Sex Differences in Parent Child Interaction: Jean Berko Gleason
Sex Differences in Parent Child Interaction: Jean Berko Gleason
Sex Differences in Parent Child Interaction: Jean Berko Gleason
Input Language
We may well ask the questions, When do little boys and girls first begin
to sound like males and females? and What role do parents have in the
development of whatever differences there are? But questions of this
sort are very recent indeed. The ontogenesis of sex differences in lan
guage has hardly been explored, and only very recently have we had
any information at all about the possible differences in the speech di
rected to girls and boys by their mothers and fathers.
One major reason for the dearth of information on what is obviously
an interesting and important topic lies in the nature of the theories that
have dominated the study of children's language development in the
years since Chomsky first published Syntactic Structures (1957). The
models of child language acquisition that dominated the field in the
1950s and 1960s were child-centered and did not consider the role of
adults, except insofar as they were thought to provide a rather degen
erate sample of language that the young language-learning child could
feed into her or his Language Acquisition Device. It was generally
assumed that differences in the language the child heard (and this lan
guage was called Input Language) did not matter, since the child's Lan
guage Acquisition Device was equipped with suitable filters for
processing out those elements that were not of use at a given time. The
search was for universals, with an emphasis on the acquisition of syntax.
The burden of acquisition lay on the child, and the role of adults in the
child's environment was minimized; it was assumed that all the child
Sex differences in parent-child interaction 191
needed to set the Language Acquisition Device in motion was to
overhear a sufficiently large sample of the target language.
In the late 1960s this picture began to change for a variety of reasons.
Among other things, a number of researchers (for instance, Gleason,
1973; Remick, 1971; Snow, 1972) began to wonder if it was really true
that young children had to learn the rules of language from listening to
a complex and degenerate corpus of adult speech. This led investigators
to study the language of mothers of young language-learning children.
The results of those studies are well known: Mothers' speech to young
children is much less complex than their speech to other adults and
appears to contain design features that may make the learning of lan
guage easier. The language of mothers to their 2-year-olds is slow,
redundant, simple, and, above all, grammatical. This special speech may
or may not make the acquisition of syntax simpler: There is still a raging
controversy in the field on this subject, with some (e.g., Moerk, 1975)
claiming that mothers' speech contains all of the elements necessary to
teach children grammatical language, and others (e.g. Gleitman, New
port, and Gleitman, 1984) claiming that what appears to be simple in
mothers' speech is not and that mothers' speech at best can have only
a superficial influence on children's acquisition of language.
No one contests, however, that mothers' speech has the particular
form that has been described by so many researchers, and it is that very
special kind of speech that is now referred to as input language. (Those
who think it is unimportant and uninteresting tend to call it "Moth
erese," but for a number of reasons that will soon become apparent,
this is a misnomer. ) Another name for input language is Child Directed
Speech (CDS), which is a bit more accurate, since the special features
of this speech are necessitated by the child who is being addressed rather
than by the person, who may or may not be a mother, who produces
it.
Once it became clear that mothers have a special way of speaking to
young children, a number of questions arose in addition to those that
center on the acquisition of syntax. These questions have to do with
stylistic (or registral) variation: Input language, or CDS, is clearly a
separate style, or register. It appears in the speech of women who are
not mothers, in the speech of fathers, and, indeed, in the speech of all
speakers, child and adult, who are addressing young children. Shatz and
Gelman (1973) showed that even 4-year-olds make some modifications
in their usual speech when they speak to 2-year-olds. Other researchers
(Giattino and Hogan, 1975; Golinkoff and Ames, 1979) showed that
fathers' speech also contained the simplifying and clarifying modifica
tions that had been noted in mothers' speech. Thus, input language
(CDS) containing some special features is produced by all speakers
192 JEAN BERKO GLEASON
addressing young children. Bohannon and Marquis (1977) suggest that
it is children themselves who cause these modifications, because it can
be demonstrated that speakers adjust the complexity of their utterances
in accordance with the signals of comprehension or noncom prehension
produced by their addressees. While this can be shown experimentally,
it is also true that speakers have preconceived notions of how to talk
to young children: Adults simplify and clarify their speech when they
only think they are talking on the phone to 2-year-olds (Snow, 1972),
and young children in preschool produce typical "baby talk" when play
ing with dolls (Sachs and Devin, 1976; Andersen, 1977). Some of the
features of CDS are undoubtedly tied to communication pressure (for
instance, clear enunciation), but others are part of a conventionalized
speech register (for instance, calling a rabbit a "bunny"). Young children
acquire this register as part of their developing communicative com
petence, and adults use this register in speaking to young children.
