magnitude, relative and absolute, to make qualitative and quantitative judg-
ments necessary (or, but not restricted to , number comparisons, to recognize
unreaM>nable results for calculations, and to use nonalgorithmic forms for
memal computation . A person who has number sense uses flex ible and
creative ways to solve problems involving numbers . The intuitive nature of
numbc:r sc;nse prevents it from being directly taught or easily measured .
The remainder of this chapter first elaborates on mental computation and
numbc:r comparison separately, and argues that devc:loping each skill will
increase numbc!f sc:nse. A diSCUSSIOn of computational c:.timation and its
developmc:nt then (0110'101'. Computational estimation i. ~hown to be a com-
plex of skills and concepts, with mental computation and number
comparison playing major roles. The conclusion of the paper includes impli-
cations for teaching and a list of topics needing further research .
MENTAL COMPUTATION
The mental computation skills advocated by some (Ralston, 1(86) as
ncecisary for numerical competency include the ability to perform whole
number operations on numbers of two or three significant digits, with mul-
tipliers ilnd divisors limited to one signtficant digit. Beyond this, estimation
is ilppropriatc . If an exact answer is necessary, use of a calculator is rec-
ommended. The discussion in this section is therefore restricted to work
with such whole numbers.
Re)e:archers who have worked in the urea of mental computation (Dick-
son, Brown, & Gibson, 1984; Hope, 19H6; Reys, 1(114) claim that the
acquisition of mental computation skills promotes the development of num-
ber understanding by encouraging the search for computational shortcuts
based on number propc!rties. Plunkett (1979) clarified this process through
his IInalysis of mental computation. He: compared methods used for wrillen
(or traditional) algorithms with those created for mental calculations. Writ-
tell algorithms are standardized, contracted (liummuri~ing several SIe:pS
involvinl! Jimibutivit)' lind auociutivity), efticien t, lIutomatic, symbolic,
ge:neral, and analytic (requiring numbers 10 be broken up and digils dealt
with separulely): they arc not easily internaliled (because th!!y do not cor-
respond (0 ways people: nalUrally thillk about numbers), and they encourage
suspende:d under~(anding.
1&4 N~, COACtpU ,ud OfH1IUJonJ til Ihe MIJJJ~ CruJu
• - •
Mcnual aJgorilhms ha~ an equa.lly interC$ling list of chilrach:risllcs, ~e:v
eral of whic h arc wo rt h men tioning here with some: accompilnying
. cxplana' ion. (1) 1bcy arc variable. Plunk..ell told of H.O childre:n using 16
difierent me thods to find 83 - 26. (2) They are fle.ubl~ and can be: adaptcd
to suit the numbers mentioned; for example, each probkm, lil - 79,
8J - 51, .nd 83 - 7 wou ld ptobably be mentally c.ilculatcd in a diffcrc:nt
way. (3) They arc act i~, allowi ng the user to choo!Oe a method, whcther
or 00& . (Note the metacogni live aspect of this ~oinl.) (4) They
are hOIisljc in that I hey dul wilh complete numberi ralher Ihan individual
di&i15. (5) Frequently they arc conilrUCtive in the !Oen!Oe Ihal they begin with
the tint number: for example . 37 + 28 is 37. 47.57.67,65. (6) They require
nding throughout. and their lise develops understanding, but they
U('nno( be used to achieve: perfoililance in advance of understanding" (p.
3). (7) 'They otten &i~ an early apptoxil'lUltion of the answer because: the
digits arc calculated first.
Some of Plunkett's examplC$ desenoc a closer analysis. How would some-
ooc nt at mental e&lcu1ation find the differences nOled in (I) and
(2)1 Ptohably. 83 - 79 tW,,1d be solved by aslcing what number added 10
79 &ilia 83. or what number a1ded to 9 gj ve5 13. This reduces to lhe basic
fKl 9 + ? - 13. or 13 - 9. Perhaps unconsciously, In - 7'J is here n!cog-
nized AI the wnc as (70 + 13) - (70 + 9). or (70 - 70) + (13 - 'J).
