[go: up one dir, main page]

Skip to main content
F1000Research logoLink to F1000Research
. 2019 Jun 25;8:F1000 Faculty Rev-963. [Version 1] doi: 10.12688/f1000research.18883.1

Translational models of adaptive and excessive fighting: an emerging role for neural circuits in pathological aggression

Herbert E Covington III 1, Emily L Newman 1, Michael Z Leonard 1, Klaus A Miczek 1,2,a
PMCID: PMC6593325  PMID: 31281636

Abstract

Aggression is a phylogenetically stable behavior, and attacks on conspecifics are observed in most animal species. In this review, we discuss translational models as they relate to pathological forms of offensive aggression and the brain mechanisms that underlie these behaviors. Quantifiable escalations in attack or the development of an atypical sequence of attacks and threats is useful for characterizing abnormal variations in aggression across species. Aggression that serves as a reinforcer can be excessive, and certain schedules of reinforcement that allow aggression rewards also allow for examining brain and behavior during the anticipation of a fight. Ethological attempts to capture and measure offensive aggression point to two prominent hypotheses for the neural basis of violence. First, pathological aggression may be due to an exaggeration of activity in subcortical circuits that mediate adaptive aggressive behaviors as they are triggered by environmental or endogenous cues at vulnerable time points. Indeed, repeated fighting experiences occur with plasticity in brain areas once considered hardwired. Alternatively, a separate “violence network” may converge on aggression circuitry that disinhibits pathological aggression (for example, via disrupted cortical inhibition). Advancing animal models that capture the motivation to commit pathological aggression remains important to fully distinguish the neural architecture of violence as it differs from adaptive competition among conspecifics.

Keywords: Aggression, violence, plasticity, phylogeny, animal models, reward, circuitry


The adaptive significance of aggressive acts has been critically examined for more than a century now 1, revealing neural substrates of agonistic behaviors, including excitatory and inhibitory amino acids, monoamines, and neuropeptides, in invertebrates, reptiles, fish, rodents, and non-human primates. Such efforts piece together the progression of phylogenetically conserved behaviors beginning with social pursuit and culminating in forceful attacks. The framework provided by these studies on species-typical aggression has allowed us to characterize deviations from adaptive agonistic encounters with the potential to identify the neurobiology of pathological aggression as it pertains to human and veterinary medicine and the criminal justice system. These discoveries guide ongoing and future scientific endeavors that will clarify the neuroplastic events that ultimately promote maladaptive aggression and violence.

Adaptive aggression

Defense of one’s territory, resources, social position, and group identity depends on the activation of neural circuits that overlap and interact with those involved in many other prominent adaptive social behaviors, including sex, parenting, bonding, and play 24. Mammalian aggression is rooted in limbic and diencephalic circuits, including connections between the medial amygdala (MeA), preoptic area, bed nucleus of the stria terminalis (BNST), lateral septum (LS), ventral portion of the premammillary nucleus (PMv), ventral lateral portion of the ventromedial hypothalamus (VMHvl), supramammillary nuclei (SUM), anterior hypothalamic nucleus, and paraventricular nucleus of the hypothalamus 5. Evidence suggests that these areas collectively encompass a hierarchical role in the generation of attack sequences, and they also appear in a generalized social network in the brain 6.

For many species, pheromonal cues from hostile social targets are coded by the depolarization of Gα O-containing vomerosensory neurons (VSNs) in the basal vomeronasal organ 7, triggering a distinctive pattern of activation across cortical, limbic, and brain stem nuclei to initiate or inhibit an attack 8. Here, evidence favors parallel processing of pro- and anti-social information to yield an adaptive behavioral output. Co-activation of Gα i-positive VSNs by alternative non-volatile pheromonal signals can modulate the pro-aggressive actions of Gα O-positive cells 9, 10.

Sensory signals carried by VSNs terminate in the accessory olfactory bulb where the topographical organization of odor information is relayed to the MeA and other limbic structures (for example, BNST) according to a social stimulus–dependent activational pattern 11. Inputs from the amygdala, BNST, and LS to the hypothalamus further coordinate the production of autonomic and somatic attack elements 1216. The PMv provides excitatory stimulation of the murine SUM and VMHvl, which modulate attack duration and the total number of attacks during a fight, respectively 17. Steroid receptor–expressing neurons (that is, ESR1 + cells) in the VMHvl appear to shape the strength and quality of these attacks 1820. Indeed, the excitability of hypothalamic nuclei adapts to repeated fighting experiences 21. Specifically, daily bouts of aggression fine-tune ensembles of ESR1 + VMHvl neurons in mice, suggesting that cell populations active during the execution of a fight are reciprocally modified by these behavioral outputs 22. Significant measures of plasticity, including changes in catecholamine uptake, have also been demonstrated in cerebral cortex of mice with only a single aggressive experience 23, 24.

Winning social confrontations or territorial fights largely depends on the functional integrity of these limbic and hypothalamic brain areas. The mesocorticolimbic dopamine system further encodes information about the outcome of these social encounters. This action–outcome link is evident in the maintenance of social status and contributes to the motivation to engage in or instigate future fights 17, 2529. The expression of neuropeptides and their receptors (that is, corticotropin-releasing factor, oxytocin, and vasopressin) that modulate monoaminergic neurons is equally important for stabilizing patterns of aggressive behavior 30, 31. These fighting-induced patterns of neural activity are accompanied by generalized increases in arousal and by elevations in serum corticosterone concentrations indicating instant sympathetic activity followed by glucocorticoid activation. Furthermore, these systems are influenced by endogenous anabolic steroids. Testosterone levels in male marine iguanas of the Galápagos rise significantly during the mating season, when their hostility toward other males also escalates; when hormone levels dip, the males cohabitate peacefully. Similar observations for species-typical aggression have been measured in fighting fish, roosters, rodents, and primates 3236.

Current neuron-specific viral manipulations are guided by evidence from early electrical recording and stimulation work with microelectrodes aimed at cell clusters in the medial and lateral hypothalamus 3742 as well as the central and periaqueductal gray 43, 44. Pioneering work by Chi and Flynn points to a modulatory role of cortical and limbic structures over hypothalamically triggered aggressive behaviors 45, including a hippocampal–septal–hypothalamic circuit 46. Leroy et al. 47 recently corroborated and extended this proposal, showing that the dorsal CA2 region of the hippocampus provides excitatory tone over the dorsal LS. This glutamatergic pathway disinhibits the VMHvl through GABAergic modulation of the ventral LS, which was found to directly reduce the tonic activity of VMH ESR1 + neurons activated during a fight. Efforts like these that dissect the complex circuitry underlying specific motivational, hedonic, and adaptive determinants of aggression will provide critical insight about potentially pathological reactive and cold forms of agonistic behaviors.

