[go: up one dir, main page]

Academia.eduAcademia.edu
1 Avoiding and Withdrawing from the Peer Group Kenneth H. Rubin1, Julie C. Bowker2, & Amy E. Kennedy1 1 University of Maryland 2 University at Buffalo, The State University of New York Kenneth H. Rubin, Ph.D. Department of Human Development University of Maryland 3304 Benjamin Building College Park, Maryland 20742-1131 Phone: (301) 405-0458 Fax: (301) 405-7735 2 Avoiding and Withdrawing from the Peer Group In many ways this chapter concerns a topic unlike most that appear in this Handbook. Rather than focusing on the ways in which children and adolescents may interact with their peers, this chapter is centered on those children who, for whatever reason, engage rarely in peer interaction. As noted throughout this Handbook, children who are socially engaging and competent interact with peers in ways that allow the establishment and maintenance of positive relationships. Such children fare well in their social and academic lives. Alternatively, their socially unskilled counterparts often suffer from peer rejection, friendlessness, and loneliness; furthermore, they are thought to be at risk for a wide range of socioemotional and academic difficulties. In this chapter, we focus on children who avoid and rarely interact with their peers and who suffer deeply for their withdrawal. HISTORICAL BACKGROUND For at least two decades, researchers have argued that children who do not have adequate or “typical” peer interactions and peer relationship experiences may be at risk for later maladjustment. Such a conclusion has been reinforced by studies demonstrating that peer rejection in childhood predicts psychopathology and school drop-out, among other negative consequences in adolescence and adulthood (see Rubin, Bukowski, & Parker, 2006 for a relevant review). From a purely clinical perspective, available evidence suggests that poor peer relationships are common reasons for children’s referrals to child specialists. For example, Achenbach and Edelbrock (1981) reported that 30-to-75 percent of children (depending on age) referred to guidance clinics were reported by their mothers to experience peer difficulties (e.g., poor social skills, aggression). And peer difficulties are roughly twice as common among clinic children as among non-referred children. The group of children generally considered at highest risk for later psychiatric difficulty comprises mainly those whose interactions may best be described as hostile and aggressive. Aggressive children are often portrayed as emotionally dysregulated, atypical in the ways they think 3 about social interactions and events, inaccurate in the ways they think about themselves and their social relationships, disruptive at home and school, and behaviorally agonistic in an often bullying manner (see Dodge, Coie, & Lynam, 2006 for a recent review). As it happens, one of the strongest correlates and predictors of peer rejection is aggressive behavior. Moreover, the emotional, cognitive, and self-system “baggage” that accompanies aggressive behaviors also predicts peer rejection. Thus, there appears to be a clear link between the frequency with which children display agonistic behavior and the establishment and maintenance of a negative peer reputation (Rubin, Bukowski, et al., 2006). Despite their agonistic inclinations, however, aggressive children have friends and are often members of cliques and peer networks. Their friends tend to resemble them behaviorally and the networks they are involved in likewise comprise angry, aggressive peers (see Dishion, this Handbook). In these latter regards, aggressive behavior may be maintained and reinforced through the process of active deviancy training (Dishion, McCord, & Poulin, 1999). For many years, it was posited that the straightest route to peer rejection and other related peer difficulties began with aggressive behavior (e.g., refer to special issues/sections of Child Development and the Merrill-Palmer Quarterly in 1983!). More recently, however, a strong case has been made for the significance of social withdrawal in predicting negative peer concomitants and outcomes. This literature is reviewed herein. CENTRAL ISSUES: CONSTRUCT DEFINITION The construct of social withdrawal has many “faces” or forms and subtypes (Rubin & Mills, 1988), and over the years, such terms of reference as social withdrawal, social isolation, shyness (fearful and selfconscious), and behavioral inhibition have been used interchangeably. Whereas these constructs may be related conceptually and statistically, they are not equivalent. As we have indicated elsewhere (e.g., Rubin & Asendorpf, 1993; Rubin & Coplan, 2004; Rubin, Burgess, & Coplan, 2002) inhibition may be defined as the disposition to display fearful, anxious behaviors in unfamiliar contexts. Fearful shyness 4 represents inhibited behavior in the face of social novelty. Self-conscious shyness, which is thought to emerge between the ages of 4-5, refers to the display of socially wary behavior evoked by intrapersonal concerns of being negatively evaluated by others. Finally, social withdrawal refers to the consistent (across situations and over time) display of solitary behavior when encountering familiar and/or unfamiliar peers. The common thread binding each of these constructs is that the thought or presence of social company evokes reactions of fear or anxiety which result in attempts to cope or self-regulate through socially restrained or avoidant behavior. Indeed, anxious-solitude is a recent term that denotes social withdrawal resulting from fear or anxiety (e.g., Gazelle & Ladd, 2003). Importantly, not all forms of solitary behavior are elicited by fear or anxiety. For example, the experience of social rejection can result in being isolated by and removed from the group. In this regard, aggressive children may evidence solitude in groups that reject them (e.g., Hymel, Rubin, Rowden, & LeMare, 1990). Other children appear to prefer solitude and lack the desire to engage others in social interaction. These children have been described as “unsociable” (Asendorpf, 1993) or “socially disinterested” (Coplan, Prakash, O’Neil, & Armer, 2004). They appear to possess a low motivation to approach others as well as a low motivation to avoid social interaction (Asendorpf, 1993). In early childhood, these children tend to engage in solitary play marked by the quiescent, often sedentary exploration and construction of objects; at the same time, for many young children, this type of quiet, constructive solitude is not associated with either peer rejection or psychological maladaptation (Rubin, Coplan, Fox, & Calkins, 1995). Beyond the preschool years, however, unsociable children may experience increased peer and psychological difficulties when developmental norms move in the direction of increased social interactive behavior and their behavior becomes viewed as socially inappropriate (Asendorpf, 1993; Rubin & Coplan, 2004). Solitude that results from social disinterest has been contrasted with the display of anxious, reticent behavior, which appears to reflect an approach-avoidance conflict; reticent children watch 5 others from afar and, at the same time, they appear to desire the companionship of others (e.g., Asendorpf, 1993; Coplan et al., 2004). Unlike solitary exploration and construction (indicators of social disinterest), reticence both reflects and predicts peer rejection and psychosocial maladjustment (e.g., Rubin, Chen, McDougall, Bowker, & McKinnon, 1995). Significantly, little attention has been paid to different forms of social withdrawal beyond the early childhood years, an issue to which we return in the final section of this chapter. In light of findings documenting significant, negative psychosocial correlates and consequences of social withdrawal (see Rubin, Burgess, Kennedy, & Stewart, 2003 for a review), it seems particularly important to not only understand its concomitants and consequences, but also its origins. Thus, in this chapter, we begin with a brief review of relevant theory followed by a description of factors that predict solitary behavior in the peer group. We follow with descriptions of the peer relationships and friendships of socially withdrawn children and young adolescents, and the social and emotional consequences of withdrawal. We conclude with a discussion of future directions. RELEVANT THEORY Theoretically derived statements about the etiology and psychological significance of social withdrawal were practically non-existent until the 1980s (e.g., Rubin, 1982a). Those searching for relevant theory had to rely on classical writings pertaining to the significance of peer interaction for normal social, emotional, and cognitive growth. For example, in the early writings of Piaget (1932), it was posited that children's relationships with peers, unlike their relationships with adults, were relatively balanced, egalitarian, and fell along a more-or-less horizontal plane