Input language is not a unitary phenomenon, however. It changes
over time and becomes more complex as children's ability to compre
hend it changes. By the time children are 4 or 5, adults speak to them
in a "language of socialization" that emphasizes not so much syntactic
clarity, or the rules of language, as the rules of society. Speech to a 2
year-old contains many phrases like "See the bunny. It's a nice bunny.
Pat the bunny," while speech to a 5-year-old contains many phrases like
"Look both ways before you cross the street," "Say thank you to Mrs.
Williams," and "Sit up at the table. "
CDS thus occurs in different forms, depending on the age of the child
being addressed. There may be some argument about the relation be
tween the syntax used by adults in their CDS and the acquisition of
syntax by children, but there is general agreement that adults explicitly
teach children social conventions and that the adult language is the
medium of that education.
What remains to be determined is whether the sex of the child as well
as the age of the child has an effect on the CDS, and, additionally,
whether CDS varies according to the sex of the speaker. Unless girls
and boys are exposed to different adult models or are spoken to dif
ferently, we are hard pressed to provide an environmental explanation
for how sex differences in their own language might possibly originate.
In the rest of this chapter, a number of studies originating in our own
laboratory will be discussed. The questions to be considered involve:
(1) differences between mothers' and fathers' speech to children, re
gardless of the child's sex, (2) differences in parents' speech to boys and
to girls, and (3)' emerging sex-associated differences in the speech of
children.
Sex differences in parent-child interaction 193
The research was carried out in both naturalistic and laboratory settings.
Initially, we obtained a small sample of families whom we visited in
their homes, making audiorecordings of family interaction. At the same
time, for comparison, we made recordings of male and female teachers
in a day-care setting (Gleason, 1975). We then obtained funding to
conduct a laboratory and home study of a much larger sample of families.
Twenty-four families participated in this study. Each family had a child
between the ages of 2 and 5; the mothers were the primary caretakers,
and the fathers worked outside the home in professional occupations.
Twelve of the child subjects were girls and twelve were boys, about
evenly matched for age.
Methods
In the laboratory portion of the study, each child was seen and video
taped twice, once with the father and once with the mother, in a coun
terbalanced design. Recording sessions lasted a half hour, which was
divided among three activities: "reading" a picture book that had no
words (Mercer-Mayer's The Great Cat Chase); taking apart (and at
tempting to put back together) a toy Playskool car; and playing store
with a number of grocery items, paper bags, and a toy cash register.
Toward the end of the session a research assistant entered the laboratory
playroom with a gift for the child. This assistant followed a script, de
signed to maximize the likelihood that the parent and child would say,
"Hi," "Thanks," and "Goodbye" (see our article of that name: Greif
and Gleason, 1980). This was accomplished by, for instance, holding
out the gift; saying, "Here's a little gift just for you"; and then waiting
expectantly. Obviously, the pressure on a parent under the circumstan
ces is to tell the child to say "Thanks" or personally to say "Thanks. "
In this way we were able to look at sex differences in politeness behavior
in fathers, mothers, girls, and boys.