Evidence for a hierarchical neural organization of species-typical aggression is further supported by studies targeting midbrain structures, including the central gray at the level of the superior colliculus. Recordings from the dorsal periaqueductal gray (PAG) reveal its functional activation during fights and electrical stimulation of this region rapidly generates intense affective defense 48, 49. Tracing studies have identified descending second-order glutamatergic PAG projections to the pontine nucleus, raphe magnus, and locus coeruleus that modulate arousal, sympathetic tone, and motor aspects of the well-characterized feline defensive attack 44. Ascending attack-promoting tracts densely target areas of the VMH, which, as mentioned above, also coordinates elements of attack performance through reciprocal PAG innervations. Much less is known about upstream telencephalic modulators of species-typical aggression, although changes in functional activity within the neocortex have been observed after fighting in rodents and hamsters 5052. In addition, the initiation of attack, execution patterns, and cessation of an aggressive bout can be significantly modulated by descending cortical projections targeting limbic and brain stem areas. Such observations point to a possible source of input for “top-down” dysregulation of what is typically adaptive fighting 53.

Aggressive behaviors, even in contests without physical contact, can be advantageous and may be the basis for the “winner effect”, particularly when resources are scarce. When aggressive displays lead to physical attacks, dominant and subordinate profiles emerge quickly and often result in the formation of a social hierarchy 54. Such encounters are a normal part of daily intercourse for most gregarious animals. Thus, it is necessary to consider efficient fighting in the context of a specific species’ adaptive behavioral repertoire. Understanding the cortical and subcortical mechanisms that are fundamental to orchestrating species-typical adaptive aggression is essential for characterizing the pathological origins and neurobiology of excessive aggression 55, 56. Effective models of pathological aggression should, in simple terms, be capable of identifying the neural processes that motivate organisms to fight excessively under conditions that would not typically produce intense or prolonged attacks. It is important to consider that even the most excessive forms of fighting can be circumstantially adaptive.

Modeling pathological aggression

According to classic ethological perspectives, pathological aggression does rarely manifest beyond humans, as aggression is considered a necessary means for survival 57. But in the 1990’s striking reports of intragroup coordinated attacks between neighboring chimpanzees provided evidence to the contrary 58. Tanzanian chimpanzee raids on neighboring troops living in bordering territories present themselves as excessive, antisocial, and violent 58. As reviewed by de Boer 59, rodent and other animal models of excessive aggression are characterized by operational criteria that include elements that are impulsive (short latency to attack), excessive (increased levels of attack), and socially atypical (injurious attack topography, disregard for submissive signals, and indiscriminate social targeting). In line with these deviations in behavior, unique patterns of functional neural activation are apparent in comparisons between potentially pathological and species-typical aggressors. For example, artificial selection of male mice with short attack latencies generates a population of excessively aggressive animals that exhibit atypical patterns of activation (that is, c-Fos expression) in the medial prefrontal cortex, central amygdala, and the aforementioned ventral lateral portion of the PAG and VMHvl after a fight 60.

Clinical evidence strongly supports a top-down theory for the occurrence of violence 61. Transcranial direct current stimulations of the human dorso-lateral prefrontal cortex decrease intentions for physical or sexual assault in healthy women and men 62. Evidence from imaging studies further demonstrates that deficits in cortical inhibition promote hyper-reactive amygdaloid responses to aggressive words, which in turn may facilitate maladaptive aggression 63. In such cases, the impulse to act aggressively may be less of a choice and more the product of hardwired cortical dysfunction. Experimental models incorporating mice corroborate the occurrence of cortically mediated pathological aggression, whereby brief optogenetic stimulations (20 Hz) of channelrhodopsin or halorhodopsin residing on neurons in the prefrontal cortex bi-directionally control patterns of fighting behavior. In fact, stimulation delays the onset of an attack whereas inhibition of cortical cells facilitates vicious attacks 64. A wealth of human studies suggest that areas of the cortex (for example, the anterior cingulate cortex) allow us to resolve emotional and cognitive conflicts via inhibitory control over downstream limbic targets 61. However, it should be noted that descending excitatory pathways targeting hypothalamic attack areas can also facilitate impulsive-like aggressive behaviors at least in rodents. Stimulation of pyramidal neurons that specifically innervate the mediobasal hypothalamus increases the number of offensive attacks, and activation of inputs to the lateral hypothalamus favors attacks directed at vulnerable areas of an opponent’s body 53. Although most preclinical studies such as these employ loss- or gain-of-function parameters to determine the relationship between cortical nuclei and downstream limbic targets, it remains important to learn how genetic 65, social 66, environmental 67, 68, and pharmacological 69 variables might shape specific nuclei within cortical and subcortical cell groups and perhaps contribute to expressions of pathological aggression. In sum, rigorous experimental analyses of aggressive behaviors following manipulations of cortical regions are of high importance given the clinical evidence.

Aggression as a reward

Neural circuits that support fighting evolved independently at several phylogenic levels, including insects, fish, reptiles, and mammals 56, 70. Therefore, it was unexpected that members of each of these animal species would eagerly work for the opportunity to attack an opponent, indicating that aggression is further controlled by a conserved reward system 7174. Winning a confrontation is indeed a positive experience that strengthens future fighting, thus serving as a potent positive reinforcer or reward 7577. The concept of a reward includes at least three separable behavioral components: conditioning, incentive motivation, and a pattern of affective responses (for example, facial expressions and vocalizations) 78. As such, when aggression serves as a reward after instrumental conditioning in mice, it appears to differ from normal territorial fighting because it is not dependent on, or necessarily initiated by, olfactory stimuli, nor does it function to suppress other behaviors, like sex. In this case, seeking an opportunity to fight is triggered by conditioned environmental cues, and operant responding is driven by the opportunity to attack a conspecific (that is, an anticipated aggressive outcome). In line with this hypothesis, a direct infusion of the dopamine D1- and D2-like receptor antagonist SCH23390 into the nucleus accumbens selectively disrupts responding for an expected aggression reward 27.

A number of different schedules of reinforcement have been employed to investigate the motivational components of an aggression reward. Patterns and rates of responding during fixed ratio and progressive ratio schedules are perhaps most commonly used for providing an index of the motivational value for a given reinforcer 79, 80. However, after completing the demands of a fixed interval (FI) schedule to gain access to a fight, mice display significantly escalated, intense forms of attack that are far more vicious than species-typical fights 81 ( Figure 1). These displays of escalated aggression are exemplified by rapid attack latencies followed by a continuous burst of attacks 82, 83. Therefore, the pattern of this conditioned form of aggression appears context-specific. Mice conditioned to respond under an FI schedule of aggressive reinforcement display quite normal, species-typical patterns of fighting when an intruder suddenly invades their home cage ( Figure 2).

Figure 1. The sequence of experimental events for establishing individual levels of motivated responding for aggression and fighting performance during a daily fixed interval (FI) trial for aggression reward.

Figure 1.