of power assertion and dominance. It was within this egalitarian context that Piaget believed peer interaction provided a unique cognitive and social-cognitive growth context for children. Piaget focused specifically on the relevance of disagreements with age-mates and the opportunities for negotiation arising from 6 disagreements. These naturally occurring differences of opinion were assumed to engender cognitive conflict that required both intra- and interpersonal resolution in order for positive peer exchanges and experiences to occur. The resolution of interpersonal disputes was thought to result in an enhanced understanding of others’ thoughts and emotions, the broadening of one’s social repertoire with which to solve interpersonal disputes and misunderstandings, and the comprehension of causeeffect relations in social interaction. Empirical support for these notions derived from demonstrations that peer exchange, conversations, and interactions produced growth in social-cognitive development and social competence (e.g., Damon & Killen, 1982). Also, in keeping with these theoretically driven findings, researchers found that perspective-taking skills could be improved through peer interactions, particularly those experiences that involved role-play. In turn, such improvements predicted improvements in social competence (e.g., Selman & Schultz, 1990). Peer interaction also allows children to understand the rules and norms of their peer subcultures (e.g., Fine, 1987). It is this understanding of norms and normative performance levels that engenders in the child an ability to evaluate her/his own competency levels against the perceived standards of the peer group. This latter view concerning self-definition and identity was addressed over seventy years ago in the writings of George Herbert Mead (1934). In his theory of symbolic interactionism, Mead suggested that exchanges among peers, whether experienced during cooperative or competitive activity, or during conflict or friendly discussion, enabled children to understand the self as both a subject and an object. Understanding that the self could be an object of others' perspectives gradually evolved into the conceptualization of a 'generalized other' or an organized and coordinated perspective of the 'social' group. In turn, recognition of the 'generalized other' led to the emergence of an organized sense of self. 7 The classic personality theory of Sullivan (1953) has served as a guide for much research concerning children's peer relationships and social skills. Like Piaget, Sullivan believed that the concepts of mutual respect, equality, and reciprocity developed from peer relationships. Sullivan, however, emphasized the significance of "special" relationships – chumships or best-friendships -- for the emergence of these concepts. In the early school years, whether friends or not, Sullivan thought children were basically insensitive to their peers. During the juvenile years (late elementary school), however, children were thought to be able to recognize and value each other's personal qualities; as a consequence, peers gained power as personality shaping agents. Sullivan's theory has proved influential in terms of the contemporary study of children's friendships, (e.g., Furman, Simon, Shaffer, & Bouchey, 2002) as well as in the understanding of loneliness as a significant motivational force in development and adjustment. (e.g., Asher & Paquette, 2003). Learning and social learning theory have also stimulated current research on children's peer relationships and social skills. It was originally suggested, and it is now known, that children learn about their social worlds, and how to behave within them, through direct peer tutelage as well as by observing each other. In this regard, children punish or ignore non-normative social behavior and reward or reinforce positively those behaviors viewed as culturally appropriate and competent (e.g., Dishion et al., 1999). Taken together, these theories and the data supportive of them have led psychologists to conclude that peer interactions and relationships are essential for normal social-cognitive and socioemotional development. But these theories are focused on the putative benefits of peer interactions and relationships. They “speak to” the development of competent behavioral styles and adaptive extra-familial relationships. The theories offer little with regard to establishing how insufficient or deficient interactions and relationships can lead to maladaptive behavioral styles or non-existent or dysfunctional extra-familial relationships. And yet, if peer interaction does lead to the development 8 of social competencies and the understanding of the self in relation to others, it seems reasonable to ponder the developmental consequences for those children who refrain from social interaction and avoid the company of their peers. It is this reasonable thought that drives much of the current research on social withdrawal. Lastly and perhaps most importantly, current research on social withdrawal is also guided by Hinde’s model of relationships. In this model, social withdrawal can be considered an individual characteristic that influences the presence or absence and quality of social exchanges (interactions) and relationships (e.g., friendship). This characteristic is also likely to influence the individual’s reputation and standing in the peer group (e.g., peer rejection). Thus, Hinde’s (1987) conceptual model serves as a useful heuristic to present central lines of inquiry and major research findings regarding children who avoid and withdraw from the peer group. A more detailed description of the research on the developmental course of social withdrawal, as it is influenced by Hinde’s model is presented below. KEY CLASSICAL AND MODERN RESEARCH STUDIES: BIOLOGICAL AND PARENTING FACTORS The Development of Social Withdrawal: The Contribution of Biology To begin with a focus on the individual in Hinde’s model, there is ample evidence to suggest that different forms of social withdrawal have varying biological and genetic roots. For example, to Kagan and collaborators (e.g., Kagan, Snidman, & Arcus, 1993), some infants are genetically hardwired with a physiology that biases them to be cautious, timid, and wary in unfamiliar social and nonsocial situations. It is contended that "inhibited" children differ from their uninhibited counterparts in ways that imply variability in the threshold of excitability of the amygdala and its projections to the cortex, hypothalamus, sympathetic nervous system, corpus striatum, and central gray. In support of the argument that inhibition has a genetic basis, Hariri and colleagues (2002) reported that the presence of the short 5 HT allele was associated with greater functioning of the 9 amygdala in response to fearful stimuli, but the same was not true for those individuals possessing the long 5 HT allele (Hariri et al., 2002). The functioning of the amygdala has long been associated with the display of inhibited behavior across species (e.g., Schwartz, Wright, Shin, Kagan, & Rauch, 2003). Consistent with the argument that there is a biological basis to social wariness and inhibition is research that indicates a pattern of greater relative right frontal EEG asymmetry associated with the display of behavioral inhibition in infancy (Calkins, Fox, & Marshall, 1996) and social reticence during the preschool years (Fox et al., 1995). Relatedly, Fox and colleagues (2001) found that continuously inhibited children (high on behavioral inhibition at 14 or 24 months and high on social reticence at four years) exhibited a pattern of greater relative right frontal EEG asymmetry at 9 months, 14 months, and four years when compared to those children who did not remain continuously inhibited (high on behavioral inhibition at 14 or 24 months and low on social reticence at four years; Fox, Henderson, Rubin, Calkins, & Schmidt, 2001). These links between EEG asymmetry and shy-reticent behavior have been documented in middle childhood as well (e.g., Schmidt, Fox, Schulkin, & Gold, 1999). Variations in the functioning of the autonomic nervous system have also been correlated with the display of shy/wary behavior in early childhood. For instance, lower cardiac vagal tone (an index of emotion dysregulation) is contemporaneously and predictively associated with the display of behavioral inhibition and socially reticent behavior in early childhood (Rubin, Hastings, Stewart, Henderson, & Chen, 1997). More recently, Henderson, Marshall, Fox, and Rubin (2004) found that two types of solitude (reticence and solitary-passive behavior) were associated with a pattern of greater relative right frontal EEG asymmetry. However, reticent children were rated by their mothers to be more socially fearful and displayed lower cardiac vagal tone than those children who were observed to spend time exploring and constructing on their own whilst in social company 10 (solitary-passive behavior). In further support of an underlying biological constitution of socially inhibited and reticent behavior, Hastings, Rubin and DeRose (2005) found that observed inhibition, maternal reports of social fearfulness, and cardiac vagal tone loaded on a single factor (with cardiac vagal tone loading negatively) at two years of age. Finally, the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenocortical (HPA) axis is thought be activated during stressful or novel situations. Researchers have demonstrated that elevated cortisol (a stress hormone) is associated with the demonstration of behavioral inhibition (Spangler & Schieche, 1998) and social reticence (Schmidt et al., 1997) in early childhood. The Development of Social Withdrawal: The Contributions of Parents Although there is evidence supporting a genetic or biological origin of shy-anxious social withdrawal, there also exists evidence implicating such distal factors as parenting style and the quality of the parent-child relationship in the development and maintenance of withdrawn behavioral patterns. Briefly, several researchers have found a contemporaneous and predictive link between insecure attachment status and the display of behaviorally inhibited behavior (e.g., Calkins & Fox, 1992). It bears noting, however, that insecure attachment relationships are also predicted by maternal behavior. For example, mothers of insecurely attached ‘C’ babies are overinvolved and overcontrolling compared to mothers of securely attached babies (Erickson, Sroufe, & Egeland, 1985). It is this overcontrolling, intrusive, and overly protective parenting style that is strongly associated, contemporaneously and predictively, with behavioral inhibition (e.g., Rubin et al., 1997; Rubin, Burgess, & Hastings, 2002) and socially withdrawn and reticent behavior (Coplan et al., 2004; Mills & Rubin, 1998; Rubin, Cheah, & Fox, 2001; Rubin & Mills, 1990, 1991). It has been suggested that such parenting practices not only reinforce anxious-withdrawn behavior but also may negatively impact the child’s sense of self-worth and autonomy. 11 Of course, children’s social reticence and anxious-withdrawal may also cause parental overprotection and overcontrol (Hastings & Rubin, 1999; Rubin, Nelson, Hastings, & Asendorpf, 1999). Sensing their young child's difficulties in social situations, some parents may try to provide support by manipulating their child's social behaviors in a power assertive, highly directive fashion (telling the child how to act or what to do). Conversely, any expression of social fearfulness in the peer group may evoke parental feelings of concern, sympathy, and perhaps a growing sense of frustration. The parent may begin to believe that their child is vulnerable and must be helped in some way (Rubin & Burgess, 2002). Such a "read" of the child may be guided by a developing belief system that social withdrawal is dispositionally-based, that it is accompanied by debilitating child feelings of fear and social anxiety, and that it is accompanied by behaviors that evoke, in peers, attempts to be socially dominant. The resultant parental behavior, guided by the processing of affect and historically- and situationally-based information, may be of a "quick fix" variety. To release the child from social discomfort, the parent may simply "take over" by telling the child what to do and how to do it (Rubin et al., 1999). Alternatively, the parent may simply solve the child's social dilemmas by asking other children for information desired by the child, obtaining objects desired by the child, or requesting that peers allow the child to join them in play. These parenting behaviors are likely to reinforce the child's feelings of insecurity, resulting in the maintenance of a cycle of child hopelessness/helplessness and parent overcontrol/overprotection (Mills & Rubin, 1993). Moreover, allowing the child to avoid feared social behaviors may, over time, prevent the child from attaining developmentally-appropriate social competencies. In summary, we have painted a portrait of mothers (and fathers, Rubin et al., 1999) of socially reticent and anxiously-withdrawn children as endorsing and practicing intrusive, controlling, and overprotective parenting strategies that are likely detrimental to the child's developing senses of autonomy and social efficacy. Such parenting beliefs and behaviors are not conducive to the 12 development of social competence or positive self-regard. Indeed, research has shown that an overprotective, overly-concerned parenting style is associated with submissiveness, dependency, and timidity in early childhood- characteristics which are typical of socially children and that may increase the likelihood of problematic peer relations (Olweus, 1993). We will expand on this point in the sections below wherein we focus on the peer relationships of socially withdrawn children. KEY CLASSICAL AND MODERN RESEARCH STUDIES: PEER INTERACTIONS AND RELATIONSHIPS Peer Interactions Drawing from Hinde’s model, the term “interaction” is reserved for dyadic behavior in which the participants' actions are interdependent such that each actor's behavior is both a response to, and stimulus for, the other participant’s behavior (Rubin, Bukowski, et al., 2006). At its core, an interaction comprises "such incidents as Individual A shows behavior X to Individual B, or A shows X to B and B responds with Y" (Hinde, 1979, p.15). Researchers have typically focused on three general forms of interactions: (1) movement toward others, (2) movement against others, and (3) movement away from others (Rubin, Bukowski, et al., 2006). Whilst important individual differences in the extent and nature of social interactions exist at all ages, children who move toward others are considered sociable or out-going, children who move against others are typically characterized as aggressive (due to the confrontational nature of their behavior), and children who move away from others are considered socially withdrawn (e.g., Caspi, Elder, & Bem, 1988). By moving away from others, withdrawn children and young adolescents interact less with their peers and spend more time on the periphery of the social scene than do their non-socially withdrawn counterparts. For example, socially reticent behavior is defined by the frequent display of watching others from afar and by aimless wandering when among peers (Coplan, Rubin, 13 Fox, Calkins, & Stewart, 1994). Given that social reticence and withdrawal is often accompanied by felt anxiety in both familiar and unfamiliar social situations (Asendorpf, 1990; Coplan & Rubin, 1998), it should not be surprising that reticent preschoolers and socially withdrawn elementary schoolers direct fewer social overtures to peers than their more sociable counterparts (Chen, DeSouza, Chen, & Wang, 2006; Coplan et al., 1994; Rubin, 1985; Stewart & Rubin, 1995). Also the forms that their overtures take differ from those of non-withdrawn peers. For instance, it has been shown that withdrawn children are less likely than non-withdrawn children to make requests of their peers that require them to carry out action requiring both effort and mobility (Rubin & Borwick, 1984; Rubin & Krasnor, 1986; Stewart & Rubin, 1995). Moreover, withdrawn children are less inclined, than children of average sociability, to produce socially assertive strategies (e.g., commands) and more likely to employ subtle, indirect means to attain their social goals (e.g., indirect requests). This pattern of social initiation suggests that children identified as withdrawn are less assertive with and also less demanding of their peers than their more sociable counterparts (Rubin, 1985; Rubin & Borwick, 1984; Stewart & Rubin, 1995). When socially withdrawn children do make requests of their peers, they are more likely than their more sociable agemates to be met by peer refusal and rejection (e.g., Chen et al., 2006; Nelson, Rubin, & Fox, 2005; Rubin & Krasnor, 1986; Stewart & Rubin, 1995). And importantly, as children move from early to middle childhood, withdrawn children become increasingly less assertive as well as less successful in their attempts to meet their social goals (Stewart & Rubin, 1995). This increasing lack of social assertiveness may be due to repeated peer rejection and/or to a "feedback loop," wherein the initially fearful and withdrawn youngster comes to believe that her/his social failures are internally based, which is strengthened by the increasing failure of her/his social initiatives. The failure to obtain peer compliance and collegiality with peers predicts the development of negative self-perceptions of social skills and peer relationships (Boivin & Hymel, 1997; Boivin, Hymel, 14 & Bukowski, 1995; Nelson et al., 2005). That is, “real life” peer rejection (e.g., non-compliance) predicts negative thoughts and feelings about the self. Importantly, socially withdrawn children’s self-perceptions are quite accurate; that is, they are