')bi5. might be ilOf"cd as the rule that if two numben; are less than 10 apan,
their difference Ls a basic facl .
1bc probkm 83 - Sl is lilccly to be thoughl of as (SO + 3) - (50 + 1)
or (SO - SO) + (3 - 1). where 80 - SO is an extension of the basic fact
8 - S. Finally. 83 - 7 'NOuld probably be found by counting down 7 from
83, petillpi in step' of three or four. or by thinking "13 - 7 is 6, so 76."
The lauer method depends on the knowledge thaI 83 is the same as 70 + 13.
Although these solutions outwardly appear quite differen t, they have
some rommonalities. Al least two questions are asked and answered in
obfaining e..ctI solution. C\'Cn though this questioning process might not be
•
a one:
1. How can I upre" the numberi to obtain basic fact ques tions?
2. How will the operational sequence proceed as Ii rt suh of Ihe way I
the numberi1
1b iCC bow these qucstw>ns influence !Oelectw>n of solution processes, con-
Uder lOme wayt of solving 83 - 26. from Plun kett', first poi nt. Some:
plMibilitici include:
(I) 83 + 3 - 26 - 3. or 86 - 26 ji 60 and 60 - 3 ji 57,
(2) 26 + • it 30, 30 + SO it /W, Ilnd IW + 3 i. lB . 4 + 50 + 3 I ~ 50 +
(. + 3) li 57.
(3) (70 + 13) - (20 + 6) is (70 - 20) + (13 - 6). or 57.
•
cboosc a form..t for the nllmbers l£l be calculated, mental calculation i!> a
¥tty QntiYe, In\lenti-'C act . 1be child is forced to relale concepts and 5"1115,
thuctw dew'ooinlP • cognitiYe s,"'Clure of how numbers and their opera-
tioo.5 ' . HcDC~ , t..cility with mental computation is a lrutnifeslation
01 number 5eRSe .
'There is oenainly cvKk nce (although admiuedly s~~ ) that menIal com-
putAtion and number underst anding arc c105ely related. In a c ros~-cullural
Iludy of schooled and unschooled O ioula (Ivory Coast) and Amc:ric .. n chil-
dren and adults. Ginsburg, Posocr, and Russell (1981) foun d lilat 70 percent
ollhe ll- and 14-)'Cer-old unschooled Dioula children (contra:.t<:d wi th 34
pel'cent of American children of the same age) used spontaneous regrlluping
to add mentally. For example, the solution proc~ss for 27 + 38
wu characterized as (20 + 7) + (30 + 8). then (20 + 30) + 7 T H, 50 T
1 + (3 + 5), 50 + (7 + 3) + 5, 50 + 10 + S, and finally 65 . (II should
be pointed out ~t ahbotlgh these cbildren had no fmlllal school ing. they
were from a mercantile society, and many had CIperiences in the mark.et-
piece.) In a 5econd litudy (PetillO & G inliburg. 1981), the ability of
Oioula to perforlil mental computation by taking iU.lvanlage of
lbc Itn-, hue and propc:nic:s of the whole number syilem and relationsh ips
betVlleCO operattotu wu Ihown 10 extend to all four o·perations . In contrast,
both lbc Oioula and the American subjects from the first study
reliled heavily 00 school-learned paper-and-pencil algorithms . In fact, the
O»lla, taught in French, were more 5"('«ss(ul calculating in
__ . Iben in lbcir natiYe language, indicating that school-learned tech-
niqucs are 10 IOmC degree contCIt bound.