Prior to their confirmation of territorial aggression, resident male mice are initially housed with females for at least 1 month. In subsequent daily resident–intruder confrontations, each resident male encounters a novel male intruder for less than 5 minutes in the resident’s home cage. After establishing an aggressive phenotype, each resident is trained during a daily FI schedule that is reinforced by the presentation of an intruder (delivered into the resident’s home cage). The duration of the FI is progressively increased from 1 second to 10 minutes over the course of about a month. Resident mice then are allowed to establish a consistent pattern of FI responding. The upper left panel displays typical FI10 scalloped patterns of responding by mice for an aggression reward. Individual responses (tick marks) that are typically generated during a 10-min FI are indicated at the bottom left for varying levels of anticipatory arousal. The first response initiated after the completion of the FI allows the introduction of an intruder into the resident’s home cage. The upper right panel displays one element of fighting behavior (attack bites) that can be examined and potentially altered by experimental manipulations. Other aggressive elements that can be quantitatively and qualitatively examined include the latency to a first attack, number of attack bouts (bottom right), and biting topography (not shown). Changes in appetitive motivational responding can occur with or without alterations in fighting performance.

Figure 2. Aggression that serves as a reward is more intense than species-typical fighting.

Figure 2.

Aggressive behaviors, including tail rattle, sideways threat, and attack bites, are substantially increased when outbred male mice work during a fixed interval schedule for the opportunity to fight a conspecific (dark bars) as compared with species-typical levels of agonistic behavior that occurs during spontaneous aggression (open bars). Adapted from Fish et al. 81 with permission from Springer-Verlag.

A significant advantage of FI-conditioned aggression is the sensitive ability to capture the intensity of arousal during the anticipation of a fight. When operant behavior is reinforced by the opportunity to fight, the accelerating rate of operant responding during the interval preceding an aggressive confrontation serves as an index for the motivational state of an animal 8486. FI schedules allow for very precise dissections of both appetitive motivational and consummatory performance components of an attack ( Figure 1). In fact, clear dissociations between the level of motivation to engage in aggression and the intensity of a fight can be discerned when aggression rewards are controlled by FI schedules, as described below in more detail 83.

We speculate that the occurrence of an interaction between arousal and reward expectancy prompts an escalation of fighting once behavioral requirements of the FI schedule are completed. Validated models of frustration similarly escalate the frequency of attacks on an opponent 8789. Indeed, the frustrative non-reward effect proposed by Amsel and Roussel 90 has provided evidence for frustration-induced surges in generalized vigor. FI schedules simply require animals to wait before being reinforced. Close examination of mice performing under an FI schedule reveals a uniquely arousing component that emerges throughout the pre-fight interval. The anticipatory phase elevates corticosterone blood levels 81, and there are progressive gains in digging and jumping behaviors from the first to the last minute of the FI 83. Likewise, explosive human aggression is preceded by physiological indicators of stress, such as activation of the hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenal axis 91 and the emotion of anger 92. Thus, despite the anticipation for a reward, some degree of arousal, possibly corresponding to human frustration, is produced by interval schedules, further increasing their translational value when capturing components of explosive clinical aggression. Parametric studies on the physiological and neurobiological components of anticipatory responding for aggression rewards are further warranted.

Alcohol escalates the urge to fight

More than half of all violent criminal acts are linked to alcohol consumption, and alcohol-related violence causes significant pain and suffering worldwide 9395. Such clinical data reveal that cycles of intoxication intensify reactive “hot” acts of violence. Alcohol induces a motivational state that culminates in repeated attempts to pursue violence despite behavioral impairments and uncoordinated motor control 96, 97. Most emergency room visits across the globe resulting from alcohol intoxication are due to violence, far outnumbering traffic accidents and incidental mishaps 98. In experimental models, alcohol has a wide range of acute and chronic effects on both motivational and consummatory behavioral processes. For example, low doses of alcohol acutely increase fighting performance in mice, rats, and monkeys by increasing the number of attacks and threats without affecting the motivation to fight. In contrast, higher doses of alcohol (>1.8 g/kg) disrupt both motivational and performance aspects of a fight 83. Poor fighting performance persists with repeated exposures to alcohol despite a rapid and full recovery of operant responding for the opportunity to fight. This recovery emerges within just a few days, suggesting the development of tolerance to the suppressive effects of alcohol on aggressive motivation. In addition, appetitive response rates actually sensitize with repeated alcohol exposures despite continued disruption of fighting performance ( Figure 3) 83. Such data are important given that physical performance is not a necessary component of most violent criminal acts, in which use of a firearm largely exceeds all other acts of aggression 97, 99.

Figure 3. Aggressive motivation escalates with repeated administrations of alcohol and remains persistently sensitized to lower concentrations of alcohol.

Figure 3.

Fixed interval (FI) response rates (upper middle) are depicted over repeated oral administrations (via gavage) of water or alcohol (2.2 g/kg; top) and 10 days later in response to a dose of water or a low challenge dose of alcohol (1.0 g/kg; bottom middle). Here, the motivation to fight is initially disrupted by alcohol before levels of FI responding recover to baseline. After just seven daily alcohol administrations, the motivation to fight in response to a low dose of alcohol becomes sensitized (bottom middle). Fighting behavior is persistently disrupted over the course of seven consecutive daily administrations of alcohol (2.2 g/kg; upper right) but not significantly affected 10 days later by a challenge dose of alcohol (1.0 g/kg; bottom right). Adapted from Covington et al. 83 with permission from Frontiers Media SA.

The behavioral distinctions between motivational and performance measures suggest that mechanisms promoting aggressive arousal are distinct from those that coordinate the attack. Both conditioned aggression and alcohol-escalated aggression depend on glucocorticoid receptor activation and thus are likely to reflect hot, reactive aggression 81, 100102. In contrast, cold aggression is associated with a lack of sympathetic arousal and is more frequently linked to predation in animals and psychopathy in humans 46, 103105. Sites of plasticity in response to repeated administrations of alcohol use (for example, prefrontal cortex, amygdala, extended amygdala, septal nuclei, and monoaminergic terminal regions) may provide neural targets for examining their role in motivating offensive aggression.

Female aggression

Hillali Matama’s 1974 eye-witness documentation of a Tanzanian chimpanzee party raid was among the first to portray violence in non-human primates which contrasted with popularized accounts of peaceful coexistence and conflict resolution 58, 106. In Matama’s account (page 16), the gruesome execution of a lone foraging male chimp was described in detail: “Once again, the victim was an isolated Kahama male—Dé was his name—and the attackers were a gang of four (chimpanzees) from Kasekela: three adult males and one adult female58. Each member in this gang of four contributed to the physical assault and brutal killing of Dé. Despite this record and other clear indications of the potential for violence in females across the animal kingdom 107111, most experimental studies on aggression focus on territorial males. Interestingly, a significant subset of outbred female laboratory mice and rats housed with sterile males will consistently exhibit “rival aggression” toward unfamiliar female intruders 112, 113. At least three distinct aggressive phenotypes emerge from a population of regularly cycling outbred Swiss–Webster (that is, CFW and SW) female mice housed with castrated males: rival and gestational aggressors (>65%), exclusively gestational aggressors (>20%), and non-aggressors (~10%) 113. These studies add to a rich literature on female gestational aggression 114, 115 and defensive maternal aggression 5, 116, 117, which are time-locked to hormonal changes associated with pregnancy, parturition, and lactation and serve a specific adaptive function to improve offspring survival.