well aware of their social difficulties. This is especially true of withdrawn children who are also unpopular (Hymel, Bowker, & Woody, 1993). The interactional style of anxious-withdrawn children suggests a negative, if not miserable cycle of anxious attempts to join others, followed by the actual, and felt experience of rejection or neglect, followed by reasonable surmising that the self is less than worthy in the company of peers. This suggested cycle suggests also that anxious-social withdrawal should be stable over time. It is. Social withdrawal and avoidance has been shown to be reasonably consistent across contexts. Thus, extremely withdrawn children move away from and avoid both familiar and unfamiliar peers in such settings as school, home, or community-at-large (Schneider, Richard, Younger, & Freeman, 2000; Schneider, Younger, Smith, & Freeman, 1998). Furthermore, social withdrawal is stable over time from early-to-middle childhood and thereafter into adolescence (e.g., Hymel et al., 1990; Rubin, Chen, et al., 1995; Rubin, Hymel, & Mills, 1989). Peer Relationships Relationships refer to the meanings, expectations, and emotions that derive from a succession of interactions between two individuals known to each other (Rubin, Bukowski, et al., 2006). Three questions dominate the literature on children’s relationships with friends (e.g., Hartup, 1996): (1) Does the child have a best friend? (2) Who are the child’s friends? (3) What is the quality of the child’s friendships? The extant literature on friendship is reviewed, by several authors, in this volume. Thus, we focus herein only on the friendships of socially withdrawn children. Because they remove themselves from the company of peers, socially withdrawn children might be expected to have difficulty making and keeping friends. Indeed, anxious-withdrawal has been found to negatively predict the number of mutual friendships during middle childhood 15 (Pedersen, Vitaro, Barker, & Borge, 2007). Nevertheless, during middle childhood and early adolescence, socially withdrawn children are as likely as non-withdrawn children to form and maintain at least one close friendship. Rubin, Wojslawowicz, Rose-Krasnor, Booth-LaForce, and Burgess (2006), for instance, reported that approximately 65% of socially withdrawn children aged 10 to 11 years had a mutual best friendship in the 5th grade, a percentage nearly identical to that of their non-withdrawn counterparts (70%). Furthermore, the majority of these friendships (69%) were stable across the fifth grade academic year, a finding almost identical to that for non-withdrawn children (70%). Similar findings have been reported in studies of young withdrawn children (e.g., Ladd & Burgess, 1999). The friends of socially withdrawn children have been characterized as being similarly withdrawn (e.g., Haselager, Hartup, van Lieshout, & Riksen-Walraven, 1998; Rubin, Wojslawowicz et al., 2006). Moreover, Rubin and colleagues demonstrated not only that the friends of withdrawn children are similarly withdrawn, but also they are similarly victimized by peers. It also appears to be the case that the quality of withdrawn children’s friendships is less than optimal. For example, withdrawn children rate their best friendships as significantly less helpful and intimate, and in the event of conflict, less likely to result in resolution than do non-withdrawn children (Rubin, Wojslawowicz et al., 2006). Furthermore, the best friends of withdrawn children also report their friendships as less fun and helpful than do the friends of non-withdrawn children. Schneider (1999) found that the friendships of socially withdrawn children were lower in communicative quantity and quality, findings which may begin to explain why the friendships of socially withdrawn children are marked by less intimate disclosure. Given that socially withdrawn children often experience in-vivo peer rejection, they may engage in self-silencing when in the company of friends. Self-silencing, a communication style which involves intentional verbal inhibition, is thought to reflect a defensive strategy to protect the self from criticism within the context of close relationships. Importantly, self- 16 silencing has been positively associated with such internalizing problems as depression, in samples of adults (e.g., Grant, Beck, Farrow, & Davila, 2007). Although self-silencing has received little attention in studies of children and adolescents (e.g., Harper & Welsh, 2007), a consideration of selfsilencing among socially withdrawn children could well enhance our understanding of the ways in which their inhibitory tendencies and fears of negative evaluation impact qualities and features of their close dyadic relationships. It is well-known that children’s friendships provide important sources of social and emotional support, especially in times of stress or transition (e.g., Berndt, Hawkins, & Jiao, 1999). This protective “power” of friendship has been shown to be particularly helpful for children who have difficulties within the larger peer group (Rubin, Bukowski et al., 2006). Hodges, Boivin, Vitaro, and Bukowski (1999), for example, demonstrated that simply having a mutual best friendship protected victimized children from increased internalizing and externalizing problems during late childhood. Thus far, there has been limited research on the protective power of friendship for socially anxious and withdrawn children. Recently, Oh, Rubin, Burgess, Booth-LaForce, & RoseKrasnor (2004) employed latent growth curve modeling to investigate the extent to which the perceived supportiveness of friendship served as a protective factor in the maintenance, decrease, or increase of anxious withdrawal over five assessments taken between the 5th and 8th grades (10 to 13 years of age). The perceived supportiveness of the child’s best friendship was a significant predictor for both the initial level of anxious withdrawal and the rate of change of withdrawal over time. Young adolescents who viewed their friendships as highly supportive were less likely to be socially withdrawn in the 5th grade and showed a greater rate of decline in social withdrawal over time. In a more recent analysis of data from the same sample, Oh and colleagues used General Growth Mixture Modeling (GGMM) to identify distinct pathways of anxious-withdrawal, differentiate valid subgroup trajectories, and examine factors that predicted change in trajectories 17 within subgroups (Oh et al., in press). When data were examined across five time points over a period of four years, three distinct social withdrawal trajectory classes were discovered: (1) a low stable class in which children were consistently low in social withdrawal; (2) an increasing class comprising children who became increasingly withdrawn over time; and (3) a decreasing class comprising children who initially were highly withdrawn, but became less so over time. With regard to friendship factors, children who had a socially withdrawn friend were more likely to show an initially high level of social withdrawal at the outset of the study (Fall, 5th grade). In addition, having a socially withdrawn friend after the transition to middle school (Fall, 6th grade) exacerbated children’s anxious-withdrawal over time, supporting the contention that withdrawn children and their best friends may mutually influence each other’s maladaptive behavior, just as is the case for aggressive children and their best friends (e.g., Dishion et al., 1999). Lastly, for those children who became increasingly more anxiously-withdrawn from 5th-to-8th grade, friendlessness and friendship instability, along with peer exclusion and victimization, were significant predictors of increased social withdrawal. Taken together, results from these two studies suggest that the benefits of friendship involvement for socially withdrawn children are positive when friendship quality is strong, but are limited or negative when the best friend is likewise withdrawn. Rejection and Victimization At the group level of Hinde’s social complexity, numerous studies have shown that anxioussolitude and withdrawal are contemporaneously and predictively associated with peer rejection throughout the childhood and early adolescent periods (e.g., Boivin et al., 1995; Hart et al., 2000; Ollendick, Greene, Weist, & Oswald, 1990; Rubin, Chen, & Hymel, 1993). It is argued that the relations between social withdrawal and peer rejection become increasingly strong across the elementary school years because social solitude becomes increasingly viewed by the peer group as 18 deviant from the norm (e.g., Gavinski-Molina, Coplan, & Younger, 2003; Younger, Gentile, & Burgess, 1993). Being perceived as anxious and withdrawn whilst simultaneously being rejected by the peer group-at-large may also serve to invite victimization by bullies. At the same time, being regularly exposed to bullying may lead to increased fear of classmates, and further withdrawal from peer