Hope and Sherrill (1987) studied process'cs used by skillc:d and unskilled
mentAl calculalOrs in grade$ 11 and 12. 1bey fou nd that unskilled students
attempted to callY out roenULI analogues of conventional papcr-and-pencil
algorithms but that ~iUcd students "sed strategies based on number prop-
: ertica IUggClited by the numbers in the taliks. What is not clear from the
Hope and Sherrill Itudy iii how the skilled calculators developed their strat-
egica. Had these ltudenU firit developed sufficien t number sense to lead
the... to bcrome proficient roental calculators? Or did some earlier incentive
to cak'llete roentAlly Lead them to explore numbers and their properties and
tbeD to UiC lbc5e propcnies 10 bccoroe more proficient?
NUMBER
h iI .uronsini to ' " how little attention has been paid to concepts of
Dumber .ize in clemenury and middle schoollcvcl malhema l ic~ tex ts. Erron
made on talk- .~h u ~ntifyini which of two number. i~ clu~cr lu it Ihird
can be calrcmely informative to the telcher by indicating children's mis-
about the Il.Ilture of the numbers they are usi ng procedurally
ill otbu 'kill areu. Understanding a Dumber as i quantity of specific mag-
Mtnl"/ ('UIII/'Ulw/lUII und Numbtr CumpufLlUfI un
nilUde, more: than, le~s Ihun, or the same as the magnitude of another
number, tS basic to number sense . Of cour!>e, when number comparisons
Ire taught in Ii highly proceduralized manner divorced from number mean-
ings, they lo~ their potemial to develop number meaning in the child and
to provide information on conceptua l development.
The de~clopmc:nt of number magnitude: concepts has nol been widely
Iludtcd . RC::H:ilrch deilting with number magntlude is usually limited in
scope, concentralttlg on one kind of number and/or one age levd, ~uch as
the Behr, Wachsmuch, Post and Lesh (19ti4) teaching eKperiment of order
lind equivillence of fractions to fou rth graders. In un effort to gain il beller
understanding of number comparison abilities of children at diffl!rent ilge
levels and with different kinds of numbers, Sowder and Wheeler (19M6)
asked studl!nt~ ranging from grade 2 through grade 10 to complele twO types
of number comparison tasks . In the first task student~ wen: aslc.ed to choose
the larger or smilller uf two numbers, while in the second they were Ilskl!d
to identify which of two numbers WilS closer to II third number. The results
of this study and of other research studies dl!aling with number magnitude
are most efficiently addressed by considering them in terms of number Iype
(whole, decimal, fractional) rather than suiclly by age h:vel. Within each
domain, modds hu~e been advancl!d or at least suggl!sted for the processes
by which children mak.e number comparisons.
Whole Ntmlbers
Pollrock and SChwanz (19&4) have shown that for adults the ·most viable
model for comparing mullidigit whole numbl!rs with the same number of
digits is the sequential place-value model, in which corrl!sponding digits are
sequentially compared. Children also use this process (Sowder & Wheeler,
19H6). By fourth gradl!, students generally did not have difficulty comparing
number size of whole numbers of four digits and in some instances more
than iour digits . Thl!Y applied the two-step algorithm: If one number has
more digils, it is larger; if Ihey both have the same number of digits, start
at the left and work toward the right onl! digil at a time until a larger digit
is found . This seemed 10 be a self-developed process ralher than one that
had been formally laught to the sludents, and Ihey seemed to recognize that
it could be applied generally.
