BBM - 978 94 009 3707 9 - 1
BBM - 978 94 009 3707 9 - 1
CHAPTER I
297
298 NOTES
6 Here we refer to the total utilities for mathematics education: textbooks, teachers'
manuals and instruction material.
In this respect we mention:
Denken en rekenen, 's Hertogenbosch 1968.
Elementair wiskundig rekenen, Assen 1970.
Ontdek het zelf, Groningen 1970.
Wiskunde voor de basisschool, Alphen aan den Rijn 1971.
Also translations of booklets from the "Nuffield Mathematics Project" and the "Invicta"
instructional material.
7 Boomsma, G.: 'Problemen rond de New Math', Mededelingenblad 4 (1968), p. 1l.
8 Around 1977 modern mathematics textbooks took up less than 10% of the total market
for mathematics including arithmetic.
9 The name for the IOWO (Institute for the Development of Mathematics Education) was
deliberately chosen by H. Freudenthal. The inclusion of the word "curriculum" before
development was rejected, since this does not adequately express what the institute stands
for. Curriculum development in the sense of document development is only a part of the
work that was tackled. The term "educational development" is much more suited to the
efforts of the institute.
Educational development can be divided into:
curriculum development;
- change support;
- research.
All of this took place at the IOWO.
For the term "educational development" see:
Hemphill, l. K.: 'Educational development', The Urban Review 4 (1969), 23-27.
Schultz, R. E.: 'The nature of educational development', Journal of Research and
Development in Education 3 (1970), 39-64.
10 This is the Dr. W. Drees school in Arnhem. For a description of the work see:
Frenay, K.: 'Bericht uit de ontwerpschool', Wiskobas-bulletin 1 (1972),269-273.
II Wiskobas: Matematika, handboek heroriiintering onderwijzers, IOWO publication,
Utrecht 1973.
12 By primary school we refer to the 6-12 age group. The curriculum for mathematics
contains a justification and description of the learning activities for everyday instruction.
These activities can be described extensively in such a curriculum or else contain a short
reference to the relevant sources (books, learning material).
13 long, R. de (ed.): Overzicht van wiskundeonderwijs op de basisschool, IOWO
curriculum development publication 2, Utrecht 1975.
14 By spin-off we mean that a number of publications have yet to be finalised, i.e., edited
for publication including the collection of responses.
These publications are:
long, R. de (ed.): Bussen en blokken (Work sheets for mathematics instruction for primary
schools), IOWO curriculum development publication 3, Utrecht 1976.
long, R. de (ed.): Inter-lokaal (Instruction material for mathematics instruction for primary
schools), IOWO-curriculum development publication 4, Utrecht 1976.
Wijdeveld, E. l.: Vierkubers (A theme for mathematics in the primary school), IOWO
curriculum development publication 5, Utrecht 1977.
long, R. de (ed.): De abacus, IOWO curriculum development publication 6, Utrecht 1977.
Heege, H. ter and Moor, E. de: Oppervlakte (Guide to a theme for the primary school),
IOWO curriculum development publication 7, Utrecht 1977.
Brink, l. van den and Wijdeveld, E. l.: De Kamping, IOWO curriculum development
publication 8, Utrecht 1978.
NOTES 299
Natura
Linnaeus once said that if an elephant were as strong as a beetle, he would be able to
move mountains. And Linnaeus was a wise man. All the things we read about ants that
can lift 52 times their own weight or snails that can pull 200 times their own weight
must be considered with some reservation.
Such performances depend on nothing but the ratio between length, area and
volume. The larger the animal, the weaker the power of his muscles in proportion to his
weight. For that reason my ten year old son can carry two of his friends on his
shoulders, while I cannot carry two colleagues. Also if snails can pull 200 times their
own weight, I cannot possibly imagine why we have not designed a cart drawn by 25
snails to carry us about, if only for the lack of noise!
A small bird needs up to 30% of its body weight in food per day, a larger bird 10%
and a chicken 4%. A field mouse eats its own weight of food per day, but the smaller
titmouse eats twice its own weight. The smaller the animal, the larger the loss of energy,
the faster the rate of metabolism, the larger the need for food.
Small animals have no time off. They are constantly looking for food, which in turn
costs energy and therefore extra food. On the other hand a crocodile can live on half a
pound of fish or meat per week. He can also fast for a long time. Man is large enough
to be thrifty with his food which leaves him time for other matters. If we were the size
of mice, there would be no culture in spite of our brains. Our entire day would be filled
up with eating, sleeping and taking care of the children.
Since I am a great admirer of your .articles I would allow myself to respond to your
remark: ''the smaller the animal, the greater the loss of energy ..." I have tried to
fathom this, but try as I may, I have not succeeded. I find it most unlikely, this law of
smaller/larger, more so after your illustrations: the chicken is large enough to eat only
4% of its weight. And a human - any hearty eater will be able to manage 4% of his
300 NOTES
weight daily - isn't he a much larger chicken, while the difference in dimensio.ns
between a small croco.dile and a big perso.n is no.t so. big that it can explain the
difference in co.nsumptio.n (croco.dile: half a po.und per week).
Mo.st intriguing is the bird that eats 30%. Has it go.t used to. this because it had little
else to. do., thus speeding up the co.nsumptio.n process? Do.es it no.t have time left to.
devo.te to. culture, ,has this been timed? Is it no.t remarkable that its bill can take such
large mo.rsels that the 30% can be managed in a sho.rt time? Or do.es it eat things that
render o.nly a little nutritio.n to. the bo.dy, so. that it will have to. eat mo.re? Or is its
metabo.lism so. fast because it has no. sto.rage place where fo.o.d can slo.wly be digested?
If it really has to. eat all day to. survive, to. retain sufficient energy, it wo.uld have to. die
at night. Or do.es it also. eat at night?
Imagine an animal as being a cube with edges o.f 2 cm. The area o.f that cube is then 6
X 2 X 2 = 24 cm 2 and the vo.lume 2 X 2 X 2 - 8 cm 3 - a ratio. o.f 3:1. If the cube is
do.ubled to. sides o.f 4 cm, the area beco.mes 6 X 4 X 4 = 96 cm 2 and the vo.lume 4 X 4
X 4 = 64 cm 3, a ratio. o.f 1.5:1.
Therefo.re, the larger the animal, the smaller his surface area, as well as the surface
area o.f his muscles, as co.mpared with co.ntents and weight. The larger the animal
beco.mes, the smaller his muscular capacity in relatio.n to. his o.wn weight. Fo.r example,
a bee can carry 24 times his o.wn weight, but the average human no. mo.re than his o.wn
weight.
The rate o.f metabo.lism alSo. varies with the surface area. Therefo.re we can say that
the smaller the animal, the mo.re lo.ss o.f energy per kg o.f bo.dy weight and the mo.re it
will have to. eat to. retain its temperature. There are a number o.f o.ther facto.rs invo.lved,
the nature o.f the metabo.lism and climate fo.r example, but in general the example o.f the
cube ho.lds true.
To. stay alive the bird will have to. eat 30% o.f his to.tal weight. That will get him
through the night since he will use up little energy while sleeping. During a harsh winter
there are many birds that do. no.t succeed in finding sufficient fo.o.d during the daytime
and hence do. no.t live thro.ugh the night. The essential difference between man and the
croco.dile is that o.ne is a mammal and the o.ther a reptile. Reptiles have a much Io.wer
rate o.f metabo.lism than birds and mammals. That is the reaso.n why croco.diles can fast
mo.re easily.
This discussio.n stimulated the idea o.f designing a theme dealing with these problems. This
is a remarkable example o.f ho.w keen the IOWO designers are to. find usable ideas.
Five phases o.f develo.pment can be distinguished in the "Gulliver" theme: a preparato.ry,
co.nstructio.n, deliberatio.n, revisio.n and fo.llo.w-up phase. During the preparato.ry phase o.ne
o.f the oesigners hit o.n the idea o.f using "Gulliver's Travels" as the scene o.f the instructio.n.
A first draft was designed during the co.nstructio.n phase, tried o.ut and revised.
Co.nstructio.n can mean taking o.ver o.thers' ideas and materials, o.r adapting them o.r
thinking o.ut relatively new ideas. In the last case a lo.ng incubatio.n perio.d (nurturing a
vague idea) is so.metimes fo.llo.wed by a sho.rt illuminative mo.ment (eureka!) and a lengthy
co.nstructio.n process. This was alSo. true fo.r Gulliver. Jo.nathan Swift's bo.o.k appeared to.
ho.ld so. many interesting Po.ints, that it was hard to. make a cho.ice.
The first draft was co.nsiderably lo.nger than the final versio.n. It alSo. included so.me
geo.metry, a number o.f physic~ experiments and an amo.unt o.f bio.lo.gical info.rmatio.n. The
geometry included assignments o.n the relatio.n between quantities in linear enlargement o.f
two. and three dimensio.nal figures and a number o.f applicatio.ns. One o.f the experiments
was to. sho.w that, under certain circumstances, the co.ntent o.f a smaller cube co.o.ls mo.re
NOTES 301
quickly than that of a larger one; another experiment showed that the bearing power of a
thread varies with its thickness, that is, with the area of its cross-section. These were
experiments referring to the relation between skin-area and volume, and muscular strength
and weight respectively. The biological information concerned subjects like food, loss of
energy, and muscular strength. Much deliberation took place during the construction of
both versions.
About""deliberation" Schwab says:
It treats both end and means and must treat them as mutually determining one another.
It must try to identify, with respect to both, what facts may be relevant. It must try to
ascertain the relevant facts in the concrete case.... It must then weigh alternatives and
their costs and consequences against one another and choose, not the right alternatives,
for there is no such thing, but the best one. (Schwab, J. J.: 'The practical: a language for
curriculum', in Levit, M. (ed.) Curriculum, Urbana 1971, p. 328).