interaction and school-related activities. These notions of reciprocal relations between withdrawal and victimization mesh well with suggestions that children characterized by a socially withdrawn demeanor can best be described as “whipping boys” (Olweus, 1993), “easy marks” (Rubin, Wojslawowicz, et al., 2006), physically weak (Hodges et al., 1999; Hodges, Malone, & Perry, 1997), and anxiously vulnerable (Perry, Kusel, & Perry, 1988). Empirical connections between anxiouswithdrawal and being victimized have been reported by several researchers (e.g., Boivin et al., 1995; Hanish & Guerra, 2004; Kochenderfer-Ladd, 2003). Importantly, the Oh et al. (in press) study described above indicated the significance of peer exclusion and victimization for the maintaining, indeed increasing, anxious-withdrawal from childhood through early adolescence. Social Withdrawal and Intrapersonal Processes There is considerable evidence indicating that such internalizing problems as loneliness, depression, and social anxiety, are both correlates and consequences of social withdrawal throughout childhood and adolescence (e.g., Boivin & Hymel, 1997; Gazelle & Ladd, 2003; Rubin et al., 1995). Furthermore, Rubin and colleagues (e.g., Rubin et al., 2003) have argued that as a result of their experiences with rejection and victimization, withdrawn children may attribute their social failures to internal causes; that is, over time they may come to believe that there is something wrong with themselves rather than attributing their social failures to other people or to situational factors. Supporting these conjectures, Rubin and Krasnor (1986) found that extremely withdrawn children tended to blame social failure on personal, dispositional characteristics rather than on external events 19 or circumstances. These results are in keeping with findings by Wichmann, Coplan, and Daniels (2004) who reported that when 9-to-13-year-old withdrawn children were presented with hypothetical social situations in which ambiguously caused negative events happened to them, they attributed the events to internal and stable “self-defeating” causes. Moreover, withdrawn children suggested that when faced with such negative situations, they were more familiar with failure experiences and that a preferred strategy was to withdraw and escape. This latter finding with regard to socially withdrawn children’s use of avoidant coping in response to negative events was replicated by Burgess, Wojslawowicz, Rubin, Rose-Krasnor, and Booth-LaForce (2006). Given the conceptual associations between social withdrawal, victimization, and peer rejection, the above noted findings by Wichmann et al. (2004) are reminiscent of work by Graham and Juvonen (2001). These latter researchers reported that youngsters who identified themselves as victimized by peers blamed themselves for their peer relationship problems. And Nolen-Hoeksema, Girgus, and Seligman (1992) have argued that self-blame can lead to a variety of negative outcomes of an internalizing nature, such as depression, low self-esteem, and withdrawal, thereby suggesting a self-reinforcing cycle of negative socioemotional functioning. In recent years, significant within-group variation in psychological adjustment has been observed for socially withdrawn children and adolescents. That is, not all withdrawn children report high levels of loneliness, depression, and anxiety (Rubin, Wojslawowicz Bowker, & Oh, 2007). Significantly, heterogeneity in adjustment has been associated with variability in peer exclusion (Boivin et al., 1995; Boivin & Hymel, 1997; Gazelle & Ladd, 2003; Gazelle & Rudolph, 2004). For example, in a longitudinal study, Gazelle and Ladd (2003) reported that shy-anxious kindergarteners excluded by peers displayed greater stability in anxious solitude through the 4th grade and had higher levels of depressive symptoms than shy-anxious peers who did not experience peer exclusion. A stress-diathesis model was suggested, whereby the experience of peer exclusion exacerbates the 20 outcomes associated with anxious-solitude. In a longitudinal study of 5th and 6th graders (10- to-11 years of age), Gazelle and Rudolph (2004) demonstrated that anxious-withdrawn children who were excluded by their peers maintained or increased the extent of their social avoidance and depression over time. In contrast, increased social approach and decreases in depression among anxious-solitary children resulted from the experience of low exclusion. Taken together, these data strongly suggest that socially withdrawn children’s adjustment may be determined, in part, by the extent to which they experience peer rejection and exclusion. Other than Gazelle’s research noted above, there have been few longitudinal investigations of the consequences of social withdrawal vis-à-vis intrapersonal processes. The majority of studies in which the relations between social withdrawal and internalizing problems have been examined have been contemporaneous in nature (e.g., Boivin & Hymel, 1997; Rubin et al., 1993). However, in the Waterloo Longitudinal Project, observed and peer assessed social reticence and passive withdrawal at seven years of age predicted negative self-regard and loneliness at nine and ten years of age (Hymel et al, 1990; Rubin et al., 1989). Moreover, seven-year social withdrawal predicted self-reported loneliness, depression, and negative self-regard at 14 years (Rubin, 1993; Rubin, Chen, et al., 1995). FUTURE DIRECTIONS OF THEORY AND RESEARCH In the early 1990’s, Rubin and colleagues proposed a theoretical model outlining developmental pathways in the etiology of social withdrawal and internalizing problems. This theoretical framework considered the joint influences of child characteristics, parental socialization practices, the quality of relationships outside the family, and macro-systemic forces. Transactional processes were postulated, describing the reciprocal and evolving relations over time between child temperamental predispositions and environmental contexts (see also Rubin et al., 2003; Rubin, Hymel, Mills, & Rose-Krasnor, 1991; Rubin, LeMare, & Lollis, 1990; Rubin & Mills, 1991). 21 In the past 20 years, researchers have examined the roles of biology and parenting in the etiology of social withdrawal in childhood. We urge researchers to continue to explore these areas, but also to consider these factors during the middle childhood and adolescent years; there are virtually no such studies of the biological and parent-child concomitants and predictors of social withdrawal during this developmental time frame. Moreover, there is a clear need to consider additional factors that may moderate the relations between biological bases of social withdrawal, the parenting contributors to social withdrawal, and adjustment outcomes over time. Borrowing from Hinde’s (1987), such additional factors might include social information processing (individual factors), social skills (interactions), friendships (relationships), and peer group status (acceptance/rejection; peer perceptions of status – as an easy mark, a “nerd,” an outlier). As an example of the significance of relationships, it is the case that during the early adolescent period, children are motivated to become increasingly autonomous whilst maintaining suitable and appropriate connections to parents (Collins & Steinberg, 2006). The motivation for increased autonomy, and the demonstration of independent, non-adult conforming (and yet peer conforming) behavior that reflects such efforts, may play a role in gaining peer acceptance during the early adolescent period. For example, whilst social-conventional reasoning might suggest to adults that bossiness, relational aggression, and risk taking behavior is less than admirable or acceptable, young adolescents might view such behaviors as within the realm of personal decisionmaking choices (see Killen, Rutland, & Jampol, this volume). In turn, such seemingly oppositional behaviors predict perceived popularity in early adolescence (e.g., Rose, Swenson, & Waller, 2004). Put another way, the display of behaviors that suggest a turn toward independence and autonomy are associated with perceived popularity and dominance in the peer group. But what about socially withdrawn young adolescents? Given the extant literature that such children are over-protected and –directed, and given that they appear to be adult dependent during 22 the early years (e.g., Rubin et al., 1997), might it be the case that socially anxious and withdrawn young adolescents do not strive for autonomy to the same extent as their more sociable peers? Relatedly, if overly connected to and reliant on parents, might these relationships provide withdrawn young adolescents with a dysfunctional model for non-familial relationships? Might their friendships and romantic relationships be marked by insecurity and over-dependence? These are questions that researchers would do well to consider as the literature on social withdrawal