However, in cases where iu application was less than straightforward,
because ot the large number of digits or because more than IwO numbers
were involved, childrl!n were less successful. For example, only 64 percent
ot the fourth graders tested could tell whelher 43,724,li!9 was closer to
43,624,ld,) or 43,724 ,3H9, whcrC:lI~ percellillge~ corrc:1.:1 were much highl!r
tor 1I11~"H with ftwcr lIigili or only two nLlmbcrs . Thai sel.:ond graders had
not developed ~uch proctdurt~ could be seen by their low iuccess rute
(chance level) on comparing 400 with 368, or choosing belween 24 and 112
as closer to 87 (32 percent correct.) Russell and Ginsburg (1984) obtained
UI8 N~, COIttCIfpU AII4 O~rQ"OIU In the M"Jdl~ G,,,JC.l
F,aaions
la the Sowder and WheeLer study (1986), s"cee" al idenlifying Ihe larger
ot two fracfWlru aJ.so increased ~r grade levels: from LO percent to 19
to 37 percent to 95 percent over grades 4, 6, 8, and LO when com-
paring 516 and 5/9, for example. 1lw: extremely low 6c~res before grade 10,
area lhoulh .COCCI incrc:uc wilh Ilge, ill'licllk thltt children lind (ractional
CIODCCptI even more difficult than declrrutJ COncepti. Children do nol litem
10 attach meaning to tractio/U beyond that of a fraction III "pan o( a whole,"
&ad their oroocdures for working with fractiolU indicate thaI they prefer 10
),kllluJ CvlllPU/U/IUfI ufII,l Nurn btf c;o"'P""o un IIS~
creat the lIumera tor and the dcMrninilt(j r iI~ two ~r<l r iltc cntlflt:S rilther
Ihal1 to accept a (rac fl on all a number in It~ own ri ght. FraclJun compaflwn
i.!. rruide more difficult by the posblbility that ne ither of two numbers I~
t;,rger, even though the numbers ilppeilr to be different.
8ecau~ eq uivalent (or equal) trac tion~ are so imponilnt in computational
work, cotnpanllg fractions has received )omewhilt morl! rc)carch attentiun
than comparing dc:clmal numbc:n (Behe et aI., lYt:!4 ; Kchlakl:, IY!sO ; Peck
& Jencks, l~IH) . Foe example, Peck and Jencks (19t:!1), working with sixth
graders, found several who claimed that 2/3 alld 3/4 were I:qual becau~
"there were the same number of pie ce ~ left over" (p . 344) . Referring back
to models 10 malc.e comparison decisions can cause additional problems
bcCilU~, as they noted, children' are "mode l poor," with many having a
circular regIon as their only model of a fraction. Funhermore, understand-
ing of order and equivalence of (ractions is dominated by knowledge of
ordering whole numbers (8eht et aI., 1984) .
The comments made earlier regarding the slow devdopment of decimal
number ,ize concepts are equally true for fractional number size concepts.
1ne usual curriculum is at fault in allowing understanding of fraction num-
ber Kile to develop so slowly. The lack of such understanding is particularly
detrimental because it hinden other work with decimal and fractional num-
bers, in some ca~s leading students 10 memorize algorithmic rules as their
only recourse. i(erslak.e (1986) concluded, after teaching experiments with
13- and 14- year-aids, first teaching a mOdule 011 fraction concepts herself
and then with six volunteer teachers, that the use of a variety of instances
of equivalence incrt:ased children's ability to deal with tasks involving equiv-
-
alcncc. Instruction with children as young as those in fourth grade can
diminish whole number dominance and lead 10 improvement on ordering
fractions (Behr et aI., 1984). The important implication from both studies
is that more instructional time on establishing meanings for fractions can
be wurth while. The next step is to link these meunings to computational
procedures and to accomplish Ihis within regular classroom settings. This
ill a fruitful area {or future research .
The connection between the comparison of fractions and development of
number sense is dear. Comparing fractions is necessary for obtaining an
intuitive feel of the size of fcactiom . If II fractional number is recognized to
be! close to 1/3 or 112, for example, one has a beller feel for its magnitude.
111i& fractional number sense ial particularly important when estimating with
fractions.
COMPUTATrONAL ESTIMATION
COntiider the following problem: Jllek'~ car gCb 2~ miles to the gUllun.