The deliberation process lacks the straightforwardness and strictness of a logical structure,
the firmness of a proof, the surveyability of a strategy: it is sometimes compared to the
activity of a jury during a trial. As far as the last comparison is concerned, it can happen
that the design group feels itself to be condemned to carrying out an impossible task - that
of constructing a consistent package from a collection of conflicting suggestions. During the
deliberation, principles are stated (''is this not a misuse of literature and in poor taste?),
convictions are pronounced (too difficult!), experiences are resported (in class ...),
reflections of a mathematical-didactical nature are uttered (how this theme fits into the
total programme), suggestions are offered (the evaluation might be done by letting the
children make a report about the theme, about their experiences, ...), questions are asked
(what do they learn?) and answers are given (yes, but ...). Concrete product goals do not
seem to play a dominating part in the deliberation process, just as they did not in the
construction process. That is, not in the sense of 'what must the pupils be capable of at the
end of this activity'? On the other hand, it might be said that the general goals are of
dominating importance in construction and deliberation since the envisaged processes are
implicitly aimed at pursuing permanently certain general goals and at concretising them in
particular learning activities.
The revision phase should, if possible, provide a definitive version that is suited to
educational practice. For Gulliver this became possible by leaving out a number of strands
in the tale and re-arranging others. Afterward the piece of instruction is fitted into the
larger context of the mathematics programme, whose lines should converge into the theme.
Thus the question arose whether or not the line of the ratio concept connected with
Gulliver, and whether or not the line of the function concept had been correctly drawn. If
not, where should the necessary adjustments in the curriculum programme be placed? So
in the construction of a piece of instruction, the still-developing curriculum around it is
permanently at issue. Broading on the main stream, the macro-process of curriculum
development, in turn brings along its own specific problems of fundamental mathematical-
didactical analysis of a subject area. For this a broad knowledge of mathematics, its
application areas, instruction and the mathematical learning processes is required.
The chain of events as sketched for Gulliver started in the exploratory phase, was
intermixed with several other theme designs, was closed off in the integration phase by the
publication of the theme in an "overview of mathematics for the primary school", and in
the spin-off phase completed by screening it during one of the four-day conferences
mentioned previously. The response was generally positive. Some question marks were put
next to the topic: Can the teacher handle this theme? Shouldn't there be some sort of brief
introductory presentation? How can the biological information be made plausible? There
were also suggestions for applications dealing with evaporation, specific weight, buoyancy,
cooking and melting.
302 NOTES
This gives an idea of the development of a part of the curriculum programme. Left out
were the discussions with children, the experiences with the first version, most of the
responses from the development team and from the macro-development.
The sketch given might even be a distorted general picture. We described the
production of a piece of instruction which was published in a later phase while in other
cases a rough product or even the initial basic idea itself was disseminated in our early
stage, thus enabling all participants to be involved in the development.
To a large degree, development of material takes place along the lines of the phases
mentioned above. Noteworthy is the increase in organisation: first some ideas, then a
structured theme, which finally was fitted into the totality of the developing curriculum.
17 Morley, A.: 'A new development in primary school mathematics. The Dutch Wiskobas
Dienes, Z. P.: Les six etapes du processus d'apprentissage en mathematique, Paris 1970.
20 Adda, J.: 'Difficultes liees it la presentation des questions mathematiques', in
Educational Studies in Mathematics 7 (1976), p. 17.
21 Loc. cit., p. 18.
22 Serious criticism of the "New Math" came from, amongst others:
Kline, M.: Why Johnny can't add. The failure of the New Math, New York 1973.
23 O'Brien, T.: Three informal essays', Educational Studies in Mathematics 7 (1976),
p.95.
24 Loc. cit., p. 96.
25 This quotation is from an address by Hans Freudenthal, given during the awarding of
an honorary doctorate at the University of Amsterdam. See: Euclides 52 (1977), p. 337.
26 The report of the questionnaire, completed in 1969 by 80 arithmetic didacticians at
teacher training colleges, is found in an internal IOWO publication, entitled: Lochem.
Besluiten Wiskobas, internal publication, Utrecht 1971.
27 Dalin, P.: Case studies of educational innovations: Strategies for innovation in education,
Paris, 1973, p. 36.
2S Ibid.
29 A discussion about the suitable strategy was held in the Wiskobas working groups on
the basis of the article mentioned below, which was also the author's starting point for a
lecture given during the first Wiskobas conference in October 1969:
Teunissen, J. M. F.: 'De verbreding van schoolwiskunde in het basisonderwijs', in
Mededelingenblad 4 (1968), 17-30.
30 The approach followed by Wiskobas is described in a special edition of Educational
Studies in Mathematics 7, nr. 3 ("Five years IOWO. On H. Freudenthal's Retirement from
the Directorship of IOWO"): See especially, 'Curriculum development. A strategy', 351-
362.
31 Dalin, P.: Case studies of educational innovations: Strategies for innovation in education,
Paris 1973, p. 36.
32 There are many descriptions of the concept of innovation that, unlike Dalin's, place the
emphasis on "new". Chin and Downey give the following summary of these definitions:
40 Dalin, P.: Case studies of educational innovations: Strategies for innovation in education,
Paris 1973, p. 216.
41 The example-curriculum of Wiskobas for the first two grades of the elementary school
was designed by J. van den Brink of the IOWO together with the Wiskobas team and the
teachers for grades one and two of the Dr. W. Drees school in Arnhem. J. van Bruggen was
responsible for the grade three programme, H. ter Heege for grade four and L. Streefland
for grades five ahd six.
Along with the primary school group, Wiskobas has collaborators involved in in-service
training, and teacher training, as well as various other activities.
4~ We should mention that the cube image was not found until the eight starting points and
twelve general goals had been formulated and published internally. The six subject areas
completed the image of the objectives cube.
304 NOTES
CHAPTER II
Goffree, F.: 'Johan, A teacher training freshman studying mathematics and didactics',
Educational Studies in Mathematics 8 (1977), 117-153.
10 The emphasis on the concept of "meaning" is found in:
Swenson, E. J.: Making primary arithmetic meaningful to children, Washington 1961.
Thorn, R: 'Modern mathematics: Does it existT, in Howson, A. G. (ed.), Developments in
mathematical education, Cambridge 1973, pp. 194-213.
II Jong, R de (ed.): Bussen en blokken (Worksheets for mathematics education in- the
primary school), IOWO curriculum development publication 3, Utrecht 1976.
Jong, R de (ed.): Inter-lokaal (working material for mathematics education in the primary
school), IOWO curriculum development pUblication 4, Utrecht 1976.
12 If it is a context problem, for example in the form of a story, the two levels can be
distinguished. In a particular instance the pupil sees that a multiplication must be carried
out to find the solution, but this operation is done by him at a low level of schematisation,
i.e., the application level is relatively high but the operation level low.
In another case the pupil does not recognise the multiplication problem, while he is
capable of performing that operation at a high level of schematisation. In short, it is
meaningful to distinguish between the level of mathematisation and the level of operation if
the learning of the basic operations takes place according to the strategy of progressive
schematisation, as sketched in sub-section 1.6. in Chapter II.
13 For differentiation in general see:
Nijhof, W.: 'Van externe naar interne differentiatieT Pedagogische Studien 53 (1976),
391-405.
Koning, P. de: Interne dijferentiatie: doel en mogelijkheden, APS edition, Amsterdam 1973.
14 The conceptions of T. Ehrenfest-Afanassjewa and D. van Hiele-Geldhof and P. M. van
CHAPTER III
The Mathematics, Architecture and Science Society at Leyden with the device
"Mathematics is the mother of science" awarded gold to a paper in 1797 with the
motto: Knowledge of geometry is the first step toward becoming a reasonable man.
Dapperen, D. van: Vormleer, Amsterdam 1825, p. 34.
10 Cuypers, K.: Het aankweeken van het wiskundig denken, Antwerpen 1940, p. 29.
II F. (initial only): 'De wiskunde op de MMS', Euclides 14 (1938), p. 31.
12 Cuypers, K.: Het aankweeken van he! wiskundig denken, Antwerpen 1940, p. 193.
13 Turkstra, H.: Psychologisch-didaktische problemen bij het onderWijs in de wiskunde aan
de middelbare school, Groningen 1934, p. 34.
14 Reindersma, W.: Over het inleidend onderwijs in de meetkunde, Groningen - The
Hague 1926,pp.16ff.
15 See the views of Ehrenfest-Afanassjewa in: Kan het wiskunde-onderwijs tot de
opvoeding van het denkvermogen bijdragen?, Purmerend 1951.
16 These are mainly the views of Van Hiele-Geldof and Van Hiele. See:
Hiele, P. M. van: De problematiek van het inzicht, Amsterdam, pp. 88-102.
17 For the views of Kohnstamm on the learning of methods of solution, and Langeveld's
theory on knowledge domains see:
Kohnstamm, Ph.: Keur uit het didactisch werk, Groningen 1952.
Langeveld, M. J.: Inleiding tot de studie der paedagogische psychologie van de middelbare-
schoolleeftijd, Groningen 1954.
18 Sluis, A. van der: 'Computerkunde bij het AVO', Euclides 46 (1970), 81-92.
19 For the meaning of number systems see the theme "The Land of Eight" in Chapter IV.
In short, the notation for the binary system is:
:4 I ~3 I :2 I ~ I I :
'10101' in the decimal system is 21 (1 + 22 + 24)
In the binary systew one has two digits at one's disposal (0 and 1). In base 3 system there
are three (0, 1 and 2), and so on.
20 This view is found in:
Beth, E. W.: 'Doel en zin van het meetkunde onderwijs, Euclidps 14 (1939).