moves beyond the years of early and middle childhood. Recent research has indicated that friendship may serve as protective factors for those young adolescents who have poor parent-child relationships (e.g., Rubin et al., 2004). This being the case, and given that which is known about the friendships of socially withdrawn adolescents, could friendship serve as a protective buffer for those whose relationships with parents are less than adequate? Again, this question is deserved of some empirical attention. Another direction to be taken in the future concerns the subtypes of social withdrawal. Since the original contention that not all forms of solitude are “necessarily evil” (Rubin, 1982b), the vast majority of research studies on the many “faces” or subtypes of social withdrawal has focused on different solitary play behaviors observed in groups of young children (for a recent exception, Coplan, Wilson, Frohlick, & Zelenski, 2006). While these studies have illustrated the developmental risks associated with certain subtypes of social withdrawal, such as reticence, during early childhood, it appears timely for similar studies to be conducted with older children and young adolescents. To begin with, researchers would do well to inquire about children’s motivations for spending time alone when in the company of others or when provided with opportunities to interact with others (e.g., Coplan, Girardi, Findlay, & Frohlick, 2007). What are the cognitive or affective underpinnings of various forms of solitary behavior among older children and adolescents (e.g., Bowker, Rubin, Rose-Krasnor, & Booth-LaForce, 2007)? 23 It is also important that we better understand the ways in which the concomitants and consequences of social withdrawal, in its many forms, differ for boys and girls. Although there is some evidence to suggest that the negative prognosis for withdrawn boys is greater than for withdrawn girls, (e.g., Caspi et al., 1988; Coplan, Gavinski-Molina, Lagacé-Séguin, & Wichmann, 2001), additional research is needed that includes a wider range of psychosocial outcomes. For example, withdrawn boys may be at greater risk for physical victimization than withdrawn girls; might it be the case that withdrawn girls are at greater risk for relational victimization (e.g., Crick & Grotpeter, 1996)? Furthermore, given girls’ tendency to co-ruminate in the company of friends (Rose, 2002), perhaps withdrawn girls’ friendships represent risk contexts as they move into adolescence, at least relative to the friendships of withdrawn boys. As a final note, it is clear that the etiology of social withdrawal must be considered within cultural context. For example, in an extensive series of studies, Chen and colleagues have demonstrated that shy, reticent, reserved behavior in the People’s Republic of China is encouraged and accepted by mothers, teachers, and peers, and positively associated with social competence, peer acceptance, and academic success (e.g., Chen, Hastings, Rubin, Chen, Cen, & Stewart, 1998; Chen, Rubin, & Li, 1995). Chen (see Chen, Chung, & Hsiao, this volume) has argued that the collectivistic values found in Chinese culture place a strong emphasis on group cohesion; consequently, shyness and reservedness is more greatly appreciated than in western cultures that espouse individualistic beliefs and norms. Although this line of work has consistently found cross-cultural differences in the prevalence and correlates of socially withdrawn behavior, there is nary a study of the biological underpinnings of the phenomenon (e.g., EEG, EKG). Importantly, for this Handbook, there are few, if any, cross-cultural studies of the prevalence, stability, and quality of socially withdrawn children’s friendships or romantic relationships during the middle childhood and adolescent years. 24 Some Final Thoughts The study of social withdrawal in childhood has garnered an enormous amount of attention in the past two decades. Researchers have examined the developmental origins of social withdrawal, its contemporaneous and predictive correlates, and its consequences. It is now known that the social lives of many socially withdrawn children are less than optimal. Withdrawn children are socially deferent, anxious, lonely, and insecure in the company of peers, as well as rejected by peers. They recognize their social incapacities, believing themselves to be deficient in social skills and social relationships. Whether or not these factors lead inexorably to the development of psychopathology or clinical disorders is not yet known. Relatively few longitudinal studies exist; therefore, researchers would do well to examine the premise that social withdrawal represents a risk factor in childhood and adolescence. And certainly, it would behoove researchers to investigate whether the friendships and group status of socially withdrawn children represent possible protective and exacerbating factors within and across cultures. 25 References Achenbach. T. M., & Edelbrock, C. (1981). Behavioral problems and competencies reported by parents of normal and disturbed children aged 4- 16. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 46, 88. Asendorpf, J.B. (1990). Development of inhibition during childhood: Evidence for situational specificity and two-factor model. Developmental Psychology, 26, 721-730. Asendorpf, J.B. (1993). Beyond temperament: A two-factorial coping model of the development of inhibition during childhood. In K.H. Rubin and J. Asendorpf (Eds.), Social withdrawal, inhibition, and shyness in childhood (265-289). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum Associates. Asher, S.R., & Paquette, J.A. (2003). Loneliness and peer relations in childhood. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 12,3, 75-78. Berndt, T.J., Hawkins, J.A., & Jiao, Z. (1999). Influences of friends and friendships on adjustment to junior high school. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 45, 13–41. Boivin, M., & Hymel, S. (1997). Peer experiences and social self-perceptions: a sequential model. Developmental Psychology, 33, 135-145. Boivin, M., Hymel, S., & Bukowski, W. (1995). The roles of social withdrawal, peer rejection, and victimization by peers in predicting loneliness and depressed mood in childhood. Development and Psychopathology, 7, 765-785. Bowker, J.C., Rubin, K.H., Rose-Krasnor, L. & Booth-LaForce, C. (2007). Social withdrawal, negative emotion, and peer difficulties during late childhood. Manuscript under review, Burgess, K.B., Wojslawowicz, J.C., Rubin, K.H., Rose-Krasnor, L., & Booth-LaForce, C. (2006). Social information processing and coping styles of shy/withdrawn and aggressive Children: Does friendship matter? Child Development. 77, 371-383. 26 Calkins, S. D., & Fox, N. A. (1992). The relations among infant temperament, security of attachment, and behavioral inhibition at twenty-four months. Child Development, 63, 14561472. Calkins, S. D., Fox, N. A., & Marshall, T. R. (1996). Behavioral and physiological antecedents of inhibition in infancy. Child Development, 67, 523-540. Caspi, A., Elder, G. H., & Bem, D. J. (1988). Moving away from the world: Life-course patterns of shy children. Developmental Psychology, 24, 824-831. Chen, X., DeSouza, A., Chen, H., & Wang, L. (2006). Reticent behavior and experiences in peer interactions in Canadian and Chinese children. Developmental Psychology, 42, 656-665. Chen, X., Hastings, P. D., Rubin, K. H., Chen, H., Cen, G., & Stewart, S. L. (1998). Child-rearing attitudes and behavioral inhibition in Chinese and Canadian toddlers: A cross-cultural study. Developmental Psychology, 34, 677-686. Chen, X., Rubin, K. H., & Li, Z. (1995). Social functioning and adjustment in Chinese children: A longitudinal study. Developmental Psychology, 31, 531-539. Collins, W. A., & Steinberg, L. (2006). Adolescent development in interpersonal context. In W. Damon, R. M. Lerner, & N. Eisenberg (Eds.), Handbook of child psychology: Vol. 3, Social, emotional, and personality development (6th ed., pp. 1003 - 1067). New York: Wiley. Coplan, R.J., Gavinski-Molina, M.H., Lagace-Seguin, D., & Wichmann, C. (2001). When girls versus boys play alone: Gender differences in the associates of nonsocial play in kindergarten. Developmental Psychology, 37, 464-474. Coplan, R.J., Girardi, A., Findlay, L.C., & Frohlick, S.L. (2007). Understanding solitude: Young children’s attitudes and responses towards hypothetical socially-withdrawn peers. Social Development, 16, 390-409. 