Male-directed postpartum female aggression increases offspring survival by preventing lactation-concurrent pregnancies and infanticide. In individually housed lactating CFW females, male- and juvenile-directed attacks clearly require activation of estrogen receptor alpha–expressing neurons in the posterior aspect of the VMHvl 118. Although females rarely attacked adult female conspecifics under these conditions, activation of VMHvlEsr1 + cells can promote female-directed aggression. In rodents, hypothalamic inputs from the posterodorsal MeA may modulate the initiation of aggression by relaying threat-related olfactory and pheromonal signals 119. It is possible that, for individually housed lactating females, an adult female conspecific does not register as a threat warranting attack. In contrast, the majority of nulliparous CFW females housed with sterile males will readily engage in intense aggression toward adult female conspecifics 113. Although gestational and postpartum maternal aggression have clear adaptive purposes, further studies are necessary to identify the neural underpinnings and evolutionary forces that maintain rival aggressors and non-aggressors in a population of nulliparous outbred females.

Future directions

  • Although male animals raised in isolation, including fish, lizards, birds, and most mammals, will readily fight in a species-specific manner (a core component of the innateness of aggression), cellular and molecular data suggest that aggression stems from a neurogenetic network that is extraordinarily plastic. Observations of neural plasticity with increases in aggressive behavioral experiences should be highlighted 22. Investigations focused on these processes are providing much-needed insight toward an understanding of the evolution of excessive aggression.

  • Quantitative measures that capture the intensity of an aggressive episode such as the latency to attack onset, the attack duration, and the attack frequency are instructive in the early characterization of agonistic behaviors. However, it can be argued that even the most formidable fights are adaptive if they guarantee access to vital reproductive or environmental resources 120. Therefore, identifying pathological aggression requires more detailed analyses of attack dynamics and the specific sequence of behaviors exhibited by both the perpetrator and victim. For example, resident male rats will exhibit high levels of aggression toward an intruder conspecific; however, when the submissive intruder displays his ventrum in a supine posture, further attacks are inhibited. Pathological aggression would be evident in an aggressor that continued to attack despite demonstration of the supine posture by the intruder. Lag sequential behavioral analyses may reveal telltale behavioral signals from the subordinate intruder that, under species-typical conditions, should elicit a specific behavioral response from the dominant individual. Similarly, how an aggressive animal attacks an intruder provides valuable information about species-typical and pathological behavior. In mice and rats, most attacks between males are directed toward the posterior dorsum. Atypical placement of attack bites on the vulnerable ventrum of an opponent can reflect pathological aggression and may provide more insight than attack frequency or duration alone 59, 60, 69, 121, 122.

  • Perhaps more revealing of potentially pathological or violent routines are the patterns of responding that are emitted in anticipation of aggression as a reward. In such cases, the amount of effort exerted before a fight depicts appetitive and anticipatory components of the motivation to fight when fighting does not serve any obvious adaptive function. Mice trained to respond on a task are reinforced by the opportunity to attack an opponent, oftentimes in their own home environment 82, 83. Fixed ratio and progressive ratio schedules of reinforcement have been used to assess this motivation in addition to protocols that assess reinstatement of extinguished responding and measures of resistance to punished responding 71, 123, 124. Herein, we highlight the use of FI schedules that lead to aggression rewards for several reasons. First, scalloped patterns of FI response curves for individual mice are remarkably stable. We therefore suggest that shifts in FI response rates may indicate sensitive changes in motivational state. Second, mice that are conditioned under these contingencies display excessive aggression only in the context of schedule-controlled aggression. Fighting outside of this context resembles species-typical patterns of fighting ( Figure 2) 81. Thus, there is a notable degree of state dependency for this display of escalated aggression in mice that otherwise behave in a species-typical manner. In addition, mice will work extremely hard for the opportunity to fight even when fighting performance has been severely disrupted as a result of alcohol. Not only is this finding translationally significant, but it highlights a unique dissociation between the motivational and consummatory components of a powerful behavioral reward, similar to other natural rewards 125.

  • One of the urgent tasks is to continue identifying the neurobiological characteristics of cell clusters and pathways mediating aggressive behavior that are conserved across species and translate readily to the human condition in both males and females. It will be important to identify neural mechanisms that control the intensity of behavioral arousal in anticipation of an aggressive opportunity as captured by accelerating rates of responding reinforced by a fight. Hierarchically organized circuits regulate social dominance (for example, amygdala–hippocampal–septal–hypothalamic) as well as defense and subordination 22, 46, 47, 102, 118, 126129. Yet it remains to be determined how neural circuits that are active during a fight are linked to networks that are active during pre-fight anticipatory arousal. Does aggression induce neural plasticity that promotes a shift in the social brain network toward pathology? It is easy to speculate that the same diencephalic and telencephalic structures that are important for organizing and integrating sensory, motor, endocrine, emotional, and cognitive functions during species-typical aggression also contribute to the production of pathologies in social behavior, including aggression. Alternatively, does the role of an “upstream” mediator determine the level of aggressive output? Cortical structures are vulnerable in the sense that they are (1) influenced by contextual stimuli, (2) highly plastic, and (3) targeted by diverse pharmacological, endocrine, and circadian variables. Experiments that measure and interrogate candidate neural mechanisms during the anticipation of an intense fight will help to clarify the ontogeny of maladaptive aggression. Recent advances in molecular and recording techniques promise to reveal new molecules, cells, circuits, and patterns of temporospatial connectivity between brain regions that contribute to specific types of aggression.

Editorial Note on the Review Process

F1000 Faculty Reviews are commissioned from members of the prestigious F1000 Faculty and are edited as a service to readers. In order to make these reviews as comprehensive and accessible as possible, the referees provide input before publication and only the final, revised version is published. The referees who approved the final version are listed with their names and affiliations but without their reports on earlier versions (any comments will already have been addressed in the published version).

The referees who approved this article are:

  • Kent Berridge, Department of Psychology, University of Michigan, Ann Arbor, MI, USA

  • Takefumi Kikusui, Department of Animal Science and Biotechnology, Azabu University, Sagamihara, Japan

  • Dayu Lin, Neuroscience Institute, New York University School of Medicine, 522 First Avenue, New York, NY 10016, USA

Funding Statement

This work was supported by the National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism (R01AA013983 and F31AA025827) and the National Institute on Drug Abuse (R01DA031734).

The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.