21 Goffree, F. and Wijdeveld, E. J.: 'Een praktikum wiskunde', Euclides 44 (1966),
193-219.
22 Much attention to the socialising aspect has been paid by Wiskivon (mathematics in
secondary education). See:
Sweers, W. (ed.): Leerplanontwikkeling onderweg I, IOWO publication, Utrecht 1977.
23 As indicated in Chapter I (see Note 12) this refers to the 6-12 age group. Kindergarten
education has been paid more attention since 1975. See articles by Jeanne de
Gooijer-Quint in the Wiskobas Bulletin.
24 See:
Proeve van een leerplan for het basisonderwijs B: Rekenen, Kohnstamminstitute, Groningen
1968,p.7.
25 Dam, P. R. L. van: Sommetjes in hokjes. Einddoelstellingen van het rekenonderwijs op
de basisschool, CITO publication, Arnhem 1975.
26 The idea is not a new one, and was popular in geometry instruction, searching for a
"definitive" pseudo-deductive treatment. NaIiles include Reindersrna, Wolda, Ehrerifest-
Afanassjewa, Van Hiele-Geldof and Van Hiele. The work of the last two especially was
theoretically based and resulted in a distinction of leanling levels. See:
Hiele, P. M. van: Begrip en inzicht, Werkboek van de wiskundedidaktiek, Purmerend 1973.
27 A very strict vertical planning according to the spiral idea is used by Dienes. See: .
Dienes, Z. P.: Les six etapes duprocessus d'apprentissage en mathematique, Paris 1970.
28 The problems will arise without any doubt, since modern arithmetic and mathematical
methods like 'Elementair Wiskundig Rekenen', 'Hoi, Rekenen', GetaJ in beeld', and 'Taltaal',
'Operatoir Rekenen', 'De Wereld in Getallen', 'Rekenwerk' and 'Rekenen en Wiskunde',
which differ from existing methods in several ways, will probably do well in the coming years.
308 NOTES
d'Ambrosio, U.: 'Overall goals and objectives for mathematical education' (mimeo), 1976;
published in the Proceedings of the Third International Congress on Mathematics
Education, 1977.
CHAPTER IV
I The following is meant to convey some insight into the possible content of a
mathematics curriculum according to Wiskobas. It is no more than a global indication.
From the given starting points and general objectives one can come to a large variety of
choices for text books, themes etc. Some of the arguments for such a choice are of
normative character - something should be so and so - and are derived from a certain
view of man, society and subject area. Other arguments are based on facts - something is
so and so - and stem from data of educational practice, didactics, mathematics and
psychology. Often the choice is made on the basis of vague personal tastes and preferences.
Take the theme "The Land of Eight": Is there an objective (in the sense of "valid for
everyone") criterion for the choice of this theme? How about for "Gulliver" or "Grains on
the Chessboard"? No coercive arguments can be brought forward for the choice of these
themes. They can be made perspicuous or even plausible through the starting points and
general objectives.
In view of this, it is not surprising that there is no consensus, either nationally or
internationally, on the desirability of a number of subjects. The activities within the subject
areas as described here are therefore no common property, although there is agreement on
a number of them. This is not the time or place to describe the procedures followed by
Wiskobas in weighing each of the parts as far as their desirability is concerned. Matters of
deduction, legitimacy and competence are not considered.
The arithmetic system
The arithmetic system is the area that consists mainly of the classical curriculum: counting,
grouping and classifying, adding, subtracting, multiplying in the field of natural numbers,
fractions and decimal numbers; becoming acquainted with the properties of these
operations and their applications.
In mathematics education, arithmetic was given a number of new impulses, especially in
the application area, leading to a more integrated and reality-bound instruction. In a more
technical sense, where the learning of arithmetic skills is concerned, certain new charac-
teristic traits can be distinguished.
We have in mind:
practic6 games for the basic operations;
systematic counting;
calculating with pocket calculators;
calculating on magnitudes, probabilities, slot machines;
open statements with relation symbols like ", <, >;
the various number aspects, counting number, measuring number, numerosity,
reckoning. number and code number, their functions and how they are "operable";
use of the number line, grid and flow chart;
connection to subject areas like mesurement, geometry, functions and probability;
calculating with negative numbers;
generalizing, detection of formulae, and the use of letters;
calculations in other number systems, for the higher grades, thus offering an
overview and review of learned algorithms in the decimal system;
properties of operations in connection with structured reckoning;
varieties of applications of the basic operations, as in counting problems and ratio.
As we have said, an attempt is made to find connections to everyday situations. The
310 NOTES
newspaper, train timetable, sports, and advertisements were proposed. Thus the arithmetic
system offers a greater opening to meaningful reality, more so than was previously the
case.
2 Measuring
In the area of measurement we have to do with magnitudes such as length, weight and time.
Magnitudes can be captured numerically: we can add them, subtract them, multiply and
divide them. Thus the area of a rectangle is found by multiplying length and width, and the
average speed of a vehicle by dividing the covered distance by the time. The resulting units
of measure are the compound units "m X m" (m 2) and "km per hour" (kmlhour). From the
original units, new units are formed by these operations, which in turn are numerically
expressed.
Working with magnitudes leads to operations with numbers. In the traditional
programme much time was devoted to the arithmetic of time, money, length, area, volume
and weight, with the stress on the algorithms of the metric system. Yet this approach to
measurement is unsatisfactory for several reasons. Firstly, working with magnitudes is
in principle independent of the numerical interpretation. We can compare, order and
combine magnitudes without introducing specific units of measure. Secondly, a large
number of mathematical activities is possible in measuring beyond the limits of routine
arithmetic:
developing measuring strategies, e.g. by indirect measuring;
developing measuring units, e.g., for population density, traffic density, etc.
the meaning of precision and imprecision in estimating, measuring, calculating,
rounding-off;
measurements in experiments and projects, making charts and graphic processing of
data;
discovery of relations between magnitudes;
detection of rules, for example the area of a circle;
the integration of mathematics and "science" in themes and projects, for example the
consequence of enlargement of an object for its circumference, area, and volume,
and the biological consequences;
the connection to counting problems, geometry, functions, probability and statistics;
Measurement can also link mathematics to reality.
3 Geometry
Until very recently, geometry was exclusive to secondary education. It was associated with
Euclid and seen as the example of a deductive system. At primary school level there were a
few remnants of elementary geometry such as the calculation of the circumference, area,
and volume of geometrical objects. However, from the nineteen-fifties onwards, the interest
in geometry in the primary school grew almost as fast as the enthusiasm for Euclidian
geometry in secondary education faded. The Arithmetic Teacher contained not one article
on geometry in the 1954-1959 period, only an average of five per year in the next five
years, then ten and in the 1968-1973 period even fifteen per year. Internationally,
however, there is little agreement on geometry programmes for primary education. This is
possibly due to the richness of the area; geometry has many aspects that can be included in
a vertically planned curriculum only with difficulty.
For example:
the form aspect, concerned with the naming, recognition and classification of geo-
metrical objects, the design of patterns, tesselations, projections and intersections;
the constructive aspect: making of grids, working with construction straws, drawing
of patterns and ground plans and constructions with the use of compasses and ruler,
construction of buildings;
the relation aspect: congruence, similarity, parallelism, spatial orientation, views of
buildings, etc;
NOTES 311
symbolising;
developing·a language of one's own;
detecting relationships between the vocabulary and syntax of ordinary language,
mathematical language and more formalised language, for example as in the use of
variables; .
translating situations into mathematical terms and vice versa: interpretating mathe-
maticallanguage into everyday terms;
the use of "and", "or", "either-or", "not", "some", "few", "always", "at least", "at
most", etc., visualising statements in which these terms occur, and vice versa: placing
statements next to illustrations;
analysing all kinds of reasoning and mental strategies on the basis of suitable
problems;
"exploration" of paradoxes.
The area of language and logic, with the exception of formal logic, has barely been touched
upon so far.
This was an overview of subject-areas, as sources of multi-dimensional objectives of
mathematics education, as pursued by Wiskobas.
7 See:
Groot, A. D. de: 'Over fundamentele ervaringen: prolegomena tot een analyse van
gesprekken met schakers', Pedagogische Studiifn 51 (1974), p. 332.
8 Popham, W. J., Eisner, E. W., Sullivan, H. J., and Tyler, Z. L.: Instructional objectives,
Chicago 1969,p. 35.
9 Loc. cit., p. 37.
10 Loc. cit., p. 33.
11 Loc. cit., p. 45.
NOTES 313
15 For the use of the terms "introduce to", "equip with", "confront with" and "unfold" see:
Oudkerk Pool, T.: 'Van zaakvak tot wereIdverkenning', Resonans 7 (1975), 183-188.
16 Popham, W. J., Eisner, E. W. et al.: Instructional objectives, Chicago 1969, p. 28.
17 Loc. cit., p. 16.
18 Loc. cit., p. 17.
19 Loc. cit., p. 26.
starting point, as Klauer does, then the problem of coverage changes to one of description,
and the question arises as to how to provide a covering description of the set of tests.
Klauer's answer to this problem is not satisfactory. In an unpublished book review on
this matter H. Freudenthal says:
From mathematics the author borrows the idea of describing a learning objective
extensionally, Le., as a set of "Aufgaben", so that a test of the objective is a random
sample from that set. The opposite, and more acceptable, view is the intentional
interpretation of an objective where the intention is conveyed by a set of examples, a
set that only extensionally gives the impression of a random sample. What, from the
extensional point of view, looks like a sample, is intentionally to be considered as a
paradigm. The author's theory concerning learning objectives looks exact, but is so only
at the cost of its operationality. The author has operationalised it only in cases where
the operationalisation is trivial. There is not the slightest evidence that the theory, in a
broader sense, can be operationalised.