The laboratory videotapes were transcribed and analyzed in all of the
standard ways (e.g., for mean length of utterance and sentence type),
as well as for features thought to be differentially represented in the
speech of females and males. We looked for tag questions, for instance,
as in "It's hot in here, isn't it?," a construction often claimed to be used
more by women than by men.
The home and day-care studies relied only on audiotapes, since we
194 JEAN BERKO GLEASON
felt that taking a videocamera into subjects' dining rooms was too in
trusive; the same held true in the day-care center. Since these studies
have been reported in detail elsewhere, the major findings, along with
their implications for the study of sex differences in language, will be
reported here rather than the means and standard deviations associated
with their statistical analyses. The interested reader is referred to Bel
linger and Gleason, 1982; Gleason, 1973, 1975, 1980; Gleason and Greif,
1983; Gleason and Weintraub, 1976; and Masur and Gleason, 1980.
These report on both the laboratory and home studies. It should be
added here that the twenty-four families who participated in the labo
ratory study were also seen at home: A recording of a family dinner
where both parents and the child were present was made in each family.
Our current work centers on these dinner transcripts (Gleason, Peri
mann, and Greif, 1984).
Home studies
Our first study was of several families at home. Like other researchers,
we found that there were very few substantive differences in the speech
of the mothers and fathers; but there were some notable exceptions. It
should be noted here that in this first home study a male research
assistant participated and remained with the family while the recording
was made. This may have led to some exaggerated "macho" behavior
m the part of the fathers. In our later home study, where we recorded
Laboratory studies
As this paper has tried to indicate, very little work has been done on
the emergence of sex differences in the language of children. Yet we
know that since men and women speak differently, those differences ·
must begin to emerge at some point in time. The research cited here
has described differences in parents' speech to children in the use of
jocular names, threats, directives, complex vocabulary, politeness, and
interruptions. For directives, we were able 'to show that preschoolers
were already stylistically similar to their same-sex parent. Some re
searchers (e. g. , Lakoff, 1973a) have suggested that all children speak
"women's language" until the age of 5 or 6, but this is probably a
reflection less of the facts than of our lack of sophisticated methods of
analysis and gob d hypotheses about what to look for when seeking
differences: Difef rences can be found in the speech of even very young
children when we have precise features to investigate. This observation
is provocative: Developmental psychologists representing various the
oretical schools (Freudian, cognitive, social learning) have suggested
that children do not have a firm sex-role identity until about the age of
5. Others (e.g., Money and Erhardt, 1972) have pointed out, however,
that after the age of 18 months it is very difficult to reannounce the sex
of a child if there has been an initial misidentification; that is, a child
who was thought to be of one sex cannot after this point easily make
the transition to the other sex, even when the chromosomes say it must .
be so. Perhaps one of the reasons for this is that the child has already
Sex differences in parent-child interaction 199
begun to absorb sex-role related behaviors at some level. These may
include specific linguistic features.
This chapter has attempted to suggest some areas for further research.
More careful research on the language of parents and other adults is
certainly in order, but it is also time to turn our attention to the emerging
language of children in order to find the earliest evidences of linguistic
sexual dimorphism.
In doing this research it will be important to examine children's lan
guage in a variety of contexts that allow us to separate out age, sex
role, and status considerations. Since, for instance, status factors militate
against children's using imperatives with their parents and with older
people, one area to look for these differences is in peer language. The
speech children use when talking with one another is, of course, a
separate register itself, and one that has hardly been studied. Some of
the features of peer language must surely be learned from other children
or other models: Male parents rarely make noises like dive-bombers or
machine guns; yet these sound effects are common in the speech of
young boys and not in that of young girls. By the same token, male and
female teenagers undoubtedly use different features in their speech.
Since communicative competence requires appropriate linguistic use
even before adulthood, all of these populations are worth studying in
order to understand the nature of sex differences in language. Transitory
phenomena should be noted, along with those enduring features that
ultimately mark as distinct the language of grown women and men.