[version 1; peer review: 3 approved]

References

  • 1. Craig W: Why Do Animals Fight? Int J Ethics. 1921;31(3):264–78. 10.1086/intejethi.31.3.2377580 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 2. Pfaff DW: Hormones, genes, and behavior. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 1997;94(26):14213–6. 10.1073/pnas.94.26.14213 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3. Kohl J, Autry AE, Dulac C: The neurobiology of parenting: A neural circuit perspective. Bioessays. 2017;39(1):1–11. 10.1002/bies.201600159 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]; F1000 Recommendation
  • 4. Li Y, Dulac C: Neural coding of sex-specific social information in the mouse brain. Curr Opin Neurobiol. 2018;53:120–30. 10.1016/j.conb.2018.07.005 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]; F1000 Recommendation
  • 5. Hashikawa K, Hashikawa Y, Lischinsky J, et al. : The Neural Mechanisms of Sexually Dimorphic Aggressive Behaviors. Trends Genet. 2018;34(10):755–76. 10.1016/j.tig.2018.07.001 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]; F1000 Recommendation
  • 6. O'Connell LA, Hofmann HA: The vertebrate mesolimbic reward system and social behavior network: a comparative synthesis. J Comp Neurol. 2011;519(18):3599–639. 10.1002/cne.22735 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7. Pérez-Gómez A, Stein B, Leinders-Zufall T, et al. : Signaling mechanisms and behavioral function of the mouse basal vomeronasal neuroepithelium. Front Neuroanat. 2014;8:135. 10.3389/fnana.2014.00135 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8. Brignall AC, Cloutier JF: Neural map formation and sensory coding in the vomeronasal system. Cell Mol Life Sci. 2015;72(24):4697–709. 10.1007/s00018-015-2029-5 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9. Trouillet AC, Keller M, Weiss J, et al. : Central role of G protein Gαi2 and Gαi2 + vomeronasal neurons in balancing territorial and infant-directed aggression of male mice. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2019;116(11):5135–43. 10.1073/pnas.1821492116 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]; F1000 Recommendation
  • 10. Chamero P, Katsoulidou V, Hendrix P, et al. : G protein G(alpha)o is essential for vomeronasal function and aggressive behavior in mice. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2011;108(31):12898–903. 10.1073/pnas.1107770108 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11. Griffiths PR, Brennan PA: Roles for learning in mammalian chemosensory responses. Horm Behav. 2015;68:91–102. 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2014.08.010 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12. Siegel A, Bandler RJ, Jr, Flynn JP: Thalamic sites and pathways related to elicited attack. Brain Behav Evol. 1972;6(1):542–55. 10.1159/000123731 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13. Todd WD, Machado NL: A time to fight: Circadian control of aggression and associated autonomic support. Auton Neurosci. 2019;217:35–40. 10.1016/j.autneu.2018.12.008 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]; F1000 Recommendation
  • 14. Haller J: The role of central and medial amygdala in normal and abnormal aggression: A review of classical approaches. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2018;85:34–43. 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2017.09.017 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]; F1000 Recommendation
  • 15. Falkner AL, Lin D: Recent advances in understanding the role of the hypothalamic circuit during aggression. Front Syst Neurosci. 2014;8:168. 10.3389/fnsys.2014.00168 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16. Caldwell HK, Lee HJ, Macbeth AH, et al. : Vasopressin: behavioral roles of an "original" neuropeptide. Prog Neurobiol. 2008;84(1):1–24. 10.1016/j.pneurobio.2007.10.007 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17. Stagkourakis S, Spigolon G, Williams P, et al. : A neural network for intermale aggression to establish social hierarchy. Nat Neurosci. 2018;21(6):834–42. 10.1038/s41593-018-0153-x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]; F1000 Recommendation
  • 18. Lin D, Boyle MP, Dollar P, et al. : Functional identification of an aggression locus in the mouse hypothalamus. Nature. 2011;470(7333):221–6. 10.1038/nature09736 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]; F1000 Recommendation
  • 19. Falkner AL, Dollar P, Perona P, et al. : Decoding ventromedial hypothalamic neural activity during male mouse aggression. J Neurosci. 2014;34(17):5971–84. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5109-13.2014 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20. Sakurai K, Zhao S, Takatoh J, et al. : Capturing and Manipulating Activated Neuronal Ensembles with CANE Delineates a Hypothalamic Social-Fear Circuit. Neuron. 2016;92(4):739–53. 10.1016/j.neuron.2016.10.015 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]; F1000 Recommendation
  • 21. Haller J, Abrahám I, Zelena D, et al. : Aggressive experience affects the sensitivity of neurons towards pharmacological treatment in the hypothalamic attack area. Behav Pharmacol. 1998;9(5–6):469–75. 10.1097/00008877-199809000-00010 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22. Remedios R, Kennedy A, Zelikowsky M, et al. : Social behaviour shapes hypothalamic neural ensemble representations of conspecific sex. Nature. 2017;550(7676):388–92. 10.1038/nature23885 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]; F1000 Recommendation
  • 23. Henley ED, Moisset B, Welch BL: Catecholamine uptake in cerebral cortex: adaptive change induced by fighting. Science. 1973;180(4090):1050–2. 10.1126/science.180.4090.1050 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24. Welch BL, Hendley ED, Turek I: Norepinephrine uptake into cerebral cortical synaptosomes after one fight or electroconvulsive shock. Science. 1974;183(4121):220–1. 10.1126/science.183.4121.220 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25. van Erp AM, Miczek KA: Aggressive behavior, increased accumbal dopamine, and decreased cortical serotonin in rats. J Neurosci. 2000;20(24):9320–5. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.20-24-09320.2000 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26. Ferrari PF, van Erp AM, Tornatzky W, et al. : Accumbal dopamine and serotonin in anticipation of the next aggressive episode in rats. Eur J Neurosci. 2003;17(2):371–8. 10.1046/j.1460-9568.2003.02447.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27. Couppis MH, Kennedy CH: The rewarding effect of aggression is reduced by nucleus accumbens dopamine receptor antagonism in mice. Psychopharmacology (Berl). 2008;197(3):449–56. 10.1007/s00213-007-1054-y [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]; F1000 Recommendation
  • 28. Aleyasin H, Flanigan ME, Golden SA, et al. : Cell-Type-Specific Role of ΔFosB in Nucleus Accumbens In Modulating Intermale Aggression. J Neurosci. 2018;38(26):5913–24. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0296-18.2018 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]; F1000 Recommendation
  • 29. Golden SA, Jin M, Heins C, et al. : Nucleus Accumbens Drd1-Expressing Neurons Control Aggression Self-Administration and Aggression Seeking in Mice. J Neurosci. 2019;39(13):2482–96. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2409-18.2019 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]; F1000 Recommendation