If I might be allowed to dwell on this point, I wonder whether Eisner does not create a
pseudo-contradiction here. In the case of expressive objectives also, one has definite
intentions with the planned encounters, that is, one has certain products in mind. What
Eisner refers to as expressive objectives, the encounters, are in fact learning activities.
The outcomes can be manifested in the activity itself and in products. That which is
pursued can be formulated beforehand in terms of behaviour.
And:
A different matter is what Eisner means by the outcomes of "expressive objectives". This is
dealt with in the text.
Eisner, W. W.: 'Epilogue', in Popham, W. J. (ed.), Instructional Objectives, Washington
1969,p.13l.
Kieviet, F. K.: Open and gesloten curricula, Groningen 1976, pp. 1O-1l.
36 Loc. cit., p. 28.
37 Ibid.
38 Ibid.
Suppose you take three or four judges and ask them to appraise a product and then you
get some kind of consensus with respect to its value. Now that's different from
describing the characteristics of the behaviour or product that the learner is trying to
produce in the first place. I think that's the distinction we need to make. I think it is an
important one, Howard, and I don't think we have made that kind of distinction. In the
process of writing, in the process of painting, and in the process of researching, ideas
emerge which become leading ideas which then direct the course of action.
Knowing strategies, then, is not all that is required for thinking; It IS not even a
substantial part of what is needed. To be an effective problem-solver, the individual
must somehow have acquired masses of organized intellectual skills.
According to Gagne's train of thought, emphasising "processes" does not exclude the need
for product goals, but rather indicates the importance of giving a sequence of product
goals. He is especially concerned with equipping for, offering means to, offering thinking-
instruments, while Bruner also has in mind the confrontation with rich learning situations
in the same way as Eisner - learning situations that lead to process goal formulation. Thus
there is a difference between the empirical and structural trend, which in "science" is
exemplified by that between the ESS-project (Elementary Science Study) and the SAPA-
project (Science, A Process Approach). In the one case there is more room for "messing
about" and for initiative on the pupil's part while, in the other case, instruction is more
guided. This problem of processes will be further discussed in the course of this
publication. See:
Shulman, L. S.: 'Psychological controversies in the teaching of science and mathematics', in
Crosswhite, F. J. (ed.), Teaching mathematics: Psychological foundations, Worthington
1973, p. 19.
40 Examples of the mathematical thinking process can be found in:
Krutetskii, V. A.: The psychology of mathematical abilities in schoolchildren, Chicago
1976.
41 Raths, J.: 'Onderwijzen zonder specifieke leerdoelen als uitgangspunt', Onderwijs en
Opvoeding 26 (1975), 211-218.
316 NOTES
Some goals are pursued to ensure a purposeful course of learning, in other words, to
ensure an effective attainment of the product goals. So for a lesson in creative art one
might first of all playa record to induce a fitting atmosphere - this is a process goal.
In other words, for him process goals are to create the necessary conditions for learning.
See:
Block, A. de: Taxonomie van leerdoelen, Antwerp 1975, p. 128.
Another alternative interpretation is what, in our terminology, could be indicated as
product goals involving the methods of a certain subject area, as in the term ''the process of
science" or ''processes of mathematics". This kind of objective is also sometimes denoted
as a process goal. In fact one uses a characteristic of content - goals related to a methodo-
logical approach - rather than, what is common, a formal criterion. This interpretation is
closely connected to what is posed in Note 39. See:
Bell, A. W.: The learning of general mathematical strategies. A development study of process
attainments in mathematics, including the construction and investigation of a process-
oriented curriculum for the first year of secondary school. Unpublished Ph.D.
dissertation, Nottingham, 1976.
44 Becker, H., Haller, H. D., Stubenrauch, H., and Wilkending, G.: Das Curriculum. Praxis,
Wissenschaft und Politik, Miinchen 1974, p. 27.
45 Goffree, F.: 'Doorkijkspiegelingen' (Semi-transparent mirror reflections) (Fifteen
reflections on teaching learning material), Wiskobas Bulletin 3 (1974),474-495.
46 McAshan, H. H.: The goals approach to performance objectives, Philadelphia 1974, p. 2.
Klauer, K. J.: Methodik der Lehrzieldefinition und Lehrstoffanalyse, Dusseldorf 1974,
p.42.
47 Many of the authors quoted agree on this point: Klauer, McAshan, De Corte and De
Groot. See also Note 49.
48 Wood, R: 'Objectives in the teaching of mathematics', in Ashlock, R B. and Herman,
W. L. (eds.), Current research in elementary school mathematics, New York 1970, pp.
22-45.
Taxonomies that show agreement with Wood's are: "Educational Testing Service"
(USA), "Tlte Indian National Council of Educational Research· Classification" (India), the
classification of ''The Schools Mathematics Study Group" and "The International Study of
Achievements in Mathematics" (Husen, Sweden). Here is a short explanation of Wood's
classification:
Knowledge
the language of mathematics: definitions, notati,ons, concepts;
specific facts;
laws and generalizations.
So that besides being able to execute all sorts of manipulations the pupil must also have
certain techniques and algorithms at his disposal.
Comprehension:
being able to transpose data; for example the conversion of data from diagrams,
charts, graphs, into verbal form and vice versa; being able to convert geometric
concepts given in a verbal form into spatial forms;
being able to interprete data: for example the interpretation of graphs, drawing
conclusions from data, the critical examination of reasoning, the distinction between
relevant and less relevant data, the distillation of the essence from an argument or
story;
interpolation and extrapolation; for example giving predictions on the basis of
available data and the filling in of gaps in a series with a certain pattern.
Applying with insight:
application of what has been learned to other situations;
application of what has been learned to relatively new situations;
determination of relations between certain data;
handling solution methods;
drawing conclusions and evaluating.
Inventiveness:
- creative performance.
49 In this connection Franssen says:
Apparently Van Parreren sees a depth structure under what we call the surface
structures. The observable behaviour is determined by this depth structure. Learning is
not in the first place a change in the observable behaviour of performance, it is a
change in the underlying structures of action. The result is not a certain behaviour
but a potential that makes this, but also other, behaviour possible. (Franssen, H.: 'Het
eksakt formuleren van doelstellingen voor het onderwijsleerproces: mogelijkheid en
betekenis', in Creemers, B. (ed.), Bijdragen tot de onderwijskunde. Den Bosch 1973,
p.163.
50 Finch, C.: Walt Disney. From Mickey Mouse to Disneyland, Amsterdam 1975.
51 For this terminology see:
Rasche, H.: 'De functie van doelstellingen in een leerplan', Pedagogische Studien 50
(1973), p. 530.
52 See Notes 39 and 43 and also:
Philp, H.: 'Mathematical education in developing countries; some problems of teaching and
learning', in Howson, A G. (ed.), Developments in mathematical education, Cambridge
1973,pp.154-181.
53 For such a strict approach to product goals see:
Corte, E. de, and Janssens, A: Praktische leidraad voor het formuleren van leerdoelen,
Leuven 1974.
54 The inadequacy of this strict approach is most clearly evident in Klauer. See the
remarks in Note 27.
55 See for example:
Greenberg, H. J.: 'The objectives in mathematics education', The Mathematics Teacher 67
(1974),639-644.
Steiner, H. G.: 'Mathematics curriculum development in the USA A look at the past
twenty years', Zentralblatt flir Didaktik der Mathematik 8 (1976), 136-141.
56 Block, A de: Taxonomie van leerdoelen, Antwerp 1975, p. 163.
318 NOTES
57 See:
Wilson, J. W.: 'Evaluation of learning in secondary school mathematics', in Bloom, B. S.,
Hastings, J. T., and Madaus, G. F. (eds.), Handbook on formative and summative
evaluation of student learning, New York 1971, pp. 643-697.
Freudenthal, H.: 'Lernzielfindung im Mathematikunterricht', Zentralblatt fUr Piidagogik 20
(1974),719-739.
58 This source of misunderstanding is also found in the above mentioned work by Wilson:
placing certain test items under certain categories is, disregarding the actual instruction
given, very arbitrary. Sullivan has also referred to this. See:
Sullivan, H. J.: 'Objectives, evaluation, and improved learner achievement', in Popham, W.
J. (ed.), Instructional objectives, Chicago 1969, p. 94.
59 See for example:
It is clear that we are concerned here with a description of the learning process, not the
learning objectives. Of course this does not exclude that sometimes we do not know
what the actual result of certain intended objectives will be. Nor does this mean that the
learning process (subject matter, methods and media) is of no importance.... Eisner
does not make sufficient distinction between the learning objectives and the learning
process and thus comes to his highly disputable theses. (Block, A. de: Taxonomie van
leerdoelen, Antwerp 1975, p.lS7.)
62 De Groot does not use the term process goals, but what he says about the subjective
aspect of objectives, the pursuit of general objectives and the fact that the affective domain
is no separate domain, but is of a cognitive nature, fits within the terms of process and
product goals. His ideas on evaluation can therefore well be applied to the evaluation of
process and product goals. We have in mind especially his "student reporting" in the form
of "I have learned that ...". See:
Groot, A. D. de: 'Hoe stelt men eindtermen op?' Universiteit en Hogeschool 20 (1974),
213-233.
Groot, A. D. de: 'Over fundamentele ervaringen: prolegomena tot een analyse van
gesprekken met schakers', Pedagogische Studien 51 (1974), 329-349.
63 Popham, W. J., Eisner, E. W., et al.: Instructional objectives, Chicago 1969, pp. 130-
131.
64 Block, A. de: Taxonomie van leerdoelen, Antwerp 1975, p. IS 7.
CHAPTER V
I Referred to here are material aids - including written assignments and texts such as the
themes presented - which are supplied to the pupils so that they may carry out learning
activities. They can be accompanied by teachers' manuals. In the text we often use the term
"pieces of mathematics instruction".