The gas tank holds 14 gallons. It it is full, about how far can Jack travel
befote he has to SlOp for more glls"
190 • N~, COftCtp U and OptrlUIOJU in Ihe MIddle Grudes
•
-
One way to 50Ive thii problem ~lIOu1d be to round 29 to 30 14 10 15 and
the ' ,
.. mcn~y compute 30 X 15. CompuLaI~nal estimation, then, involves
~ IUblkiUs: apprOlimatk>n and mental computation. Skill at approximat-
ana de.pcn~s upon the ability to compa re n u mbers. Allhough the
approxlmAt1na done in the Cllampll! problem ~ems 10 be a case of simple
roundina, it is not 50 "mpk: as it hrst appears. Rounding, liS it is frequently
UU&N in Ichools, is a purely procedural skill in which "you round down if
the number ends in 1 through. and up if it ends in 5 through 9." Many
deUlentary school $ludenu would round 14 to 10 rather than to IS in this
probkm.
Inability to make judgments of relative size when approximating numbers
is detrimental to woccss at computational estimation. In a study (Thread-
•
gill-Sowder, 1984) in which middle school age children were asked to do
Of' NAEP-type computational estimation t as~s and tell how they
L __ ' _ their answers, a large number of errors could be attributed to lack
- -'
R.eY' Cl al. (11)8 2) also found Ihal good estimator) conllis tently used three
r basic proces~ s . The first. reformulation, calls for replacing Ihe original
numbers with new Ones before computing . Examples of reformulat io n
include lruncalion ("chopping off' the right -most digits). rounding. aver-
aging (noticing thai several addends are all close to one number and usi ng
Ihut number 10 compute) . lind chllnging the form for a number (for example .
(rom 112 to 0.5) . Notice that this process incorporales the approx imat ion
skill described earlier and is heavily dcpcndent on the ability to compare
numberi . The~e changes, particularly trunceation, often ceal! for an adJu st-
ment in the eSlinlale through the second process, called compensation . The
third process, translation, takes place when the estimation process brings
about a change in the structure of the problem. For example. rather than a
multiplication followed by a division. the mental computation might be
eallier if division is followed by a multiplication. This process is used 10
make the resulting mental computation easier.
Consideration of the work of children JUSt beginning to estimate also led
to a bette!r undemanding of the (actors involved in the estimation process .
Interviews (Sowder & Wheeler, 1986) with children in grades 2. 4. 6, ti, and
. 10 indicated that children in grades 2 and 4 were generally unsuccessful on
estimation tasks. By sixth grade, students exhibited a bella understanding
o( the goal of a computational estimation lask but continued to be lIery
hesitant hnd uncerlain as to how to go about reaching that goal. Not until
grade 10 did students seem to comprehend immediately the nature of a
computational task. and exhibit confidence during the solution process.
If became clear during these interviews thai lack of success on the esti-
mation task.s is not simply an inability to carry out the component processes.
The students who could not estimule did not understand the naturc of the
computational task . They did not recognize that it was appropriate to
approximLite before computing ralher than after, that the final outcome was
to be an approximation of the answer that would be obtained by straight
computation. that the needed accuracy of the approximation depended on
the problem context. or that it WIU. possible to obtain different but equally
good estimates by different but equally good processes.
In Figure 2 r have allempted to delineate and rdate the vurious compo-
nents of computational estimation .
The dependence of computational estimation on mental computation and
number comparison skills was explored in a study of the development of
computational eSlimation ability (Case & Sowder, 19H7). This study was
based 011 Case's (1985) nco-Piagetian theory of intellectual development.
The theory propo'cs thaI children's co~njtjve ~rowth proceeds throu~h two
MtUgcli durinllihe IIchool yeatli. The dimtllsiofluJ sla~e (approximulely uges
5-10) is characterized by the number of unitli (or dimensions) children locus
on at any Olle time: one, two, or multiple ullits during the three substages.
When children can focus 011 and inte~rate two or more units they puss into
Primary Skills Required SpecifIC
- Durin, Cumpul.ltiaNI FIOO
(Reya et Il.. 1982)
'ft),. AppoWMlt
wtaak 8I'C u.JCd lID en ,I
(Chan,e nwnben 10 •
~m ..