  • 30. Morrison TR, Melloni RH, Jr: The role of serotonin, vasopressin, and serotonin/vasopressin interactions in aggressive behavior. Curr Top Behav Neurosci. 2014;17:189–228. 10.1007/7854_2014_283 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31. Ferris CF, Melloni RH, Jr, Koppel G, et al. : Vasopressin/serotonin interactions in the anterior hypothalamus control aggressive behavior in golden hamsters. J Neurosci. 1997;17(11):4331–40. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.17-11-04331.1997 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32. Eibl-Eibesfeldt I: Ontogenetic and maturational studies of aggressive behavior. UCLA Forum Med Sci. 1967;7:57–94. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33. Oliveira RF, Lopes M, Carneiro LA, et al. : Watching fights raises fish hormone levels. Nature. 2001;409(6819):475. 10.1038/35054128 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34. Mench JA, Ottinger MA: Behavioral and hormonal correlates of social dominance in stable and disrupted groups of male domestic fowl. Horm Behav. 1991;25(1):112–22. 10.1016/0018-506X(91)90043-H [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35. Ohno S, Geller LN, Lai EV: Tfm mutation and masculinization versus feminization of the mouse central nervous system. Cell. 1974;3(3):235–42. 10.1016/0092-8674(74)90137-8 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36. Fraile IG, McEwen BS, Pfaff DW: Progesterone inhibition of aggressive behaviors in hamsters. Physiol Behav. 1987;39(2):225–9. 10.1016/0031-9384(87)90013-8 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37. Hess WR, Akert K: Experimental data on role of hypothalamus in mechanism of emotional behavior. AMA Arch Neurol Psychiatry. 1955;73(2):127–9. 10.1001/archneurpsyc.1955.02330080005003 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38. Wasman M, Flynn JP: Directed attack elicited from hypothalamus. Arch Neurol. 1962;6:220–7. 10.1001/archneur.1962.00450210048005 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39. Egger MD, Flynn JP: Amygdaloid suppression of hypothalamically elicited attack behavior. Science. 1962;136(3510):43–4. 10.1126/science.136.3510.43 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40. Roberts WW, Bergquist EH: Attack elicited by hypothalamic stimulation in cats raised in social isolation. J Comp Physiol Psychol. 1968;66(3):590–5. 10.1037/h0026524 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41. Bermond B, Mos J, Meelis W, et al. : Aggression induced by stimulation of the hypothalamus: effects of androgens. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. 1982;16(1):41–5. 10.1016/0091-3057(82)90010-7 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42. Kruk MR, van der Poel AM, Meelis W, et al. : Discriminant analysis of the localization of aggression-inducing electrode placements in the hypothalamus of male rats. Brain Res. 1983;260(1):61–79. 10.1016/0006-8993(83)90764-3 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43. Adams D, Flynn JP: Transfer of an escape response from tail shock to brain-stimulated attack behavior. J Exp Anal Behav. 1966;9(4):401–8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44. Shaikh MB, Barrett JA, Siegel A: The pathways mediating affective defense and quiet biting attack behavior from the midbrain central gray of the cat: an autoradiographic study. Brain Res. 1987;437(1):9–25. 10.1016/0006-8993(87)91522-8 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45. Chi CC, Flynn JP: Neural pathways associated with hypothalamically elicited attach behavior in cats. Science. 1971;171(3972):703–6. 10.1126/science.171.3972.703 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46. Siegel A, Edinger HM: Role of the limbic system in hypothalamically elicited attack behavior. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 1983;7(3):395–407. 10.1016/0149-7634(83)90045-3 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47. Leroy F, Park J, Asok A, et al. : A circuit from hippocampal CA2 to lateral septum disinhibits social aggression. Nature. 2018;564(7735):213–8. 10.1038/s41586-018-0772-0 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]; F1000 Recommendation
  • 48. Adams DB: Cells related to fighting behavior recorded from midbrain central gray neuropil of cat. Science. 1968;159(3817):894–6. 10.1126/science.159.3817.894 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49. Jürgens U, Richter K: Glutamate-induced vocalization in the squirrel monkey. Brain Res. 1986;373(1–2):349–58. 10.1016/0006-8993(86)90349-5 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50. Biro L, Toth M, Sipos E, et al. : Structural and functional alterations in the prefrontal cortex after post-weaning social isolation: relationship with species-typical and deviant aggression. Brain Struct Funct. 2017;222(4):1861–75. 10.1007/s00429-016-1312-z [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]; F1000 Recommendation
  • 51. Pan Y, Xu L, Young KA, et al. : Agonistic encounters and brain activation in dominant and subordinate male greater long-tailed hamsters. Horm Behav. 2010;58(3):478–84. 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2010.05.001 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52. Takahashi A, Miczek KA: Neurogenetics of aggressive behavior: studies in rodents. Curr Top Behav Neurosci. 2014;17:3–44. 10.1007/7854_2013_263 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53. Biro L, Sipos E, Bruzsik B, et al. : Task Division within the Prefrontal Cortex: Distinct Neuron Populations Selectively Control Different Aspects of Aggressive Behavior via the Hypothalamus. J Neurosci. 2018;38(17):4065–75. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3234-17.2018 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]; F1000 Recommendation
  • 54. Ginsburg B, Allee WC: Some Effects of Conditioning on Social Dominance and Subordination in Inbred Strains of Mice. Physiol Zool. 1942;15(4):485–506. 10.1086/physzool.15.4.30151662 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 55. Lorenz K: On aggression. (Methuen),1966. Reference Source [Google Scholar]
  • 56. Gómez JM, Verdú M, González-Megías A, et al. : The phylogenetic roots of human lethal violence. Nature. 2016;538(7624):233–7. 10.1038/nature19758 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]; F1000 Recommendation
  • 57. Eibl-Eibesfeldt L: Evolution of Destructive Aggression. Aggr Behav. 1977;3(2):127–44. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 58. Wrangham RW, Peterson D: Demonic males : apes and the origins of human violence. (Houghton Mifflin),1996. Reference Source [Google Scholar]
  • 59. de Boer SF: Animal models of excessive aggression: implications for human aggression and violence. Curr Opin Psychol. 2018;19:81–7. 10.1016/j.copsyc.2017.04.006 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]; F1000 Recommendation
  • 60. Haller J, Tóth M, Halasz J, et al. : Patterns of violent aggression-induced brain c-fos expression in male mice selected for aggressiveness. Physiol Behav. 2006;88(1–2):173–82. 10.1016/j.physbeh.2006.03.030 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 61. Davidson RJ, Putnam KM, Larson CL: Dysfunction in the neural circuitry of emotion regulation--a possible prelude to violence. Science. 2000;289(5479):591–4. 10.1126/science.289.5479.591 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 62. Choy O, Raine A, Hamilton RH: Stimulation of the Prefrontal Cortex Reduces Intentions to Commit Aggression: A Randomized, Double-Blind, Placebo-Controlled, Stratified, Parallel-Group Trial. J Neurosci. 2018;38(29):6505–12. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3317-17.2018 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]; F1000 Recommendation