2 Baker, E."L.: 'The technology of instructional development', in Travers, R M. W. (ed.),
Second handbook of research on teaching, Chicago 1973, p. 249.
3 The term curriculum development can refer to a wide range of planning, directed
towards instruction as a whole, school programmes, and series of lessons. Here we are
NOTES 319
In their first few weeks of discussions the Project staff made several decisions that
shaped their subsequent work. They decided to prepare some lessons immediately,
rather than, say, prepare flow charts of the whole Project's work, or write a rationale or
other planning document. Also they decided to do lesson writing outside staff meetings,
working individually or in teams of two, reserving for general discussion only questions
that arose in the outside work. This decision meant that the work would be divided into
two ongoing parts, production of lessons and support materials, and discussions, or as I
later came to call it, deliberation.
less than 10% the reaction was that the problem at issue was unrealistic. Experience has
shown that in a few cases the context story had been somewhat modified. We had expected
teachers to show more reserve towards this kind of theme.
19 Allerdings ist durchaus moglich, Guilford's dreidimensionale Intelligenzfaktoren als
Lehrziele zu betrachten, was beispielsweise auch in der piidagogisch-psychologiaschen
Kreativitiitsforschung geschieht (see also Muhle und Schell 1970).
Dabel ist dann auf vorbildliche Weise das Problem gelost, wie man vom Lehrziel
zum Lehrstoff und zu lehrzielorientierten Testaufgaben gelangen kann.
Ein diesem formal sehr iihnliches Konzept wird weiter unten vorgestellt, wobei die
Lehrziele durch eine Klasse von Testaufgaben definiert und gleichzeitig nach dem
Inhalts- und Verhaltenaspekt (evtl. auch nach dem Produkt-aspekt) bestimmt werden.
(Klauer, K J.: Methodik der Lehrzieldefinition und Lehrstoffanalyse, Dusseldorf 1972,
p.24.)
20 The four dimensions as suggested by De Corte are: subject matter dimension, content-
information dimension, product-information dimension and operation dimension. Klauer
takes De Corte's dimensions one and two together, which in our opinion is meaningful.
Corte, E. de: Onderwijsdoelstellingen. Bijdrage tot de didaxologische theorievorming en
aanzetten voor het empirisch onderzoek over onderwijsdoelen, Leuven 1973, p. 146.
21 Wheeler, D. K: Curriculum Process, London 1967, p.I13.
22 Block, A. de: Taxonomie van leerdoelen, Antwerp 1975, pp. 55ff.
23 Johnson, D. A. and Rising, E. R.: Guidelines for teaching mathematics, Belmont 1969.
Dormolen, J. van: Didactiek van de wiskunde, Utrecht 1974.
24 Pikaart, L. and Travers, K J.: 'Teaching elementary school mathematics: a simplified
Swenson's book which considers initial instruction in arithmetic from the point of view of
meaningful instruction. She says:
Meaning is experience. Children learn to know and understand what they have
experienced. ... Meaning is context. Children learn best within a rich context of
meanings.... Meaning is intent. Children's purposes and intentions are as important as
adults' purposes and intentions for them.... Meaning is organization. If the results of
learning are to be useful, they must be organized.
See:
Swensen, E. J.: Making primary arithmetic meaningful to children, Washington 1961.
Thorn, R: 'Modern mathematics: Does it exist?', in Howson, A. G. (ed.), Developments in
mathematical education, pp. 194-213.
For examples of meaningful instruction see Wiskobas publications by Van den Brink, De
Gooijer-Quint, Ter Heege, Streefiand, Wijdeveld and others in Wiskobas Bulletin; and as
regards Wiskivon (secondary education), see articles by Van Barneveld, Kindt, Kremers,
De Lange, Sweers, Leenders, Goddijn, Schoemaker, and others in both Wiskobas Bulletin
and Wiskrant.
27 From research by Inhelder, Piaget, Bruner, and others, the designer of "Freckleham"
knew that third graders, generally speaking, are capable of reproducing, completing, and
re-arranging a two-way order. It was not known, however, that children of this age are
capable of constructing a two-way order themselves. See:
Inhelder, B. and Piaget, J.: The early growth of logic in the child. Classification and
seriation, London 1964.
Bruner, J. S., Olver, R, and Greenfield, P. M.: Studies in cognitive growth, New York 1966.
Modgil, S.: Piagetian research. A handbook of recent studies, New York 1974, pp.
137-154.
28 The question of transitivity reasoning has been examined frequently in educational and
developmental psychology. The results are often conflicting. The heart of the problem lies
in the essence of reasoning by transitivity. How does it work? Suppose that for four
individuals A, B, C and D it is known that A is heavier than B, B heavier than C and D is
heavier than C; who is the heaviest; and who is the heaviest in the group A, B, C and D?
And if the relation ''is heavier than" is substituted by "is faster than" or "is older than" or
"is further ahead of" or "is longer than", will this make any difference for the reasoning? If
in the last case the lengths are drawn or represented by straws, will this change matters?
To make clear the problem we will give four illustrations, on the basis of which we will
try to answer these kinds of questions.
- A professor and his grandson are taking a walk in the snow. At an intersection they
see four bicycle tracks. First they walk over them, but later they turn back to find out
in which order the bicycles had passed. Find the original pattern.
The following drawing shows a window with four holes. Find out in which order the
damages occurred.
322 NOTES
In Greater Freckleham (for example on a ten-by-ten grid) a has more hairs than c; b
more than a and c more than d. Arrange a, b, c and d according to the number of
hairs.
A few remarks may be made on these four isomorphic order problems:
For the first problem: each track can be given a name, for example a, b, c and d, and
the relationship "a was before b" can be recorded as (~). The tracks are related as follows:
(~), (~) and (~). Afterwards they can be placed in one scheme:
By symbolising and visualising the relations in this way, the transitivity of the order relation
is locked up in the linear order.
If we consider the four problems in their mutual relationship again in the opposite
direction, we notice an even more abstract presentation of the same mathematical problem:
the last problem calls for quantitative ordering, as in "Freckleham". The last but one can be
schematised by representing the persons by marbles of various sizes, which allows solving
without reasoning. In the second case we can see the solution, but it is seeing supported by
reasoning. In the first case we must symbolise and reason, while possibly making use of
linear order.
This shows a vertical line that can be drawn from one Freckleham basic problem to
'secondary mathematics instructjon. We could have gone even further backwards. More
"primitive" than the Freckleham problem is ordering linear objects (straws, etc.) based on
activity and observation.
The question is how such problems are solved. Is this done directly via linear order or
indirectly via transitivity reasoning? Inhelder and Piaget suppose that, with the exception of
strongly "visual" cases such as sticks, it is done indirectly. Our experiences confirm these
suppositions. With the first problem even adults also usually follow the difficult route of
reasoning rather than using the linear order.
Tests that seem to prove the opposite, thus indicating that children do not reason
deductively but "see" via the linear order, are based on the relation "is longer than" and
therefore strongly related to the linear order, where transitivity is locked in, which allows
for an immediate conclusion along visual lines.
NOTES 323
Our relation examples are different, which makes the resulting problems much more
difficult: pure deductive reasoning prevails and impedes the use of linear order. The crucial
point is whether one can learn to use linear order as an aid to make the solution of more
difficult problems possible.
Experiences with "Freckleham" and its sequel have given rise to the hypothesis that
linear order does in fact offer an important support for reasoning problems on order
relations and that along these lines pupils are capable of performances that look impossible
by means of transitivity reasoning. The third graders in the Wiskobas design school initially
had great difficulty with transitivity reasoning, but once the linear order was used in
Freckleham and in the following seesaw problems, instruction was seen to progress
reasonably well.
The supposition that linear order and transitivity reasoning should be kept well apart
seems justified. Piaget does so, but other researchers do not. On the other hand we feel
that Piaget has not discerned the connection sufficiently. For instruction it is just the
relationship between linear order and transitivity reasoning that is of importance. The
crucial problem is whether linear order can be used as a thinking model. According to our
present rough information, this is the case, indeed. Compare:
Bryant, P.: Perception and understanding in young children. An experimental approach,
London 1974.
Johnson, M. L.: 'The effects of instruction on length relations, on classification, variation
and transitivity performances of first- and second-grade children, Journal for Research
in Mathematics Education 5 (1975), 115-126.
Smedslund, J.: 'Development of concrete transitivity of length in children', Child
Development 34 (1963),389-405.
Inhelder, B. and Piaget, J.: The early growth of logic in the child. Classification and
seriation, London 1964.
See also the articles by Davydov and Levinova in: Parreren, C. F. van and Nelissen, J. M.
C. (eds.): Rekenen, Groningen 1977.
Trabasso, T.: 'Representation, memory and reasoning', in Pick, A. D. (ed.), Minnesota
symposium on child psychology, Vol. 9. 1975.
29 This was established for some 50 teachers in in-service courses, who were presented
with the problem ofthe bicycle tracks in the snow (see Note 28).
30 Frederique Papy, especially, has stimulated the use of arrow-language for young
children. The fact that children must learn to use this language is also noticed in a study by
Wallrabenstein: older children are not capable of filling in arrow-diagrams on their own.
See:
Papy, F.: Graph Games, New York 1971.
Wallrabenstein, H.: 'Development and signification of a geometry test', in Educational
Studies in Mathematics 5 (1974), 81-91.