27 Coplan, R.J., Prakash, K., O’Neil, K., & Armer, M. (2004). Do you 'want' to play? Distinguishing between conflicted shyness and social disinterest in early childhood. Developmental Psychology, 40,2, 244-258. Coplan, R.J. & Rubin, K.H., (1998). Exploring and assessing nonsocial play in the preschool: The Development and validation of the preschool play behavior scale. Social Development, 7, 1, 71–91. Coplan, R.J., Rubin, K.H., Fox, N.A., Calkins, S.D., & Stewart, S.L. (1994). Being alone, playing alone, and acting alone: Distinguishing among reticence, and passive-, and active-solitude in young children. Child Development, 65, 129-138. Coplan, R.J., Wilson, J., Frohlick, S.L., & Zelenski, J. (2006). A person-oriented analysis of behavioral inhibition and behavioral activation in childhood. Personality and Individual Differences, 917-927. Crick, N. R., & Grotpeter, J. K. (1996). Children's maltreatment by peers: Victims of relational aggression. Development and Psychopathology, 8, 367-380. Damon, W., & Killen, M. (1982). Peer interaction and the process of change in children’s moral reasoning. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 28, 3, 347-367. Dishion, T.J., McCord, J., & Poulin, F. (1999). When interventions harm: Peer groups and problem behavior. American Psychologist, 54, 9, 755-764. Dodge, K. A., Coie, J. D., & Lynam, D. (2006). Aggression and antisocial behavior. In N. Eisenberg (Ed.), Handbook of child psychology. Volume 3. Social, emotional, and personality development (pp. 719788). New York etc.: Wiley. Erickson, M.F., Sroufe, L.A., & Egeland, B. (1985). The relationship between quality of attachment and behavior problems in preschool in a high-risk sample. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 50,1-2, 147-166. 28 Fine, G.A. (1987). With the boys. University of Chicago Press, Chicago. Fox, N. A., Henderson, H. A., Rubin, K. H., Calkins, S. D., & Schmidt, L. A. (2001). Continuity and discontinuity of behavioral inhibition and exuberance: Psychophysiological and behavioral influences across the first four years of life. Child Development, 72, 1-21. Fox, N. A., Rubin, K. H., Calkins, S. D., Marshall, T. R., Coplan, R. J., Porges, S. W., Long, J., & Stewart, S. (1995). Frontal activation asymmetry and social competence at four years of age. Child Development, 66, 1770- 1784. Furman, W., Simon, V.A., Shaffer, L., & Bouchey, H.A. (2002). Adolescents’ working models and styles for relationships with parents, friends, and romantic partners. Child Development, 73,1, 241-255. Gavinski-Molina, M.H., Coplan, R.J., & Younger, A. (2003). A closer look at children’s knowledge about social isolation. Journal of Research in Childhood Education, 18, 93-104. Gazelle, H., & Ladd, G.W. (2003). Anxious solitude and peer exclusion: A diathesis-stress model of internalizing trajectories in childhood. Child Development, 74, 257-278. Gazelle, H., & Rudolph, K. D. (2004). Moving toward and away from the world: Social approach and avoidance trajectories in anxious solitary youth. Child Development,75, 829–849. Graham, S., & Juvonen, J. (2001). An attributional approach to peer victimization. In J. Juvonen and S. Graham (Eds.), Peer harassment in school: The plight of the vulnerable and victimized. (pp. 49-72). New York, NY: Guilford Press. Grant, D. M., Beck, J. G., Farrow, S. M., & Davila, J. (2007). Do interpersonal features of social anxiety influence the development of depressive symptoms? Cognition and Emotion, 41, 3, 646663. Hanish, L.D. & Guerra, N.G. (2004). Aggressive victims, passive victims, and bullies: Developmental continuity or developmental change. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 50, 17-38. 29 Hariri, A. R., Mattay, V., Tessitore, A., Kolachana, B., Fera, F., Goldman, D., Egan, M. F., & Weinberger, D. R. (2002). Serotonin transporter genetic variation and the response of the human amygdala. Science, 297, 400-403. Harper, M. S. & Welsh, D. P. (2007). Keeping quiet: Self-silencing and its association with relational and individual functioning among adolescent romantic couples. Journal of Social and Personal Realtionships, 24, 99-116. Hart, C.H., Yang, C., Nelson, L.J., Robinson, C.C., Olsen, J.A., Nelson, D.A., Porter, C.L., Jin, S., Olsen, S.F., & Wu, P. (2000). Peer acceptance in early childhood and subtypes of socially withdrawn behaviour in China, Russia and the United States. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 24,1, 73-81. Hartup, W. W. (1996). The company they keep: Friendships and their developmental significance. Child Development, 67, 1–13. Haselager, G.J.T., Hartup, W.H., van Lieshout, C.F.M., & Riksen-Walraven, J.M.A. (1998). Similarities between friends and nonfriends in middle childhood. Child Development, 69, 4, 1198-1208. Hastings, P.D., & Rubin, K.H. (1999). Predicting mothers' beliefs about preschool-aged children's social behavior: Evidence for maternal attitudes moderating child effects. Child Development, 70,3, 722-741. Hastings, P. D., Rubin, K. H., & DeRose, L. (2005). Links among gender, inhibition, and parental socialization in the development of prosocial behavior. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 51,4, 467-493. Henderson, H. A., Marshall, P. J., Fox, N. A., & Rubin, K. H. (2004). Psychophysiological and behavioral evidence for varying forms and functions of nonsocial behaviors in preschoolers. Child Development, 75,1, 251-263. Hinde, R.A. (1979). Towards understanding relationships. London: Academic Press. 30 Hinde, R. A. (1987). Individuals, relationships and culture: Links between ethology and the social sciences. NY: Cambridge University Press. Hodges, E. V. E., Boivin, M., Vitaro, F., & Bukowski, W. M. (1999). The power of friendship: Protection against an escalating cycle of peer victimization. Developmental Psychology, 35, 94101. Hodges, E., Malone, M. J., & Perry, D. G. (1997). Individual risk and social risk as interacting determinants of victimization in the peer group. Developmental Psychology, 33, 1032-1039. Hymel, S., Bowker, A., & Woody, E. (1993). Aggressive versus withdrawn unpopular children: Variations in peer and self-perceptions in multiple domains. Child Development, 64, 3, 879-896. Hymel, S., Rubin, K., Rowden L., & LeMare, L. (1990). Children’s peer relationships: Longitudinal prediction of internalizing and externalizing problems from middle to late childhood. Child Development, 61,6, 2004-2021. Kagan, J., Snidman, N., & Arcus, D. (1993). On the temperamental categories of inhibited and uninhibited children. In K.H. Rubin and J.B. Asendorpf (Eds.), Social withdrawal, inhibition, and shyness in childhood. (pp. 19-28). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Kochenderfer-Ladd, B. (2003). Identification of aggressive and asocial victims and the stability of their peer victimization. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 49,4, 401–425. Ladd, G. W., & Burgess, K. B. (1999). Charting the relationship trajectories of aggressive, withdrawn, and aggressive/withdrawn children during early grade school. Child Development, 70, 910-929. Mead, G.H. (1934). Mind, self, and society: From the standpoint of a social behaviorist. Oxford, England: University of Chicago Press. 31 Mills, R.S.L., & Rubin, K.H. (1993). Socialization factors in the development of social withdrawal. In K. H. Rubin & J. B. Asendorpf (Eds.), Social withdrawal, inhibition and shyness in childhood (pp. 117-148). New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Mills, R.S.L., & Rubin, K.H. (1998). Are behavioural and psychological control both differentially associated with childhood aggression and social withdrawal? Canadian Journal of Behavioural Science, 30,2, 132-136. Nelson, L.J., Rubin, K.H., & Fox, N.A. (2005). Social and nonsocial behaviors and peer acceptance: A longitudinal model of the development of self-perceptions in children ages 4 to 7 years. Early Education and Development, 20, 185-200. Nolen-Hoeksema, S., Girgus, J.S., & Seligman, M.E. (1992). Predictors and consequences of childhood depressive symptoms: A 5-year longitudinal study. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 101,3, 405-422. Oh, W., Rubin, K.H., Bowker, J.C., Booth-LaForce, C.L., Rose-Krasnor, L., & Laursen, B. (in press). Trajectories of social withdrawal middle childhood to early adolescence. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology. Oh, W., Rubin, K. H., Burgess, K., Booth-LaForce, C. L., & Rose-Krasnor, L. (2004, July). A developmental perspective on social withdrawal across middle childhood and adolescence: Predictions from parental and peer factors. Poster presented at 18th Biennial Meeting of the International Society for the Study of Behavioral Development, Ghent, Belgium, Ollendick, T.H., Greene, R.W., Weist, M.D. & Oswald, D.P. (1990). The predictive validity of teacher nominations: A five-year follow up of at-risk youth. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 18, 6, 699-713. 