  • 63. Lee TM, Chan SC, Raine A: Strong limbic and weak frontal activation to aggressive stimuli in spouse abusers. Mol Psychiatry. 2008;13(7):655–6. 10.1038/mp.2008.46 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 64. Takahashi A, Nagayasu K, Nishitani N, et al. : Control of intermale aggression by medial prefrontal cortex activation in the mouse. PLoS One. 2014;9(4):e94657. 10.1371/journal.pone.0094657 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 65. Cases O, Seif I, Grimsby J, et al. : Aggressive behavior and altered amounts of brain serotonin and norepinephrine in mice lacking MAOA. Science. 1995;268(5218):1763–6. 10.1126/science.7792602 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 66. Allsop SA, Wichmann R, Mills F, et al. : Corticoamygdala Transfer of Socially Derived Information Gates Observational Learning. Cell. 2018;173(6):1329–1342.e18. 10.1016/j.cell.2018.04.004 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]; F1000 Recommendation
  • 67. Hohmann CF, Hodges A, Beard N, et al. : Effects of brief stress exposure during early postnatal development in balb/CByJ mice: I. Behavioral characterization. Dev Psychobiol. 2013;55(3):283–93. 10.1002/dev.21027 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 68. Veenema AH, Bredewold R, Neumann ID: Opposite effects of maternal separation on intermale and maternal aggression in C57BL/6 mice: link to hypothalamic vasopressin and oxytocin immunoreactivity. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2007;32(5):437–50. 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2007.02.008 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 69. Newman EL, Terunuma M, Wang TL, et al. : A Role for Prefrontal Cortical NMDA Receptors in Murine Alcohol-Heightened Aggression. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2018;43(6):1224–34. 10.1038/npp.2017.253 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 70. Eibl-Eibesfeldt IU: Ethology, the biology of behavior. 2d edn, (Holt, Rinehart and Winston),1975. Reference Source [Google Scholar]
  • 71. Golden SA, Jin M, Shaham Y: Animal Models of (or for) Aggression Reward, Addiction, and Relapse: Behavior and Circuits. J Neurosci. 2019;39(21):3996–4008. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0151-19.2019 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]; F1000 Recommendation
  • 72. Berkowitz L: Some determinants of impulsive aggression: role of mediated associations with reinforcements for aggression. Psychol Rev. 1974;81(2):165–76. 10.1037/h0036094 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 73. Vernon WM: Animal aggression: review of research. Genet Psychol Monogr. 1969;80(1):3–28. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 74. Nell V: Cruelty's rewards: the gratifications of perpetrators and spectators. Behav Brain Sci. 2006;29(3):211–24; discussion 224–57. 10.1017/S0140525X06009058 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 75. Thompson T: Effect of chlorpromazine on "aggressive" responding in the rat. J Comp Physiol Psychol. 1961;54:398–400. 10.1037/h0048753 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 76. Thompson T: Operant and classically-conditioned aggressive behavior in Siamese fighting fish. Am Zool. 1966;6(4):629–41. 10.1093/icb/6.4.629 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 77. Thompson TI: Visual Reinforcement in Fighting Cocks. J Exp Anal Behav. 1964;7:45–9. 10.1901/jeab.1964.7-45 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 78. Berridge KC, Kringelbach ML: Pleasure systems in the brain. Neuron. 2015;86(3):646–64. 10.1016/j.neuron.2015.02.018 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 79. Skjoldager P, Winger G, Woods JH: Analysis of fixed-ratio behavior maintained by drug reinforcers. J Exp Anal Behav. 1991;56(2):331–43. 10.1901/jeab.1991.56-331 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 80. Markou A, Weiss F, Gold LH, et al. : Animal models of drug craving. Psychopharmacology (Berl). 1993;112(2–3):163–82. 10.1007/BF02244907 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 81. Fish EW, Debold JF, Miczek KA: Escalated aggression as a reward: corticosterone and GABA(A) receptor positive modulators in mice. Psychopharmacology (Berl). 2005;182(1):116–27. 10.1007/s00213-005-0064-x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 82. Fish EW, De Bold JF, Miczek KA: Aggressive behavior as a reinforcer in mice: activation by allopregnanolone. Psychopharmacology (Berl). 2002;163(3–4):459–66. 10.1007/s00213-002-1211-2 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 83. Covington HE, 3rd, Newman EL, Tran S, et al. : The Urge to Fight: Persistent Escalation by Alcohol and Role of NMDA Receptors in Mice. Front Behav Neurosci. 2018;12:206. 10.3389/fnbeh.2018.00206 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 84. Dews PB: Studies on responding under fixed-interval schedules of reinforcement: the effects on the pattern of responding of changes in requirements at reinforcement. J Exp Anal Behav. 1969;12(2):191–9. 10.1901/jeab.1969.12-191 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 85. Egli M, Thompson T: Effects of methadone on alternative fixed-ratio fixed-interval performance: Latent influences on schedule-controlled responding. J Exp Anal Behav. 1989;52(2):141–53. 10.1901/jeab.1989.52-141 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 86. Cherek DR, Thompson T, Heistad GT: Responding maintained by the opportunity to attack during an interval food reinforcement schedule. J Exp Anal Behav. 1973;19(1):113–23. 10.1901/jeab.1973.19-113 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 87. Dollard J, Miller NE, Doob LW, et al. : Frustration and aggression. Pub. for the Institute of human relations by Yale university press; H. Milford, Oxford university press,1939. 10.1037/10022-000 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 88. Kelly JF, Hake DF: An extinction-induced increase in an aggressive response with humans. J Exp Anal Behav. 1970;14(2):153–64. 10.1901/jeab.1970.14-153 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 89. De Almeida R: Aggression escalated by social instigation or by discontinuation of reinforcement (“frustration”) in mice: inhibition by anpirtoline: a 5-HT1B receptor agonist. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2002;27(2):171–81. 10.1016/S0893-133X(02)00291-9 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 90. Amsel A, Roussel J: Motivational properties of frustration. I. Effect on a running response of the addition of frustration to the motivational complex. J Exp Psychol. 1952;43(5):363–6. 10.1037/h0059393 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 91. Gowin JL, Green CE, Alcorn JL, et al. : The role of cortisol and psychopathy in the cycle of violence. Psychopharmacology (Berl). 2013;227(4):661–72. 10.1007/s00213-013-2992-1 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 92. Alia-Klein N, Goldstein RZ, Tomasi D, et al. : Neural mechanisms of anger regulation as a function of genetic risk for violence. Emotion. 2009;9(3):385–96. 10.1037/a0015904 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 93. Pernanen K: Causal attributions in the explanation of alcohol-related accidents. Addiction. 1993;88(7):897–906. 10.1111/j.1360-0443.1993.tb02107.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 94. Beck A, Heinz AJ, Heinz A: Translational clinical neuroscience perspectives on the cognitive and neurobiological mechanisms underlying alcohol-related aggression. Curr Top Behav Neurosci. 2014;17:443–74. 10.1007/7854_2013_258 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 95. Bye EK, Rossow I: The impact of drinking pattern on alcohol-related violence among adolescents: An international comparative analysis. Drug Alcohol Rev. 2010;29(2):131–7. 10.1111/j.1465-3362.2009.00117.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 96. Jaffe JH, Babor TF, Fishbein DH: Alcoholics, aggression and antisocial personality. J Stud Alcohol. 1988;49(3):211–8. 10.15288/jsa.1988.49.211 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 97. Mayfield D: Alcoholism, alcohol, intoxication and assaultive behavior. Dis Nerv Syst. 1976;37(5):288–91. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 98. Cherpitel CJ, Witbrodt J, Ye Y, et al. : A multi-level analysis of emergency department data on drinking patterns, alcohol policy and cause of injury in 28 countries. Drug Alcohol Depend. 2018;192:172–178. 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2018.07.033 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]; F1000 Recommendation