31 See also Note 18 for this chapter.
32 This became evident at conferences and in-service courses. First of all a number of
teachers and teacher trainers feared that the problem would not appeal to the children; and
secondly they had difficulty in coming to grips with the objectives. At the first in-service
course in Hilversum, it was seen that the instruction progressed poorly because of the lack
of holistic goal description. The first teacher-participants, who had to work without these
descriptions and explanations, were therefore less contented with the theme. Later, when
the theme was disseminated with a three-dimensional goal description attached, this
changed. We learned particularly from conferences that a clear goal description is
considered to be of great importance.
33 See Chapter I, Sub-sections 2.2, 2.3, 4.1, 4.2 and 4.3.
34 See for example:
Stake, R. E.: 'A theoretical statement of responsive evaluation', Studies in Educational
Evaluation 2 (1976), 19-23.
324 NOTES
CHAPTER VI
1 Brown, M.: Rules without reasons? Some evidence relating to the teaching of routine
skills to low-attainers in mathematics', International Journal of Mathematics Education in
Science and Technology 13 (1982),449-461.
Foxman, D. D. (ed.): Mathematical development, London 1980.
Hart, K. (ed.): Children's understanding of mathematics: 11-16, London 1981. .
2 A somewhat comparable approach has been sketched by Gerd Walther, although the
progressive schematisation is as it were imposed on the learner: the phases follow each
other too rapidly and little is left to the children themselves. Hutton gives the children a full
opportunity for shortening, though there is little information on the phases of the process.
Walther, G.: 'Acquiring mathematical knowledge', Mathematical Teaching, Nr. 101 (1982),
10-12.' .
NOTES 325
Thus even if the rules can be remembered they are largely without reasons and are not
related to other number knowledge. They are far from aiding the understanding of
numbers; rather they encourage a belief that mathematics is essentially arbitrary.
A person who has to do a lot of such calculations will soon develop methods of a
brevity suited to his needs. Here is a non-standard method for long multiplication:
20 3
10 200 30
8 160 24
-+
414
which shows how much diagrams can help. The important thing is a process which is
intelligible (to the user), rather than one which is standardised or quickest. Plunkett, S.:
'Decomposition and all that rot', Mathematics in SchoolS (1979), p. 3.
8 This appears from various Dutch pieces of research and from practical experiences in
experimental schools. The gain in time is about 50%, though from this number should be
subtracted the attention given to mental, that is to say, flexible and clever, arithmetic (basic
abilities). There are virtually no failures, at least if less abridged algorithms are admitted as
final, in particular in long division. Compare the general exposition in:
Dekker, A., Heege H. ter, and Treffers, A.: Cijferend vermenigvuldigen en delen volgens
Wiskobas, Utrecht 1982.
9 Resnick, L. B. and Ford, W.: The psychology of mathematics for instmction, Hillsdale
1981.
Gagne, R. M.: 'Some issues in the psychology of mathematics instruction', Journal for
Research in Mathematics Education 14 (1983),19-29.
Gal'perin, P. J.: 'Stages in the development of mental acts', in Cole, M. Maltzman, J. (eds.),
Handbook of contemporary soviet psychology, London 1969, 149-173.
326 NOTES
Gal'perin, P. J. and Talyzina, N. F.: 'Die Bildung erster geometrischer Handlungen des
Schiilers', in Gal'perin, P. J. and Leontjew (eds.), Probleme der Lerntheorie, Berlin
1972.
Davydov, V. V.: 'Ueber das Verhiiltnis zwischen den abstrakten und den konkreten
Kentnissen im Unterricht', in Lompscher, J. (ed.), Probleme der Ausbildung geistiger
Handlungen, Berlin 1972.
Davydov, V. V.: 'Logical and psychological problems of elementary mathematics as an
academic subject', in Steffe, L. P. (ed.), Soviet studies in_the psychology of learning and
teaching mathematics, Chicago 1975, pp. 109-207.
10 See Sub-section 5 in Chapter I.
II The orientation points were described by Goffree and Treffers in an internal
publication in 1970 and 1971.
12 In the guidebook for the first in-service training block, the objectives were already
formulated in terms of activities. See:
Smaling, A and Treffers, A: Stadsplan, IOWa publication, Utrecht 1971.
13 Treffers, A: 'De Klok', in Meijer, G. H. (ed.), Matematika, IOWa publication, Utrecht
1973, pp. 7-27.
Treffers, A and Wijdeveld, E.: 'Leerplanologle. Over doelstellingen van het
wiskundeonderwijs', in Wiskobas Bulletin 2 (1973), 983-991.
14 From the moment that the three-dimensional goal description was introduced, the call
for the objectives of mathematics education has been noticeably lacking, while before that
time it was an ever-recurring point of discussion.
15 Treffers, A.: De kiekkas van Wiskobas. Beschouwingen over uitgangspunten en doelstel-
lingen van het aanvangs- en vervolgonderwijs in de wiskunde, IOWa curriculum develop-
ment publication 1, Utrecht 1975.
16 See Bloom, B. S.: Human characteristics and school learning, New York 1976, p. 217.
17 Resnick, L. B. and Ford, W.: The psychology of mathematics for instruction, Hillsdale
1982.
18 Schoenfeld, A H.: 'The wild, wild, wild, wild world of problem solving: A review of
sorts', For the Learning ofMathematics 3 (1983),40-47.
19 Cobb, P. and Steffe, L. P.: 'The constructivist researcher as teacher and model builder',
Journalfor Research in Mathematics Education 14 (1983), 95-101.
Carpay, J. A. M.: 'Westeuropese benadering van onderwijsleerprocessess', in De Corte, E.
(ed.), Onderzoek van onderwijsleerprocessen, 's Gravenhage 1982,28-36.
CHAPTER VII
Hilton, P.: 'Do we still need to teach fractions?', in Zweng, M. et al. (eds.), Proceedings of
the Fourth International Congress on Mathematics Education, Boston 1983, pp. 37-
41.
6 Lakatos, I.: Proofs and refutations. The logic of mathematical discovery, Cambridge UK,
1977.
7 Freudenthal,H.: Didactical phenomenology of mathematical structures, Do'rdrecht 1983,
p.ix.
8 Freudenthal, H.: Mathematics as an educational task, Dordrecht 1973, p. 121.
9 Hiele, P. M. van: Begrip en inzicht, Purmerend 1973.
Hiele, P. M. van: Structure and insight. A theory of mathematics education, New York
1985.
Hiele-Geldof, D. van and Hiele, P. M. van: English translation of selected writings,
Brooklyn New York, 1984.
10 Hofstadter, D. R.: Godel, Escher, Bach: An eternal golden braid, New York 1979.
11 Kilpatrick, J.: 'Reflection and Recursion', Educational Studies in Mathematics 16
(1985),1-26.
12 Burger, W. F. and Shaughnessy, J. M.: 'Characterizing the van Hiele levels of develop-
ment in geometry', Journal for Research in Mathematics Education 17 (1986), 31-48.
13 Freudenthal, H.: Didactical phenomenology of mathematical structures, Dordrecht
1983, pp. 32-33.
14 Morley, A: 'A new development in primary school mathematics. The Dutch Wiskobas
23 Cooney, T. J.: 'A beginning teacher's view of problem solving', Journal for Research in
Mathematics Education 16 (1985), 324-336.
Cooney, T. J.: 'The contribution of theory to mathematics teacher education', in Steiner, H.
G. et al. (eds.), Theory of Mathematics Education, Bielefeld 1984, pp. 120-132.
24 Menninger, K.: Zahlwort und Zijfer, band II, Gottingen 1958.
25 Gagne, R M.: The conditions of learning, London 1965, p. 60.
26 Gagne, R M.: 'Some issues in the psychology of mathematics instruction', Journal for
Research in Mathematics Education 14 (1983), 7-19.
27 Dienes, Z. P.: The six stages in the process of learning mathematics, London 1973, p. 9.
28 See also:
Dienes, Z. P.: Building up mathematics, London 1960.
29 Dienes, Z. P.: The stages in the process of learning, London 1973, p. 9.
30 See for example:
Karplus, R, Pulos, S. and Stage, E. K.: Proportional reasoning of early Adolescents, in
Lesh, R and Landau, M. (eds.), Acquisition of Mathematics Concepts and Processes, New
York 1983, p. 81.
31 Bruner, J. S.: Toward a theory of instruction, Cambridge 1966.
Piaget, J.: Comments on mathematical education, in Howson, A. G. (ed.), Developments in
mathematical education, Cambridge 1973, pp. 79-88.
Piaget, J.: Science of education and the psychology of the child, London 1971.
32 Friedman, M.: 'The manipulative material strategy: The latest pied _paper?', Journal for
Research in Mathematics Education 9 (1978), 78-81.
Khoury, H. A. and Behr, M.: 'Student performance, individual differences and modes of
representation', Journal/or Research in Mathematics Education 13 (1982), 228-235.
Kieren, T. E.: 'Manipulative material in mathematics learning', Journal for Research in
Mathematics Education 2 (1971), 228-235.
33 -Behr, M. J., Lesh, R, Post, T. R and Silver, E. A.: 'Rational number concepts', in Lesh,
R. and Landau, M. (eds.), Acquisition of mathematics concepts and processes, New
York 1983, pp. 92-128.
Behr, M. J., Wachsmuth, I., Post, T. R and Lesh, R: 'Order and equivalence of rational
numbers: A clinical teaching experiment', Journal for Research in Mathematics Educa-
tion IS (1984), 323-342.
Lesh, R, Landau, M. and Hamilton, E.: 'Conceptual modes and applied mathematical
problem-solving research', in Lesh, R and Landau, M. (eds.), Acquisition of mathe-
matics concepts and processes, New York 1983, pp. 264-345.
34 Davis, R: 'A conceptual basis for problem-solving studies', in Dorfier, W. and Fisher, R
(eds.), Empirische Untersuchungen zum Lehren und Lernen von Mathematik, Wien 1985,
pp.70-71.