•, wbol
Callpensa t ion
Transl8 !ioo
(Olanae snucture 0[
plOblun for ease of
menial OOllputation)
Mcwethanc.Lr
rea ~ ona bl c to cons ider each ala feeding on lind strengthening the others in II
~pi ra l fa ~llI o n , with computational estimation entering the pic ture slightly
later than ea rly stages of mental computation and number comparison abil-
ities, accompa/lled by an increasing number sense .
•
CONCLUSION
The: relallonships among mental computation, number comparison, and
computational estimation can be found in their common heritage of depend-
ence on place value concepts, on the ability to work. with powers of 10 and
to use propenies of number operations, and on the understanding of the
symbol systems used to represent numbers. The development of these skills
(orces one to explore the role of the prerequisite concepts and skills and in
so doing connect them to a cognitive structure that is here called number
sense . This structure continually changes and extl!nds to incorporate new
knowledge as the practice of these skills brings about the discovery of new
relationshi ps.
At the Research Agenda Conference on Effective Mathematics Teaching,
Peterson (11J1J7) presented a paper on teaching for higher order thinking in
mathematics. (A revised version of the puper appears in a companion vol-
ume .) According to Peterson, recent research and theory suggest thr~~
processes that facilitate higher order learning in malhc:matics: "(a) II focus
on rru:aniog aDd understanding of the learning task, (b) encouragement of
studem autonomy, independence, self-direction and persistence in learning,
and (c) teaching of higher-level executive processes and strate!gies" (p. 45).
The type of Ic:arning we have been discussing in this paper is certainly higher
order learning, and the processes listed by Peterson offer some guidance in
thinking aboul (he issue of instruction. Both Peterson (1987) and Doyle
(J983) recommend indirect instruction to bring about the changes demanded
by higher order thinking. According to Doyle, i.!,\direct instru~t! .
carc:fully slructure 0 rlunities for (f ' . ..
L _-.. . 'h direct and ample experiences wjlh cootrOl.
----
This brings us to the first set of research queslions, all dealing with
imtruction . Can effective teaching methods be developed for mental com-
putation, number comparison, and computational estimation that will lead
to incn:al)ed number sense'! How should these topics be sequencl!d'/ What
etliclency level:. arc appropriate and reasonable? How can We! best monitor
student performance and growth? What effect does long-term instruction in
these topics have 011 the development of rational number concepts and
skills? And what effect does it have on calculator usage and on the use of
wrillen algorithms?
Another rCl>can:h question cOllcerns the current movcmenltO incorporate
algorilhmic thinking into the curriculum at all Itvels (Steen, 1986). Algo-
rithmic thinking is used here in the "contemporary" sense, referring to Ihe
J96
•
RE.f'E1lf NCF S
BdIJ. ... J., Wacblawlb, 1.. lUI. T. R .• It. R. (1984) . Order and equivalence o( rallonlll
-"cIa: A diKaI te"';:~;"11 JowMJ jx R.tuGtcJt ill ~JnlUia Ed.. callon.
lJ,l2l-14I.
C",-I". T.P.. O:lbun" T,G. , Rty'. R.E, ... WHwo. l ,W (1976) , Nocc. (rom NaIiOnaJ
. FA i'a I jon . ArIiJanalIic TMdwT, 2J. 296-302.
C-. I.. (ISIIS). JllJAl«mo' III. 0riaDd0. fL: MI+mic Pr". ,
C-. I.., .. J. T. (1987) . 1M lk..dopMelll of comp.... GorA' WUnelU>n : A n~o-
,........ ~ , . 10 be ,ubcnillcd lot publialioo. .
~. L. Browa. 104 , ... G1b.on. 0 , (1984) , ~ tnllIlvnuwcs: A uucnu'.
f'tlI'IM 10 reu:al1'f:IIM'dt . Fe., Sana. Fnaland: Ho". Rindw1.nd WiMlOO .
Do,tc. W (I~J) . .f:gAcmic WOI'k . RI~ilw of FduCeDNW R.tuGtcJt. 5J. 159-1\19.