  • 99. Tessler RA, Arbabi S, Bulger EM, et al. : Trends in Firearm Injury and Motor Vehicle Crash Case Fatality by Age Group, 2003-2013. JAMA Surg. 2019;154(4):305–310. 10.1001/jamasurg.2018.4685 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 100. Conner RL, Vernikos-Danellis J, Levine S: Stress, Fighting and Neuroendocrine Function. Nature. 1971;234(5331):564–6. 10.1038/234564a0 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 101. Arbel R, Rodriguez AJ, Margolin G: Cortisol Reactions During Family Conflict Discussions: Influences of Wives' and Husbands' Exposure to Family-of-Origin Aggression. Psychol Violence. 2016;6(4):519–28. 10.1037/a0039715 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]; F1000 Recommendation
  • 102. Miczek KA, Takahashi A, Gobrogge KL, et al. : Escalated Aggression in Animal Models: Shedding New Light on Mesocorticolimbic Circuits. Curr Opin Behav Sci. 2015;3:90–5. 10.1016/j.cobeha.2015.02.007 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 103. Thompson DF, Ramos CL, Willett JK: Psychopathy: clinical features, developmental basis and therapeutic challenges. J Clin Pharm Ther. 2014;39(5):485–95. 10.1111/jcpt.12182 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 104. Scarpa A, Raine A: Psychophysiology of anger and violent behavior. Psychiatr Clin North Am. 1997;20(2):375–94. 10.1016/S0193-953X(05)70318-X [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 105. Haller J, van de Schraaf J, Kruk MR: Deviant forms of aggression in glucocorticoid hyporeactive rats: a model for 'pathological' aggression? J Neuroendocrinol. 2001;13(1):102–7. 10.1111/j.1365-2826.2001.00600.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 106. Lodwick JL, Borries C, Pusey AE, et al. : From nest to nest--influence of ecology and reproduction on the active period of adult Gombe chimpanzees. Am J Primatol. 2004;64(3):249–60. 10.1002/ajp.20076 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 107. Miczek KA, Maxson SC, Fish EW, et al. : Aggressive behavioral phenotypes in mice. Behav Brain Res. 2001;125(1–2):167–81. 10.1016/S0166-4328(01)00298-4 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 108. Reinhardt V, Reinhardt C, Reinhardt A: Evaluating Sex Differences in Aggressiveness in Cattle, Bison and Rhesus Monkeys. Behaviour. 1987;102:58–66. 10.1163/156853986X00045 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 109. Stockley P, Campbell A: Female competition and aggression: interdisciplinary perspectives. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 2013;368(1631):20130073. 10.1098/rstb.2013.0073 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 110. Denson TF, O'Dean SM, Blake KR, et al. : Aggression in Women: Behavior, Brain and Hormones. Front Behav Neurosci. 2018;12:81. 10.3389/fnbeh.2018.00081 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 111. Haug M, Mandel P: Strain differences in aggressive behaviour of female mice against lactating and non-lactating individuals. Neurosci Lett. 1978;7(2–3):235–8. 10.1016/0304-3940(78)90174-X [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 112. Albert DJ, Dyson EM, Petrovic DM, et al. : Activation of aggression in female rats by normal males and by castrated males with testosterone implants. Physiol Behav. 1988;44(1):9–13. 10.1016/0031-9384(88)90339-3 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 113. Newman EL, Covington HE, Suh J, et al. : Fighting females: Neural and behavioral consequences of social defeat stress in female mice. Biol Psychiatry. 2019. 10.1016/j.biopsych.2019.05.005 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 114. Noirot E, Goyens J, Buhot MC: Aggressive behavior of pregnant mice toward males. Horm Behav. 1975;6(1):9–17. 10.1016/0018-506X(75)90018-5 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 115. Ogawa S, Makino J: Aggressive behavior in inbred strains of mice during pregnancy. Behav Neural Biol. 1984;40(2):195–204. 10.1016/S0163-1047(84)90303-0 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 116. Svare B, Gandelman R: Postpartum aggression in mice: inhibitory effect of estrogen. Physiol Behav. 1975;14(1):31–5. 10.1016/0031-9384(75)90137-7 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 117. Svare B, Gandelman R: Postpartum aggression in mice: the influence of suckling stimulation. Horm Behav. 1976;7(4):407–16. 10.1016/0018-506X(76)90012-X [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 118. Hashikawa K, Hashikawa Y, Tremblay R, et al. : Esr1 + cells in the ventromedial hypothalamus control female aggression. Nat Neurosci. 2017;20(11):1580–90. 10.1038/nn.4644 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]; F1000 Recommendation
  • 119. Unger EK, Burke KJ, Jr, Yang CF, et al. : Medial amygdalar aromatase neurons regulate aggression in both sexes. Cell Rep. 2015;10(4):453–62. 10.1016/j.celrep.2014.12.040 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 120. Darwin C: The descent of man, and selection in relation to sex. D. Appleton and company,1871. 10.5962/bhl.title.24784 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 121. Miczek KA, de Boer SF, Haller J: Excessive aggression as model of violence: a critical evaluation of current preclinical methods. Psychopharmacology (Berl). 2013;226(3):445–58. 10.1007/s00213-013-3008-x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 122. Takahashi LK, Blanchard RJ: Attack and defense in laboratory and wild Norway and black rats. Behav Processes. 1982;7(1):49–62. 10.1016/0376-6357(82)90052-3 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 123. Cherek DR, Pickens R: Schedule-induced aggression as a function of fixed-ratio value. J Exp Anal Behav. 1970;14(3):309–11. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 124. Golden SA, Heins C, Venniro M, et al. : Compulsive Addiction-like Aggressive Behavior in Mice. Biol Psychiatry. 2017;82(4):239–48. 10.1016/j.biopsych.2017.03.004 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]; F1000 Recommendation
  • 125. Volkow ND, Wise RA, Baler R: The dopamine motive system: implications for drug and food addiction. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2017;18(12):741–52. 10.1038/nrn.2017.130 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 126. Kollack-Walker S, Watson SJ, Akil H: Social stress in hamsters: defeat activates specific neurocircuits within the brain. J Neurosci. 1997;17(22):8842–55. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.17-22-08842.1997 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 127. Nikulina EM, Marchand JE, Kream RM, et al. : Behavioral sensitization to cocaine after a brief social stress is accompanied by changes in fos expression in the murine brainstem. Brain Res. 1998;810(1–2):200–10. 10.1016/s0006-8993(98)00925-1 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 128. Hashikawa K, Hashikawa Y, Falkner A, et al. : The neural circuits of mating and fighting in male mice. Curr Opin Neurobiol. 2016;38:27–37. 10.1016/j.conb.2016.01.006 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 129. Golden SA, Heshmati M, Flanigan M, et al. : Basal forebrain projections to the lateral habenula modulate aggression reward. Nature. 2016;534(7609):688–92. 10.1038/nature18601 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]; F1000 Recommendation

Articles from F1000Research are provided here courtesy of F1000 Research Ltd

RESOURCES