35 Davis, R B.: Learning mathematics. The cogn!tive science approach to mathematics
education, London 1984.
36 Davis, R B.: Learning mathematics. The cognitive science approach to mathematics
education, London 1984, p. 313.
37 Lawler, R W.: 'Extending a powerful idea', Journal of Mathematical Behavior, 3, 2
(1982),81-98.
Nason, R and Cooper, I.: A theory of mathematics education - an information processing
view (or developments towards an expert system for mathematics education), 1985,
draft. •
38 Kilpatrick, J.: 'Reflection and recursion', Educational Studies in Mathematics 16
(1985), p. 21.
39 Bauersfeld, H., Krummheuer, G. and Voigt, J.: 'Interactional theory of learning and
teaching mathematics and related micro-ethnographical studie$', in Foundation and
methodology of the discipline mathematics education (didactics of mathematics), Bielefeld
1985,pp.12-17.
NOTES 329
331
332 BIBLIOGRAPHY
(ed.), Proceedings of the Ninth International Conference for the Psychology of Mathe-
matics Education, Utrecht 1985, pp. 35-53.
Duckworth, E.: 'The having of wonderful ideas', Harvard Educational Review 42 (1972),
217-232.
Dunn, J. A: 'Tests of creativity in mathematics', International Journal of Mathematical
Education in Science and Technology 6 (1975), 327-,332. •
Edwards, E. L. et al.: 'Mathematical competencies and skills essential for erilightened
citizens', The Arithmetic Teacher 20 (1972), 601-607.
Ehrenfest-Afanassjewa, T.: Wiskunde. Didactische opstellen, Groningen 1960.
Eisenberg, T. A: 'Behaviourism: the bane of school mathematics', International Journal of
Mathematical Education in Science and Technology 6 (1975), 163-171.
Eisner, E. W.: Instructional and expressive educational objectives: Their formulation and
use in curriculum, in Popham, W. J. (ed.), Instructional ObjeCtives, Chicago 1969, pp.
1-19.
Eisner, E. W.: 'Persistent dilemma's in curriculum decision-making', in Eisner, E. W. (ed.),
Confronting curriculum reform, Boston 1971, pp. 162-174.
Ellrott, D. and Schindler, M.: 'Reform des Mathematikunterricht', Bad Heilbrunn 1975.
Engel, A: 'Teaching probilbility in intermediate grades', in Rade, L. (ed.), The teaching of
probability and statistics, New York 1970, pp. 87-151.
Engen, H. van and Gibb, E. G.: General mental func'ions associated with division, Cedar
Falls 1956.
Erlwanger, S. H.: 'Benny's conception of rules and answers in IPI-mathematics', The
Journal of Children's Mathematical Behavior 1 (1973),7-26.
Erlwanger, S. H.: 'Case studies of children's conceptions of mathematics', The Journal of
Children's Mathematical Behavior 1 (1975), 157-283.
Evyatar, A.: 'On influencing students, or why do we teach mathematics', International
Journal of Mathematical Education in Science and Technology 1S (1974), 161-167.
Fishbein, E.: 'Intuition, structure and heuristic methods in the teaching of mathematics', in
Howson, A G. (ed.), Developments in mathematical education, Cambridge 1973, pp.
222-233.
Fletcher, T. J. (ed.): Some lessons in mathematics, Cambridge 1965 2•
Foxman, D. D. (ed.): Mathematical development, London 1980.
Franssen, H.: 'Het eksakt formuleren van doelstellingen voor het onderwijsleerproces:
mogeJijkheid en betekenis', in Creemers, B. (ed.), Bijdragen tot de onderwijskunde, Den
Bosch 1973,pp.147-168.
Franssen, H. and Meyer, K.: 'Curriculumontwikkeling in de opleiding aan de Pedagogische
Acadernie', Pedagogische Studien S4 (1977), 271-279.
Frenay, K.: 'Bericht uit de ontwerpschool', Wiskobas Bulletin 1 (1972),269-273.
Freudenthal, H.: Mathematics as an educational task, Dordrecht 1973.
Freudenthal, H.: 'Soviet research on teaching algebra at the lower grades of the elementary
school', Educational Studies in Mathematics S (1974),391-412.
Freudenthal, H.: 'Lernzielfindung im Mathematikunterricht', ZeitschriJt fUr Piidagogik 20
(1974),719-739.
Freudenthal, H.: 'Pupils' achievements international compared. The lEA', Educational
Studies in mathematics 6 (1975), 127-186.
Freudenthal, H., et al.: 'Op veilig spoor', Wiskobas Bulletin-6 (1977),40-49.
Freudenthal, H.: Weeding and sowing. Preface to a science of mathematics education,
Dordrecht 1978.
Freudenthal, H.: Didactical phenomenology of mathematical structures, Dordrecht 1983.
Freudenthal-Lutter, S. J. c.: Naar de school van morgen, Alphen aan den Rijn 1968.
Frey, K.: Theorien des Curriculums, Weinheim 1972 2•
Friedman, M.: 'The manipulative material striltegy: the latest pied paper?', Journal for
Research in Mathematics Education 9 (1978), 78-81.
336 BIBLIOGRAPHY
Fullan, M.: 'Overview of the innovative process and the user', Interchange 3 (1972), 1-47.
Gagne, R M.: The conditions of learning, New York 1965.
Gagne, R M.: 'Varieties of learning and the concept of discovery', in Shulman, L. S. and
Keisler, E. R (eds.), Learning by discovery, Chicago (1966), pp. 135-151.
Gagne, R M.: 'Some issues in the psychology of mathematics instruction', Journal for
Research in Mathematics Education 14 (1983), 7-19.
Gal'perin, P. J.: 'Stages in the development of mental acts', in Cole, M. and Maltzman, J.
(eds.), Handbook of contemporary soviet psychology, London 1969, 149-173.
Gal'perin, P. J. and Talyzina, N. F.: 'Die Bildung erster geometrischer Begriffe auf der
Grundlage organisierter Handlungen den Schiller', in Gal'perin, P. J. and Leontjew
(eds.), Probleme der Lerntheorie, Berlin 1974, pp. 106-130.
Gal'perin, P. J.: 'Het onderzoek van de cognitieve ontwikkeling van het kind', Pedagogische
Studien49 (1972), 441-454.
Gelder, L. van: Grondslagen van de rekendidactiek, Groningen 1964 3 •
Gelder, L. van et al.: Moderne wiskunde en het basisonderwijs, Groningen 1968.
Gilissen, L. and Streefland, L.: 'Tabellen', Wiskobas Bulletin 6 (1977), 66-82.
Ginsburg, H. and Opper, S.: Piaget's theory of intellectual development, Englewood Cliffs
1969.
Goddijn, A.: Shadow and depth, Utrecht 1983.
Goffree, F. and Wijdeveld, E. J.: 'Een praktikum wiskunde', Euclides 44 (1969), 193-219.
Goffree, F.: 'Kijk op kans. Prpefwerk nieuwe stijl', Wiskobas Bulletin 2 (1973),907-919.
Goffree, F.: 'Doorkijkspiegelingen. Vijftien bespiegelingen bij onderwijsleerpakketten',
Wiskobas Bulletin 3 (1974),474-495.
Goffree, F.: Kijken, doen, denken en zien. Analyse van wiskundig-didaktisch werk van
studenten P. A., internal IOWO publication, Utrecht 1976.
Goffree, F. and Jansen, H. (eds.): 20 gevallen van stagebegeleiding, internal IOWO publica-
tion, Utrecht 1977.
Goffree, F.: Tellen. Analyse van wiskundig-didaktisch werk van studenten P. A., internal
IOWO publication, Utrecht 1977.
Goffree, F.: 'Johan. A teacher training freshman studying Mathematics and Didactics',
Educational Studies in Mathematics 8 (1977), 11 7-15 3.
Goffree, F.: 'De onderwijzersopleiding: een (leerplan)ontwikkelingsgebied', Pedagogische
Studien 53 (1976), 239-264.
Gooijer-Quint, J. de and Wijdeveld, E. J.: 'Kleuters en wiskunde', Wiskobas Bulletin 6
(1977),13-23.
Gravemeijer, K. and Kraemer, J. M.: Met het oog op ruimte, Tilburg 1985.
Greenberg, H. J.: 'The objectives in Mathematics Education', The Mathematics Teacher 67
(1974),639-644.
Gronlund, N. E.: Stating behavioral objectives for classroom instruction, London 1970.
Groot, A. D. de: 'Hoe stelt men eindtermen opT, Universiteit en Hogeschool20 (1974),
213-233.
Groot, A. D. de: 'Over fundamentele ervaringen: prolegomena tot een analyse van
gesprekken met schakers', Pedagogische Studren 51 (1974), 329-349.
Grossman, R: 'Open-ended Jessons bring unexpected surprises', Mathematics Teaching, nr
71 (1975), 14-16. .
Gutte, R: 'Controle in plaats van hulp', Resonans 9 (1977), 124-128.
Hacker, H.: Curriculumpianung und Lehrerrolle, Weinheim 1976 2 •
Hadamard, J.: The psychology of invention in the mathematical field, New York 1954.
Hak, H. van der: 'De doelstellingen van het wiskundeonderwijl', .Euclides 45 (1970),
289-300.
Hart, K. (ed.): Children's understanding of mathematics: 11-16, London 1981.
Havelock, R G.: The change agent's guide to innovation in education, Englewood Cliffs
1973.
BIBLIOGRAPHY 337
Kratzer, R O. and Willoughby, S. S.: 'A comparison of initially teaching division employing
the distributive and Greenwood algorithms with the aid of a manipulative material',
Journal for Research in Mathematics Education 4 (1973), 197-205.
Krutetskii, V. A: The psychology of mathematical abilities in schoolchildren, Chicago
1976.