~ A . (l984). ea'im"Q b N"'''racy. Fd'ICQliOllDl SludltJ JJI M.iJl/K •
..-ia, lJ, S9-71.
GIMhur,. r.,
H. ~, J. K .•• puncH. P. l. (19111) . Tbc deoclopmcnl of mcnl~1 add'llOf\
_. ~ al.,IKIOIina ud DillllfC . JowJttJJ of Crou·C..ulUili PsycJtoJogy. 12. 163·1711.
tWbcil. J . .t YIlurK. D . 1986). Plocedures OOJcr COOlCCpI$: Tbc acqui~llion of Ikcuru,1
. ID J, (Ed ,). C~ptWJ AItd p'oudlUili ICI'O...kd~,· TIv C4U of
(pp. 203-22)) , Hilltdllc. NJ : LawtCI"X Erlbeum AnoriILCS ,
11npc. J . A. (1986). Mcnlll nkul&lion: AMchroo i'm Of balic ""ill? tn H, l. Schoen &I M. J.
Zooeaa (Eda.), £uiM9liDIt ..d IfVnlp' ' (pp. 4S-S4). R.ca1Oll. VA: ruliooal Coun·
cil 01 Tea' ",i 01 Malheh"'ia.
I . , J. A .• .t ,J , 104 , (1981). Cbaractcri"ics 01 un~kil1cd and ""illed mcRlIII Clllcula·
Ir.I. JowM/ jx RaMleJa ill M.eUtt"lQljc. £4ucP'Wn. 18, 98-111.
• D. , factioN: CJtjJJrtn', 'JralCll1U IINI CmNJ . WiMIOI. BcrlUiirc. Enlll"lId :
NfER·NEl.S()\/.
Wawrr, S. 8 . (1984,. n.o oIllpilhmic m· .bemalics. MiuNmQJia ullCnu, 77. 43U-
ill,
NaUauI eou.aI alb'hm 01 toUlhcmalia. (1~) . An lJ~ndiI for ecUDII: /UcomnunJilllolU
p, IdtnftI . of 1M J980',. DC'lOII. VA: .""hor.
lY7
HelMe) Sc;cnce &.rJ C(){(ln",I ICHl ; IUpon ,,/ ItlC CoolettllC.< fi.o.1Cl of the M.It.un. IIC. 1
SaotDCa . (1~2J . TM fftlJl~M4J1CIIJ 'OC)I« CWIIC WIUJI X-l1: WNuU U"I/~fl/41 aNI
~ u ,.Of. Wulu I\~lon, DC: N.lt ion, t Sauu Fou~ 1100.
ftck: . D . M . .It knclu,~ . M. (l~tlIJ . U>ncCPhlal iUI>CI In ItiC leechJn& iinel Ic:.r wng of 1r;.'lIoo~ .
J"."IUJ frx Ruulfch In MDIMmOJIC.J F.d'lclUioJl , 11 , HIJ-J.48 .
ft~uon, P. L . (1IJH7.M~rchJ UIlChlll1 frx JlIgM,-wdtllillfl)ung In mwllufrIJwCl The cJa.dkl'lllt
jew 1111 MAl dtZ'ilcJl . Po per pre~nled ,t ~W:ilch Agenda Project Meelina 00 EltcClI~C
M.iltiCmMlica TellChullI, ColumbIa, MO .
Pchl~, A . L., '" Gin.burg, H. Po (19111) . Menllil ifllhmcllc III Afr.c" 000 Amcrx;.a : Slfilc~e"
prIOClpkl, 101\0 upl~n.tiom . Inltrnul/Ofl4/ Jou"I{~IIJ/ PJyclwlugy, 17, 11I -IU2.
fwn"", S. (InlJ) . l)ecomf>V\lIlOn IoU alllhill rot. MDlhmwui("J 11'1 StilOulJ , II , 2-S.