32 Olweus, D. (1993). Victimization by peers: Antecedents and long-term outcomes. In K. H. Rubin & J. B. Asendorpf (Eds.), Social withdrawal, inhibition and shyness in childhood (pp. 315-341). New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Pedersen, S., Vitaro, F., Barker, E., & Borge, A. (2007). The timing of middle-childhood peer rejection and friendship: Linking early behavior to early-adolescent adjustment. Child Development, 78, 4, 1037-1051. Perry, D.G., Kusel, S.J., & Perry, L.C. (1988). Victims of peer aggression. Developmental Psychology, 24,6, 807-814. Piaget, J. (1932). The moral judgment of the child. Glencoe, Ill: Free Press. Rose, A. (2002). Co-rumination in the friendships of girls and boys. Child Development, 73, 1830-1843. Rose, A.J., Swenson, L.P., & Waller, E.M. (2004). Overt and relational aggression and perceived popularity: Developmental differences in concurrent and prospective relations. Developmental Psychology, 40, 378-387. Rubin, K.H. (1982a). Social and social-cognitive developmental characteristics of young isolate, normal and sociable children. In K.H. Rubin & H.S. Ross (Eds.), Peer relationships and social skills in childhood. New York: Springer-Verlag. (pp. 353-374) Rubin, K.H. (1982b). Nonsocial play in preschoolers: Necessarily evil? Child Development, 53(3), 651657. Rubin, K.H. (1985). Socially withdrawn children: An "at risk" population? In B. Schneider, K.H. Rubin, & J. Ledingham (Eds.), Children's peer relations: Issues in assessment and intervention. New York: Springer-Verlag. (pp. 125-139) Rubin, K. H. (1993). The Waterloo Longitudinal Project: Correlates and consequences of social withdrawal from childhood to adolescence. In K. H. Rubin & J. B. Asendorpf (Eds.), Social withdrawal, inhibition, & shyness in childhood, pp. 291-314. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. 33 Rubin, K. H., & Asendorpf, J. (1993). Social withdrawal inhibition, and shyness in childhood. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum Associates. Rubin, K.H., & Borwick, D. (1984). The communication skills of children who vary with regard to sociability. In H. Sypher & J. Applegates (Eds.), Social cognition and communication (pp. 152170). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Rubin, K. H., Bukowski, W., & Parker, J. G. (2006). Peer interactions, relationships, and groups. In W. Damon, R. M. Lerner, & N. Eisenberg (Eds.), Handbook of child psychology: Vol. 3, Social, emotional, and personality development (6th ed., pp. 571–645). New York: Wiley. Rubin, K. H., & Burgess, K. (2002). Parents of aggressive and withdrawn children. In M. Bornstein (Ed.), Handbook of parenting (2nd ed., Vol. 1, 383-418). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Rubin, K.H., Burgess, K., & Coplan, R. (2002). Social inhibition and withdrawal in childhood. In P.K. Smith & C. Hart (Eds.), Handbook of Childhood Social Development. London: Blackwell. Rubin, K. H., Burgess, K. B., & Hastings, P. D. (2002). Stability and social-behavioral consequences of toddlers’ inhibited temperament and parenting behaviors. Child Development, 73, 483-495. Rubin, K. H., Burgess, K. B., Kennedy, A. E., & Stewart, S. L. (2003). Social withdrawal in childhood. In E. J. Mash & R. A. Barkley (Eds.), Child psychopathology (2nd ed., pp. 372406.). New York: Guilford Press. Rubin, K. H., Cheah, C. S. L., & Fox, N. A. (2001). Emotion regulation, parenting and display of social reticence in preschoolers. Early Education and Development, 12(1), 97-115. Rubin, K. H., Chen, X., & Hymel, S. (1993). The socio-emotional characteristics of extremely aggressive and extremely withdrawn children. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 39, 518-534. 34 Rubin, K. H., Chen, X., McDougall, P., Bowker, A., & McKinnon, J. (1995). The Waterloo Longitudinal Project: Predicting adolescent internalizing and externalizing problems from early and mid-childhood. Development and Psychopathology, 7, 751-764. Rubin, K.H. & Coplan, R.J. (2004). Paying attention to and not neglecting social withdrawal and social isolation. In G. Ladd (Ed.) Appraising the Human Developmental Sciences: Essays in Honor of Merrill-Palmer Quarterly. Detroit: Wayne State University Press. Rubin, K.H., Coplan, R.J., Fox, N.A., & Calkins, S.D. (1995). Emotionality, emotion regulation, and preschoolers’ social adaptation. Development and Psychopathology, 7,1, 49-62. Rubin, K.H., Dwyer, K.M., Booth-LaForce, C.L., Kim, A.H., Burgess, K.B., & Rose-Krasnor, L. (2004) Attachment, friendship, and psychosocial functioning in early adolescence, Journal of Early Adolescence, 24, 326-356 Rubin, K. H., Hastings, P. D., Stewart, S. L., Henderson, H. A., & Chen, X. (1997). The consistency and concomitants of inhibition: Some of the children all of the time. Child Development, 68,3, 467-483. Rubin, K. H., Hymel, S., & Mills, R. S. L. (1989). Sociability and social withdrawal in childhood: Stability and outcomes. Journal of Personality, 57, 237-255. Rubin, K.H., Hymel, S., Mills, R.S.L. & Rose-Krasnor, L. (1991). Conceptualizing different pathways to and from social isolation in childhood. In D. Cicchetti and S. Toth (Eds.), The Rochester Symposium on Developmental Psychopathology, Vol. 2, Internalizing and externalizing expressions of dysfunction, (pp. 91-122). New York: Cambridge University Press. Rubin, K.H. & Krasnor, L.R. (1986). Social-cognitive and social behavioral perspectives on problem solving. In M. Perlmutter (Ed.),Cognitive perspectives on children's social and behavioral development. The Minnesota Symposia on Child Psychology (Vol. 18). Hillsdale, N.J.: Erlbaum.(pp.1-68). 35 Rubin, K. H., LeMare, L. J., & Lollis, S. (1990). Social withdrawal in childhood: Developmental pathways to peer rejection. In S. R. Asher & J. D. Coie (Eds.), Peer rejection in childhood. (pp. 217-249). New York, NY: Cambridge University Press. Rubin, K.H., & Mills R.S.L. (1988). The many faces of social isolation in childhood. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 56,6, 916-924. Rubin, K. H., & Mills, R. S. L. (1990). Maternal beliefs about adaptive and maladaptive social behaviors in normal, aggressive, and withdrawn preschoolers. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 18, 419-435. Rubin, K. H., & Mills, R. S. L. (1991). Conceptualizing developmental pathways to internalizing disorders in childhood. Canadian Journal of Behavioral Science, 19, 86-100. Rubin, K. H., Nelson, L. J., Hastings, P. D., and Asendorpf, J. (1999). The transaction between parents’ perceptions of their children’s shyness and their parenting styles. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 23, 937-957. Rubin, K.H., Wojslawowicz, J.C., Burgess, K.B., Rose-Krasnor, L., & Booth-LaForce, C.L. (2006). The friendships of socially withdrawn and competent young adolescents. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 34, 139-153. Rubin, K.H., Wojslawowicz Bowker, J.C., & Oh, W. (2007). The peer relationships and friendships of socially withdrawn children. In A.S. LoCoco, K.H. Rubin, & C. Zappulla (Eds.), L’isolamento sociale durante l’infanzia (Social withdrawal in childhood). Milan, Italy: Unicopli. Schmidt, L. A., Fox, N. A., Rubin, K. H., Sternberg, E. M., Gold, P. W., Craig, C., & Schulkin, J. (1997). Behavioral and neuroendocrine responses in shy children. Developmental Psychobiology, 30, 127-140. 36 Schmidt, L. A., Fox, N. A., Schulkin, J., & Gold, P. W. (1999). Behavioral and psychophysiological correlates of self-presentation in temperamentally shy children. Developmental Psychobiology, 35, 119-135. Schneider, B. H. (1999). A multi-method exploration of the friendships of children considered socially withdrawn by their peers. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 27, 115-123. Schneider, B. H., Richard, J. F., Younger, A. J., & Freeman, P. (2000). A longitudinal exploration of the continuity of children’s social participation and social withdrawal across socioeconomic status levels and social settings. European Journal of Social Psychology, 30, 497-519. Schneider, B. H., Younger, A. J., Smith, T., & Freeman, P. (1998). A longitudinal exploration of the cross-context stability of social withdrawal in early adolescence. Journal of Early Adolescence, 18, 734-396. Schwartz, C. E., Wright, C. I., Shin, L. M., Kagan, J., & Rauch, S. L. (2003). Inhibited and uninhibited infants ‘grown up’: Adult amygdalar response to novelty. Science, 300, 5627, 1952-1953. Selman, R.L., & Schultz, L.H. (1990). Making a friend in youth: Developmental theory and pair therapy. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press. Spangler, G. & Schieche, M. (1998). Emotional and adrenocortical responses of infants to the Strange situation: The differential function of emotional expression. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 22, 681-706. Stewart, S. L., & Rubin, K. H. (1995). The social problem solving skills of anxious-withdrawn children. Development and Psychopathology, 7, 323-336. Sullivan, H. S. (1953). The interpersonal theory of psychiatry. New York: Norton. Wichmann, C., Coplan, R. J., & Daniels, T. (2004). The social cognitions of socially withdrawn children. Social Development, 13, 377–392. 37 Younger, A. J., Gentile, C., & Burgess, K (1993). Children's perceptions of social withdrawal: Changes across age. In K.H. Rubin & J.B. Asendorpf (Eds.), Social withdrawal, inhibition and shyness in childhood (pp. 215-235). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.