Kuhnel, J.: Neubau des Rechenunterricht II, Leipzig: Klinkhardt 1925 5 •
Laing, R A and Meijer, R A: 'Transitional division algorithms', Arithmetic Teacher 29
(1982),10-13:
Lakatos, I.: Proofs and refutations. The logic of mathematical discovery, Cambridge 1977.
Lamon, W. E. (ed.): Learning andthe nature of mathematics, Chicago 1972.
Landsheere, V. de: 'On defining educational objectives', Evaluation in Education 1 (1977),
73-190.
Lange, J. de and Kindt, M.: 'The Hewet project - Report on an experiment leading to a
new curriculum for pre-university students', Zentralblatt for Didaktik der Mathematik
16 (1984), 74-79.
Langeveld, M. J.: Inleiding tot de studie der paedagogische psychologie van de middel-
bareschoolleeftijd, Groningen 19544.
Lawler, R W.: 'Extending a powerful idea', Journal of Mathematical Behavior 3,2 (1982),
81-98.
Lazarus, M.: A program in mathematics, USMES publication 1973.
Lenne, H.: Analyse der Mathematikdidaktik in Deutschland, Stuttgart 1969.
Lesh, R, Landau, M. and Hamilton, E.: 'Conceptual models and applied mathematical
problem-solving research', in Lesh, R and Landau, M. (eds.), Acquisition of Mathe-
matics Concepts and Processes, New York 1983, pp. 264-345.
Lesh, R: 'Conceptual analysis of mathematical ideas and problem solving processes', in
Streefland, L. (ed.), Proceedings of the Ninth International Conference for the Psychol-
ogy of Mathematics Education, Utrecht 1985, pp. 73-97.
MacDonald, B.: 'The evaluation of the humanities curriculum project: a holistic approach',
Theory into Practice 10 (1971),163-167.
Macdonald, J. B.: 'Responsible curriculum development', in Eisner, E. W. (ed.), Con-
fronting curriculum reform, Boston 1971, pp. 120-134.
MacDonald-Ross, M.: 'Behavioral objectives - A critical review', Instructional Science 2
(1973),1-52.
Mackenzie, G. N.: 'Curricular change: participants, power, and processes', in Miles, M. B.
(ed.), Innovation in education, New York 1964, pp. 399-425.
Mager, R F.: Preparing educational objectives, Palo Alto 1962.
Maslova, G. et al.: 'Improve the Teaching of Mathematics', Soviet Education 19 (1977),
91-107.
McAshan, H. H.: The goals approach to performance objectives, Philadelphia 1974.
McNelis, S. and Dunn, J. A: 'Why teach mathematics?', International Journal of
Mathematical Education in Science and Technology 8 (1977), 175-184.
Melis, P.: 'Wiskunde en de basisschool', Persoon en Gemeenschap 28 (1976), 154-160,
224-230.
Meijer, H. and Streefland, L.: 'Vijf vijftigers', Wiskobas Bulletin 6 (1977), 51-54.
Menninger, K.: Zahlwort und Zijfer, band II, GOttingen 1958.
Mies, T. et al. Tendencies and problems in the training of mathematics teachers, Bielefeld
1975.
Ministerie van Nationale Opvoeding en Nederlandse Cultuur: Vernieuwde wiskunde in de
basisschool, Brusse11976.
Modgil, S.: Piagetian research. A handbook of recent studies, New York 1974.
Moller, C.: Technik der Lernplanung. Methoden und Probleme der Lernzieierstellung,
Weinheim 1970 2 •
340 BIBLIOGRAPHY
Mommers, C.: De plaats en de betekenis van het exemplarisch onderwijs in de didactiek, 's
Hertogenbosch 1967.
Moor, E. de and Treffers, A: 'Het aanvankelijk meetkunde-onderwijs III', Euclides 50
(1974),81-99.
Moor, E. de and Brink, J. van den: 'Operatoir rekenen', Wiskobas Bulletin 6 (1977),
18-27.
Moor, E. de: Gevarieiird rekenen, IOWO curriculum development publication 2, Utrecht
1979.
Morley, A: 'A new development in primary school mathematics. The Dutch Wiskobas
project', Mathematics Teaching69 (1975), 15-18.
Mort, P. R: 'Studies in educational innovation from the Institute of Administrative
Research: an overview', in Miles, M. B. (ed.), Innovation in Education, New York 1964.
Miiller, G. and Wittman, E.: Der Mathematikunterricht in der Primarstufe, Braunschweig
1977.
Narrol, H. and Bachor, D. G.: 'An introduction to Feuerstein's approach to assessing and
developing cognitive potential, Interchange 6 (1975), 1-16.
Nason, R and Cooper, I.: A theory of mathematics education - An information processing
view (or developments towards an expert system for mathematics education), 1985,
draft.
National Advisory Committee on Mathematical Education: Overview and analysis of
school mathematics grades K-12, Washington 1975.
N. C. T. M.: Report on the Second National Assessment of the National Assessment of
Educational Progress, NAEP, Reston 1980.
Nelissen, J., et al.: Wat Tanecka niet leert zal Tanja nooit weten, Verslag van een studiereis
naar Moskou, Utrecht 1977.
Nieland, J.: 'Frontale aanval. Een leergang voor breuken', Wiskobas Bulletin 1 (1972),
324-331.
Nuthall, G. and Snook, I.: 'Contemporary models of teaching', in Travers, R M. W. (ed.),
Second handbook of research on teaching, Chicago 1973 2, pp. 47-77.
Nijhof, W.: 'Van externe naar interne differentiatie?', Pedagogische Studiiin 53 (1976),
391-405.
O'Brien, T. C.: 'Some notes on multiplication of whole numbers', Educational Studies in
Mathematics 3 (1970),63-68.
O'Brien, T. C.: 'Three informal essays', Educational Studies in Mathematics 7 (1976),
89-109.
Oort, D. W.: 'Basje, eenjonge onderzoeker', Wiskobas Bulletin, 1-4.
Ortega y Gasset, J.: De mens en de mensen, Den Haag 1958.
Otte, M.: 'Die didaktische Systeme van V. V. DavidovlD.B. Elkonin einerseits und L. V.
Zankov andererseits', Educational Studies in Mathematics 6 (1976), 475-497.
Oudkerk Pool, T.: 'Van zaakvak tot wereldverkenning, Resonans 7 (1975), 183-188.
Oudkerk Pool, T.: 'Leerdoelen, wat doe ik ermee?', Onderwijs en Opvoeding 26 (1975),
2.2J.-229.
Papert, S.: Mindstorms: Children, computers and powerful ideas, New York 1980.
Papy, F.: Graph games, New York 1971.
Papy, F.: 'Le livre pour enfants dans l'enseignement de la mathematique', Nico 18 (1974),
73-160.
Papy, G.: 'Schaakbord en zakrekenrnachine', Nico 20 (1976),67-80.
Papy, G.: MatMmatique moderne, vol.l, Bruxelles 1963.
Parlett, M. and Hamilton, D.: 'Evaluation as illuniination', in Towney, D. (ed.), Curriculum
evaluation today: Trends and implications, London 1976, pp. 84-101.
Parreren, C. F. van: Psychologie van het leren I, Zeist 1963 2•
Parreren, C. F. van: Psychologie van het leren II, Zeist 1962.
Parreren, C. F. van: Leren op school, Groningen 1965.
BIBLIOGRAPHY 341
activity: role in mathematics education 60- "Chessboard" see "Grains on the Chess-
61 board"
Adda, Josette 15 clever reckoning: column arithmetic 204-
Adler 148 206
algorithms "The Clock" 212
mathematical material 26,198-210 cognitive psychology 289-294
role in mathematics education 44 column arithmetic
applicability educational psychology 216-217
goal of mathematics education 103-104, mathematical problems 199-210
184 Wiskobas curriculum 225
mathematical rules 66-68 Wiskobas instruction theory 253
arithmetic computers: mathematics instruction 287
column arithmetic 199-210, 216-217, concrete product goals 125-126, 168-
225,253 169
Wiskobas curriculum 223-225 context: Wiskobas instruction theory 255-
Wiskobas instruction theory 253 258
arithmetical goal of mathematics education Cooney, T. J. 265
101-102,184 Cooper, I. 282
arithmetical trend of mathematics teaching counting problems
9,10,16-17 development by Wiskobas project 26
Arnold, W. R. 133 examples 33-36
attitude development through mathematics examples, analysed 37-53
107-108 "Freckleham" project 183
Wiskobas instruction theory 225,259
Baker, E. L. 121, 168-169 Courant 240
Balacheff, N. 285,294 "The Cube" 212
Bauersfeld, H. 279,282-285,287,294 curriculum development
Becker, H. 134 current objectives 83
Behr, M. J. 279,287 early trends 9-10
Biggs, Edith 10 Wiskobas project 10-14
Bishop, A 285, 294
Bloom, B. S. 149,215 Dalin, P. 19,21,24
Boomsma, G. 10 Davis, R. 148, 279, 280-282, 286, 287,
Bouman en Van Zelm 17 294
Bourbaki 293 Davydov, V. V. 210,217,290,293
Brousseau, G. 285,287,294 DeBlock,A 149,154,186
Braunfeld, P. 148 . decimals
Bruner, J. S. 63, 132, 251, 278, 282, 286, Wiskobas curriculum 226-231
294 Wiskobas instruction theory 253
De Corte, E. 84,186,293
calculators: use of pocket calculators 104, DeGroot,AD. 120
231 didactical phenomenology 246-247,257
Canada: mathematics education 240 didactising: Wiskobas instruction theory
Carpenter, T. P. 293 57-59
cartoons see "Land of Eight" Diets and Nauta 17
347
348 INDEX