PoIliit . H. (191lob) . n~ 1C1IOu1,fl4llhe11\Q1IU cumcu/um . RilUl/ig nUl/unu/ t.lp~ClU/jUru. Jumnuuy
of COfl~'t"U . W.. ullnillon, DC: MliltICmaliclil Sciel\('~1 EducallOn Ua,ltd .
PoIUlICk, S. E .• & Schw.. m, D. R. (IIJ~) . Complitilive judgA:menil 01 mulildlgll numbefl .
Jry'TIwJ of £..cpmmtntul PJychol0BY : uarning. Memory and CognillOn, 10, 32-'15 .
Ralilon, A . (11J1I6) . DIU/I upu" ullM uuJc foTct on cutrleu/um /flm~wo,Ju for X-12 fftlJllu-
1NU/c1 . WllWIlnlllOll, DC: MIII~malicAI Sc:ieflU' EducllliDn BDII/d.
RtYI. R . E . (11j1j4) . Mental cumpulation and ulilTUllion : Put, prcKnlilJld fulurc . EkrrunuITY
SclutOJ Journul. 1l4, 5<17-557.
RtYI, R . E., Ryboll, J . E. Elt~I~C:Il, 8 . J., & WYIIIl, J. w. (IYII2) . Prtx:CI~~ u..cd hy ~oud
cOlllput.lioruAl c.lim~IU(l . journal frx JUJtjjfrlt in MoiIJufftlJiIC' £duaw"", IJ. 111)·201.
lWbc:nllcin, R. N . (IYHS). CompullillOfUl cllimlulon Imd rclaled tnllltIClTUlliul Ilul1l . Jo..r1WJ
p RlllaTch III Mal/~f"f\IJl/CJ £dW:U/jlHl, 16, 106-1111 .
, R. L.. & GimburK, H. P. (III~) . CoKnihV\: IIMly,il oC .:hildtc:n'~ nlllltiClTUIlLct dilli-
cWlacl. COllflilion u"J IrulruelilHl, J. 117-175. .
SKlt.ur-Gri."ard, C . & u:onard, f. (1985). Inlc:rmc:lIlille OOtlnllivl: organilat lun~ an the process
oC lcarninllil malhcmaltcal cDncc:pl: 1bc: order 0.1 posilive decinUlI numbers . Cognilion and
lfl,S/ructioll. 2. 151- 174 .
Sowder, J . T., & Whc:c:lc:r, M. M. (191Ul) . Thl dtw:Jopmenl of compultlllonul tSlflfI4IliOIl Ilnd
IUUfIMr It'Ut: ,",0 tApioralory m~ditJ . S&n Dicl!o SllIle: Uni'le:rsIlY: CRMSE Repon.
Sowder, J. T., &. Whc,l,r, M. M. (in pre") . The dt;vc:lopmenl DC .:oncc:p" IInli ,wlleglC:1 used
in COIIlp41 III Ii<lnlll c:»1 imilliu/l. JournQI for Restareh ill MulhtmJll;cS £duelllwn .
Steen. L. A . (I~). olCes ful clulngt ill. 'he K-J.2 m'dhtlTlU/f~S eurri~·ululII . Paper prepared lor
liac Conlcrcnc, on School MattICmlillCl CUrriculum: RIIUInI! Nalton,,1 ElpeclllllOm .
TIucadgiU-Sowde:r, J. (11JK4) . Compullilional cllimllli<ln procc:duru ol IiChool children . .Joomul
0/ £4f1CUliOfUJI RlJturch. n. 332-))5.
ACKNOWI.EDGEMENTS
TIac work reponed hue: WiU putililly runded by Ihe: Nlllional .ScIl:ncc Fou~alion under
Oranl OPE 1l320211H. Any oplnioOi, tinJmg_, lind cOl\Clu~iom or. rc:com~cmJllllon~ lirc lhose:
III IbG aUlbcm liod do nol nc:a;cuarily rcReel the vie:wl or the: NJilionil Sclc:m;c Foundlillon,